Mathematics teachers: Dealing with difference.
Cheeseman, Jill ; Klooger, Michele
Mathematics teachers: Dealing with difference.
A major problem for teachers is that students are different, they
learn mathematics differently, and at different rates. Regardless of
this, in schools we organise students into large groups or classes of 20
to 30 students all about the same age. Then there is the Australian
Curriculum: Mathematics that states the student learning outcomes that
are expected for students at each Year level of school - which means at
each year of their lives. Teachers are expected to teach the curriculum
prescribed for students of the specific age group in their class and
still keep in consideration student differences. How does a teacher of
primary mathematics deal with the range of student thinking and
knowledge in his or her class every day? With difficulty, we hear you
say. That is true. Catering for student difference is a substantial
challenge for teachers.
There is a view by some policy-makers, school leaders and teachers,
that management of students in ability groups in classrooms helps to
deal with the range of mathematical knowledge and improves learning
outcomes. Unfortunately, the research evidence over a long period of
time shows this is not the case and that ability grouping in mathematics
leads to a decline in learning standards overall (Boaler, 2014). The
weight of evidence from countries across the world indicates that
ability grouping harms the achievement of students in low and middle
groups and does not affect the achievement of high attaining students
(Boaler, 2014). Globally, the countries that have the most successful
results in terms of international achievement are those who do not use
ability grouping practices (Anthony & Hunter, 2017; Boaler, 2014).
Despite evidence to the contrary it seems that ability grouping in
mathematics is widespread. In some schools, teachers test the students
in their classes and divide them into teaching groups according to their
results on the tests. In other schools the students are tested and
divided into Year level achievement-based classes.
Theoretically, these processes put students into likeability
groups. In fact, the groups are still mixed-ability groups, although
each group may have a narrower range of student mathematical knowledge
about the topic tested.
The actual term ability is in common use in education and usually
goes unquestioned (Marks, 2014). There are two issues about the use of
ability groups that seem to be problematic: the name, and the assumption
that grouped students are then alike. The consequences of referring to
the clusters of students who achieve similar test results as of similar
ability is unfortunate to say the least. Synonyms for ability are
capability, skill, talent, capacity, gift, and knack. Of these words,
perhaps skill is the only one that is being measured by a classroom
mathematics test.
In addition, "ability" brings with it a fixed mindset.
The metaphor of mindsets was described by Dweck (2000) who categorised
students' orientation to learning. She described mindset in terms
of whether students hold either mastery goals or performance goals. A
fixed mindset, in which students think they are as smart as they are
ever going to get, is connected to performance goals. Students with a
fixed mindset believe they are as intelligent as they will ever get and
they; seek success but mainly on tasks with which they are familiar;
avoid or give up quickly on challenging tasks; and derive their
perception of ability from their capacity to attract recognition.
In contrast, students with mastery goals pursue understanding of
the content, and evaluate their success by whether they feel they can
use and transfer their knowledge. Students with a growth mindset believe
they can get smarter by trying hard and this is connected to mastery
goals. Such students: tend to have a resilient response to failure;
remain focused on mastering skills and knowledge even when challenged;
do not see failure as an indictment on themselves; and believe that
effort leads to success (Dweck, 2000).
It seems imperative that we encourage a growth mindset in our
students.
The construction of ability groups by teachers may promote
performance goals and a fixed mindset. However, we think that most
teachers believe that ability grouping is in the best interests of their
students. Their belief seems to be based on their thought that
constructing ability groups enables them to target students'
learning needs with different mathematical tasks.
The assumption that, in each constructed group, the students are
alike in their mathematical knowledge and thinking based on a single
short test is questionable. While the range of knowledge of the topic
tested may be truncated in each group, the students are still different.
The danger is that the teacher who has students with the same test
scores treats the students as the same in terms of their thinking.
Possibly the groups are labelled by the teacher's characterisation
of them as well.
We have heard teachers privately refer to a group as the
"lower group" and the "top group". We know that
students live up to--or down to--teachers' expectations of them.
There is widespread acceptance that teachers' expectations are
important for student learning (Rubie-Davies, 2009). By dividing
students into groups and thinking of them according to their
"ability" teachers are, often unwittingly, giving messages
about their expectations to their students through their interactions
(Cooper & Brophy, 1983; Marks, 2014; Nunes et al., 2009).
A serious consequence of grouping the underachieving students is
that they are then given different mathematics--often simpler exercises
requiring fewer steps in reasoning and devoid of problem solving
contexts. Teachers can often simplify the mathematics, encourage
rote-learnt processes, where little understanding is required (Clarke
& Clarke, 2008; Marks, 2014). These practises, we would argue,
systematically impoverish students mathematically. In addition to being
offered a watered-down curriculum these students do not have the
opportunity to hear the mathematical reasoning of other students whose
mathematical approaches may offer them new ways of thinking through
problems. Sullivan and his colleagues (2014) argued that
"classrooms in which students act as a community are more likely to
support the learning of all students" (p. 127). This view
emphasises the social process of learning mathematics where students
engage in discussion around the same content, justify and share their
strategies, establish connections and extend the contributions of
others.
The danger of dividing the students into "like groups" is
that they are then treated as a group instead of as individuals. For
teachers, working with four groups of seven students seems a more
manageable task than teaching 28 individuals. You can see the appeal of
grouping as a management technique if you are convinced that it is
effective. However, research findings show "that most students are
disadvantaged by classes grouped according to ability" (Clarke
& Clarke, 2008, p. 31).
It is a practice that results in a hierarchy of competence based on
a fixed mindset and gives inequitable access to mathematical learning
(Boaler, 2014; Marks, 2014). We would doubt that most primary classroom
teachers would be aware that their well-intentioned grouping of students
disadvantages the very students about whom they are most concerned.
Without minimising the problem of the range of mathematical
thinking in classrooms, we would like to consider an approach that is
not a management solution but a pedagogical one. Our first suggestion is
to use mixed-ability approaches to teaching as they have consistently
demonstrated more equitable outcomes than ability grouping (Boaler 2008,
2005, 2014; Cohen & Lotan 1997; Linchevski & Kutscher 1998).
We also suggest that broadening the types of mathematical tasks
(Sullivan, Clarke & Clarke, 2013) offered to students can help
address the range of student thinking. By incorporating challenging
problems, investigations, mathematical games and open-ended tasks to any
mathematics program teachers can address some of the differences in
student thinking in their classrooms and potentially extend all students
(Cheeseman & Montgomery, 2000; Siegler, 2010; Sullivan et al.,
2014). Recent literature suggests that "changes towards more
flexible heterogeneous grouping practices aligned with collaborative
problem-solving learning environments will better support equitable and
productive learning opportunities." (Anthony & Hunter, 2017, p.
73).
Problem solving tasks have features that enable students to engage
in mathematics in their own ways. Posing open problems and preparing
prompts can vary the task so that children who need support on that
particular problem can complete the task and those who solve the problem
quickly can be further challenged. This pedagogical technique allows
tasks to be differentiated in the moment in response to what teachers
notice as the students' needs for support and extension.
"Enabling" and "extending" prompts (Mousley,
Sullivan & Zevenbergen, 2004) vary the original task and may include
strategies such as: adjusting the size of the numbers, reducing the
number of steps, or representation of the problem. An example is shown
in Table 1. Most importantly though, these prompts allow all students to
engage with the same problem and engage in the discussion and review of
the task.
This approach is in contrast to the approach in ability groups
where students in different groups, pursue different learning goals and
possibly even focused on different mathematical content where a
conclusion of the lesson that elicits the central mathematical concepts
is not feasible.
We believe that all students can learn mathematics and should have
the opportunity to do so. The research is clear, the single most
important factor in a student's learning is his or her teacher
(Askew & Brown, 2003). Therefore, it is the responsibility of the
teacher to provide challenging and engaging learning opportunities for
students while they are at school. Merely placing students in groups or
not grouping students, does not seem to be enough to promote substantial
gains in achievement.
It is how students collaborate in groups, what the teacher values,
says, does and how the students respond that is most important (Anthony
& Hunter, 2017; McDonough, 2003). There appears to be no substitute
for effective teaching and quality teachers.
Conclusion
As Clarke and Clarke (2008) acknowledged "catering for the
wide range of levels of confidence and competence in mathematical
understanding is possibly the greatest challenge which teachers
face" (p. 32). However, dealing with this challenge by ability
grouping students in mathematics classrooms is not the answer. It is
disappointing to note that despite Clarke and Clarke's compelling
case arguing, for nine main reasons of effectiveness and social justice,
that the time for ability grouping was up, we are still arguing against
its use a decade later. The pedagogical strategies that were suggested
to teachers at the time are echoed in those recommended here for use in
primary schools. Perhaps the first and most telling step is to convince
teachers that ability grouping of students in mathematics is likely to
do more harm than good. It is a practice focused on performance goals
and, based on test results, and suggests to students that they are as
smart as they are going to get. The next step in to integrate productive
and inclusive pedagogies into the everyday teaching and learning of
mathematics.
References
Anthony, G., & Hunter, R. (2017). Grouping practices in New
Zealand mathematics classrooms: Where are we at and where should we be?
New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 52(1), 73-92.
Askew, M., & Brown, M. (2003). How do we teach children to be
numerate? Southwell, UK: British Educational Research Association.
Boaler, J. (2005). The 'psychological prisons' from which
they never escaped: The role of ability grouping in reproducing social
class inequalities. Forum, 47(2), 135.
Boaler, J. (2008). What' math got to do with it? Helping
children learn to love their least favourite subject-and why its
important for America. Penguin, New York.
Boaler, J. (2014). Ability grouping in mathematics classrooms. In
S. Lerman (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of mathematics education (pp. 1-5).
Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.
Boaler, J., Wiliam, D., & Brown, M. (2000). Students'
experiences of ability grouping disaffection, polarisation and the
construction of failure. British Educational Research Journal, 26(5),
631-48.
Cheeseman, J., & Montgomery, P. (2000). Extending all children
in the primary mathematics classroom. Prime Number, 15(2), 5-7.
Clarke, D., & Clarke, B. A. (2008). Is time up for ability
grouping? EQ_Australia, 6(5), 31-33.
Cohen, E., & Lotan, R. (1997). Working for equity in
heterogeneous classrooms: Sociological theory in practice. New York:
Teachers College Press
Cooper, H., & Brophy, T. (1983). Pygmalion grows up: Studies in
the expectation communication process. New York: Longman.
Dweck, C. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation,
personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Linchevski, L., & Kutscher, B. (1998). Tell me with whom
you're learning and I'll tell you how much you've
learned: Mixed ability versus same-ability grouping in mathematics.
Journal of Research in Mathematics Education 29, 533-554.
Marks, R. (2014). Educational triage and ability-grouping in
primary mathematics: A case-study of the impacts on low-attaining
pupils. Research in Mathematics Education, 16(1), 38-53.
McDonough, A. (2003). Effective teachers of numeracy in the early
years and beyond. Making mathematicians. Brunswick, Victoria:
Mathematical Association of Victoria.
Mousley, J., Sullivan, P., & Zevenbergen, R. (2004).
Alternative learning trajectories. In P. I. R. Faraher & M. McLean
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Mathematics Education Research
Group of Australasia Conference (Vol. 2). Townsville: MERGA.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Sylva, K., & Barros, R. (2009).
Development of maths capabilities and confidence in primary school (vol
Report RR118). DCSF.
Rubie-Davies, C. (2009). Teacher expectations and labeling. In L.
J. Saha & A. G. Dworkin (Eds.), International handbook of research
on teachers and teaching (Vol. 21) pp. 695-707. Boston, MA: Springer.
Sahlberg, P. (2011). Finnish lessons: What can the world learn from
educational change in Finland? Series on school reform. Teachers College
Press, New York.
Siegler, R S. (2010). Playing numerical board games improves number
sense in children from low-income backgrounds (Vol. 7): The British
Psychological Society.
Sullivan, P., Clarke, D., & Clarke, B. (2013). Teaching with
tasks for effective mathematics learning. New York: Springer.
Sullivan, P., Clarke, D., Cheeseman, J., Mornane, A., Roche, A.,
Sawatzki, C., & Walker, N. (2014). Students' willingness to
engage with mathematical challenges: Implications for classroom
pedagogies.
In J. Anderson, M. Cavanagh, & A. Prescott (Eds.), Curriculum
in focus: Research guided practice (Proceedings of the 37th annual
conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia)
(pp. 597-604). Sydney: MERGA.
Jill Cheeseman
Monash University
<
[email protected]>
Michele Klooger
Monash University
<
[email protected]>
Table 1: Learning task with an enabling and extending prompt.
Open task for Enabling prompt Extending prompt
investigation
When Josie was When Josie was When Josie was
counting out loud counting out loud counting out loud
from 0 to 50 by from 0 to 25 by a from 0 to 100 by
a number other number other than a number other than
than 1, one of 1, one of the 1, one of the
the numbers she numbers she said numbers she said was
said was '48'. was '24'. What '96'. What number
What number number might she might she have
might she have have been counting been counting by?
been counting by?
by?
COPYRIGHT 2018 The Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.