Benevolent Theodicies Protect Against PTSD Following a Natural Disaster.
McElroy-Heltzel, Stacey E. ; Davis, Edward B. ; Davis, Don E. 等
Benevolent Theodicies Protect Against PTSD Following a Natural Disaster.
In 2016 and 2017 alone, there were 31 U.S. natural disasters (e.g.,
severe storms, floods, droughts, and wildfires) that each caused $1
billion or more in damages. Collectively, these 31 disasters resulted in
500 deaths and $354.4 billion in estimated damages, with an average
event cost of $11.4 billion. By comparison, between 1980 and 2015, there
were an average of 7.2 high-cost natural disasters in the U.S. per year,
with an average event cost of $6.3 billion and an average annual cost of
$45.5 billion (National Centers for Environmental Informatoin [NCEI],
2018). Given the increased prevalence and impact of natural disasters,
it is crucial to understand how disasters affect survivors' mental
health and what factors might protect against common negative mental
health consequences of disasters.
One common type of mental health problem that can be caused by
natural disasters is Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). When an
experience is stressful enough to violate one's core beliefs about
the world, it can initiate a process of meaning-making--attempting to
reconcile the trauma with one's existing belief system (Park,
Mills, & Edmondson, 2012; Park et al., 2016). In such times, many
individuals turn to their religious/spiritual (R/S) beliefs to find
answers and make sense of their experiences (Aldwin, 2007; Bussing,
Ostermann, & Matthiessen, 2005). However, little is known about how
individuals may engage their R/S belief systems to make sense of trauma
and how doing so might impact their mental health (Hale-Smith, Park,
& Edmondson, 2012). Scholars have suggested that when people are
able to mitigate belief violations by incorporating the experience into
their current belief systems, it can reduce the likelihood they will
develop PTSD symptoms (George, Park, & Chaudoir, 2016).
Empirical work on this topic is scarce, in part due to lack of
appropriate measures. Just recently, scholars have developed measures of
belief violations (Park et al., 2016) and R/S views of suffering (i.e.,
theodicies; Hale-Smith et al., 2012). Initial studies utilizing these
measures have demonstrated a link between belief violations and PTSD
symptoms and between benevolent theodicies and positive mental health
(e.g., George et al., 2016; Wilt, Exline, Grubbs, Park, & Pargament,
2016). However, most of these studies are limited in that they rely on
undergraduate samples rather than recruiting trauma-exposed community
samples. Accordingly, the purpose of the present study is to examine a
community sample of disaster survivors and test whether the relationship
between belief violations and PTSD symptoms is buffered by benevolent
theodicies.
The Role of Belief Violations in the Development of PTSD
Traumatic experiences can violate individuals' core beliefs
about the world (Horowitz 1986; Park et al., 2016). Such belief
violations have been linked with increased psychological distress,
including symptoms of PTSD (Chukwuorji, Ifeagwazi, & Eze, 2017;
George et al., 2016; Park et al., 2012, 2016). Stress response theory
provides a framework for understanding this link: when individuals
experience an event that is incompatible with their existing belief
structure, their attempts to reconcile this discrepancy can lead to
symptoms of avoidance and intrusive memories, which are hallmarks of
PTSD (Horowitz, 1986). Similarly, shattered assumptions theory
(Janoff-Bulman, 1992) posits that individuals have an unarticulated
assumption of a just and predictable world, and this assumption provides
them with a sense of meaning and invulnerability. But traumatic events
violate this assumption and therefore can lead to the development of
PTSD symptoms.
Although these theories are generally accepted within the
scientific literature, they have largely gone untested due to a lack of
appropriate measures (Park et al., 2012). With recent advancements in
measurement, however, scholars have begun to examine these theories
empirically. For example, in a sample of 130 undergraduates who had
experienced a traumatic event, Park et al. (2012) found evidence that
belief violations were related to negative beliefs about oneself and the
world, which were in turn related to PTSD symptoms.
Likewise, at least two studies have examined whether belief
violations mediate the relationship between event centrality (i.e., how
central a stressful event is to one's identity and life story) and
PTSD symptoms. George et al. (2016) found evidence of this mediated
relationship using a sample of 367 undergraduates who had experienced a
traumatic event, and Chukwuorji et al. (2017) replicated these results
in a sample of 279 older, internally displaced adults in Nigeria. Taken
together, these studies offer preliminary support that traumatic events
can cause belief violations, which in turn can lead to the development
of PTSD symptoms. Two important next steps include continuing to
replicate these findings in community samples and examining protective
factors that might reduce the impact of belief violations on mental
health.
Benevolent Theodicies as a Protective Factor
When individuals experience belief violations, they may engage in
meaning-making processes that attempt to reconcile or integrate the
violating experience with their existing worldview in order to reduce
distress (Park et al., 2016). One way individuals may do this is through
engaging their theodicies (i.e., R/S beliefs about suffering; Brown,
1999; Hale-Smith et al., 2012). Although there are a number of
theodicies, in this study we are primarily focused on benevolent
theodicies--that is, "beliefs that attribute a benevolent role to
God in suffering" (Wilt, Exline, Lindberg, Park, & Pargament,
2017, p. 137). Beliefs about God's role in suffering may vary
considerably, but we were interested in whether three benevolent
theodicies might serve as protective factors for natural disaster
survivors: (a) the belief that God has providential control over
suffering and uses it for a higher purpose (i.e., providence theodicy),
(b) the belief that God is present in the midst of suffering and suffers
compassionately alongside people (i.e., suffering-God theodicy), and (c)
the belief that God uses suffering to build virtues into people's
character (i.e., soul-building theodicy; Hale-Smith et al., 2012; Wilt
et al., 2016, 2017).
These benevolent theodicies might help individuals make sense of
their suffering. For example, benevolent theodicies can help
monotheistic religious believers reconcile traumatic life events with
their beliefs in a good, loving, and all-powerful God (Wilt et al.,
2016, 2017). Indeed, there is initial empirical support for this
possibility. In two independent samples of U.S. undergraduates (N =
3,083) and adults (N = 1,047) who were facing nonspecific stressors,
Wilt et al. (2016) found evidence that benevolent theodicies were linked
to higher psychological well-being (rs = .21 to .29). Similarly, in an
undergraduate sample (N = 454) who reported experiencing a specific
individual R/S struggle in the past few months, Exline, Hall, Pargament,
and Harriott (2017) found that benevolent theodicies predicted
students' engagement in positive religious coping, which in turn
predicted their perceived posttraumatic growth. Moreover, in a
nationally representative sample of chaplains in the Veterans Healthcare
Administration (N = 298), benevolent theodicies were linked with
chaplains' professional quality of life (i.e., their sense of
purpose and enjoyment in their work), leading currier, Drescher,
Nieuwsma, and McCormick (2017) to conclude that benevolent theodicies
"might serve as a pathway to resilience for individuals in
spiritual communities and traditions in [the] USA" (p. 286).
Taken together, these studies offer preliminary evidence that
benevolent theodicies might serve as a protective factor against the
negative mental health impact of adverse life events. The existing
evidence base has focused on samples dealing with a range of stressors,
but it is important to see how well this theorizing holds up in the
context of collectively experienced traumatic events such as natural
disasters.
Overview and Hypotheses
Accordingly, the purpose of the present study of natural disaster
survivors was to examine the link between belief violations and PTSD
symptoms and then test whether benevolent theodicies buffered this
relationship. We focused on survivors of the 2016 Louisiana flood.
Because this flood was one of the costliest and most severe natural
disasters to hit the U.S. in the past 5 years (Yan & Flores, 2016),
and because Louisiana is one of the most religious states in the U.S.
(Lipka & Wormald, 2016), survivors of this flood represent an
optimal population for studying the role of religion in mitigating the
negative mental health impact of disasters. The 2016 Louisiana flood
began between August 12 and 15, 2016, as vast areas of southern
Louisiana received 20 to 30 inches of rain, resulting in catastrophic
flooding (NOAA, 2018). This flooding rendered 200 roadways impassable
(including parts of two interstate highways) and damaged over 109,000
homes, 90,000 vehicles, and 6,000 businesses ("AIR Worldwide,"
2016; Funes, 2016; Terrell, 2016). In all, the 2016 Louisiana flood
caused over $10 billion in damage, ranking it among the 25 costliest
natural disasters in U.S. history (NOAA, 2018; Yan & Flores, 2016).
Thirteen people lost their lives (NOAA, 2018). Additionally, more than
30,000 people and 1,000 pets had to be rescued by emergency responders
and civilian volunteers (Di Liberto, 2016; Visser et al., 2016). A large
percentage of these survivors lived in areas that were also hit by
Hurricane Katrina in 2005 (Lipinski, 2016).
In the current study, we predicted that higher levels of belief
violations would be positively associated with PTSD symptoms (Hypothesis
1). Moreover, we predicted that benevolent theodicies would moderate the
relationship between belief violations and PTSD symptoms, such that
greater endorsement of benevolent theodicies would attenuate the
relationship between belief violations and PTSD symptoms (Hypothesis 2).
See Figure 1 for a conceptual diagram of the moderation effect we test
in Hypothesis 2.
Method
Participants and Procedure
One month after the 2016 Louisiana flood, we recruited adults from
Baton Rouge, Louisiana and its surrounding communities. Participants
were recruited through local community agencies (e.g., faith communities
and mental health clinics), and they completed study measures via an
online survey platform. They were compensated with a $25 gift card
either to Amazon.com or Walmart. Individuals were eligible to
participate if they were at least 18 years old and had been living in
one of the flood-affected Louisiana parishes at the time of the flood.
Initially, 935 participants either partially or fully completed the
survey. Participants were excluded from analysis if they failed more
than one of the attention checks (e.g., validity questions such as
"Please respond to this question with 'moderately'";
n = 190) or completed the survey in an impossibly fast amount of time (n
= 180). Additionally, participants were excluded from analysis if they
did not meet inclusion criteria (i.e., did not reside in Louisiana at
the time of the flood) or provided duplicate responses (determined by
the same name/email combination entered more than once; n = 119). Some
participants failed more than one of the attention checks or inclusion
criteria; ultimately, 469 people were excluded from data analysis.
The final sample consisted of 466 participants (46.1% women) who
ranged in age from 20 to 77 years old (M = 36.69, SD = 7.21).
Participants were mostly White (75.5%), with smaller numbers of Latino/a
(10.7%), Black/African-American (10.3%), and other races (< 2% each
for Asian, Pacific Islander, American Indian, or multiracial). The most
frequently endorsed household income category was $100,000 to $149,999
(28.5%), followed by $50,000 to $74,999 (23.8%) and $75,000 to $99,999
(21.9%). Additionally, most participants were currently married (85.4%)
and had a bachelor's degree or higher (61.4%).
Most participants were religiously affiliated (89.3%).
Specifically, participants identified as Christian Protestant (42.7%),
Christian Catholic (34.3%), Mormon (1.9%), Orthodox Christian (0.4%),
Jewish (2.8%), Muslim (5.4%), Buddhist (1.1%), Hindu (0.6%), atheist
(1.7%), agnostic (0.4%), and nothing in particular (8.6%). This sample
is religiously representative of Louisiana (www.pewforum.org). The
sample was highly religious; on a scale from 1 (not at all important) to
4 (very important), the average religious importance rating was 3.34 (SD
= 0.82).
When asked to use a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not stressful to 5 =
extremely stressful) to indicate how stressful the flood was for them,
the average level of disaster-related stress was 3.23 (SD = 0.98).
Additionally, 29.0% of participants responded "yes" when asked
if they were still dealing with some negative consequences from the
flood, whereas 56.4% said "somewhat" and 14.6% said
"no." Around half the participants (53 2%) did not have flood
insurance. Finally, 64.6% of participants indicated that, in 2005, they
lived in an area affected by Hurricane Katrina.
The materials and measures presented here are drawn from a larger
set of measures administered to this sample. The measures that are not
discussed here address other research questions, but the results
presented in the current study have not been published elsewhere.
Measures
Belief violations. We assessed belief violations using the 5-item
Belief Violations subscale from the Global Meaning Violation Scale
(GMVS; Park et al., 2016). Respondents were asked to use the 5-point
GMVS Likert scale (1 [not at all] to 5 [very much]) to rate each of the
five items in response to the following prompt: "When you think
about how you felt before and after experiencing the 2016 Louisiana
flood ..." Two example items are "How much does this stressful
experience violate your sense that God is in control?" and
"How much does the occurrence of this stressful experience violate
your sense of the world being fair or just?" Item scores are summed
to yield a total score, with higher scores reflecting higher belief
violations. The GMVS has demonstrated strong evidence of reliability and
of concurrent, discriminant, and predictive validity (Park et al.,
2016). For the present sample, Cronbach's alpha was .81.
PTSD symptoms. We assessed PTSD symptoms using the 20-item
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5; Blevins et
al., 2015), which measures PTSD symptoms according to the PTSD symptom
criteria indicated in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The PCL-5 asks
respondents to use a 5-point Likert scale (0 [not at all] to 4
[extremely]) to indicate "how much you have been bothered by that
problem in the last month." Two example items are "Repeated,
disturbing, and unwanted memories of the stressful experience" and
"Having strong negative beliefs about yourself, other people, or
the world (for example, having thoughts such as: I am bad, there is
something seriously wrong with me, no one can be trusted, the world is
completely dangerous)." item scores are summed to yield a total
score, with higher scores indicating higher levels of PTSD symptoms. The
PCL-5 has demonstrated strong evidence of reliability and of convergent,
discriminant, and predictive validity (Blevins et al., 2015; Bovin et
al., 2016). For the present sample, Cronbach's alpha for the PCL-5
total score was .95.
Benevolent theodicies. We assessed benevolent theodicies using
three subscales from the Views of Suffering Scale (VOSS; Hale-Smith et
al., 2012): the Providence, Suffering God, and Soul Building subscales.
Each of these subscales consists of three items and asks respondents to
indicate the extent of their belief or disbelief, using a 6-point Likert
scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Three
sample items are: "Everything that we experience--including
suffering--is planned in detail by God" (Providence subscale),
"When we suffer, God is suffering along with us" (Suffering
God subscale), and "Suffering is intended by God to be a source of
personal growth" (Soul-Building subscale). Each subscale's
responses are summed to create a subscale score, with higher scores
reflecting higher levels of providence theodicy beliefs, suffering-God
theodicy beliefs, and soul-building theodicy beliefs, respectively. The
VoSS has demonstrated strong evidence of reliability and of concurrent,
discriminant, and predictive validity (Hale-Smith et al., 2012; Wilt et
al., 2017). For the present sample, Cronbach's alphas for the three
VoSS subscales were .80, .83, and .75, respectively.
Results
Before imputing data, we examined patterns of missing data. Less
than 2% of data were missing per item. Little's MCAR test was
significant; however, because less than 1% of data was missing overall,
we could move forward with imputation without introducing bias
(Schlomer, Bauman, & Card, 2010). We used expectation maximization
to impute missing data.
Next, we examined the data for outliers and normality. outliers (2%
or less per variable) were adjusted to three standard deviations from
the mean. Skewness and kurtosis values were all within an acceptable
range (between -1 and +1). In Table 1, we report the psychometric
properties and bivariate correlations of study variables.
Hypothesis 1: Are Belief Violations Related to PTSD Symptoms?
In support of Hypothesis 1, belief violations were strongly
positively correlated with PTSD symptoms (r = .62, p < .001).
Interestingly, belief violations were positively related to all three of
the assessed benevolent theodicies (rs = .17 to .29, all ps < .001),
and PTSD symptoms also were positively correlated with providence
theodicy beliefs (r = .31, p < .001) and with suffering-God theodicy
beliefs (r = .23, p < .001).
Hypothesis 2: Do Benevolent Theodicies Buffer the Negative Effects
of Belief Violations?
To test Hypotheses 2, we used Model 1 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS
(Hayes, 2013), which tests for a single moderator (M [benevolent
theodicies]) of the relationship between a single antecedent variable (X
[belief violations]) and a single outcome variable (Y [PTSD symptoms]).
We ran three separate moderation analyses to test the moderation effect
for each of the three benevolent theodicies we assessed: providence
theodicy beliefs ([M.sub.1]), suffering-God theodicy beliefs
([M.sub.2]), and soul-building theodicy beliefs ([M.sub.3]). Belief
violations and benevolent theodicies were mean-centered prior to
analysis. See Table 2 for results of these three separate regression
analyses, and see Table 3 for a summary of the conditional effect of
belief violations on post-disaster PTSD symptoms at values of each
distinct moderator.
Providence theodicies. As can be seen in the first moderation
analysis, belief violations were positively related to PTSD symptoms (b
= 2.16, p < .001), but providence theodicy beliefs were not (p =
.420). supporting Hypothesis 2, the moderation effect was significant,
indicating the interaction between belief violations and providence
theodicy beliefs is predictive of PTSD symptoms, b = -.15, p < .001;
[DELTA][R.sup.2] = .03, F(1, 462) = 11.87, p < .001. Specifically,
the association between belief violations and PTSD symptoms was reduced
at higher levels of providence theodicy beliefs.
Suffering-God theodicies. Similarly, in the second moderation
analysis, belief violations were positively related to PTSD symptoms (b
= 2.19, p < .001), but suffering-God theodicy beliefs were not (p =
.649). Again, supporting Hypothesis 2, the moderation effect was
significant, indicating the interaction between belief violations and
suffering-God theodicy beliefs is predictive of PTSD symptoms, b = -.16,
p < .001; [DELTA][R.sup.2] = .03, F(1, 462) = 21.30, p < .001.
Specifically, the association between belief violations and PTSD
symptoms was reduced at higher levels of suffering-God theodicy beliefs.
Soul-building theodicies. in the third and final moderation
analysis, both belief violations (b = 2.36, p < .001) and
soul-building theodicy beliefs (b = -0.50, p = .034) were related to
PTSD symptoms. Supporting Hypothesis 2, the moderation effect was
significant, indicating the interaction between belief violations and
soul-building theodicy beliefs is predictive of PTSD symptoms, b = .12,
p = .006; [DELTA][R.sup.2] = .01, F(1, 462) = 7.78, p = .006.
Specifically, the association between belief violations and PTSD
symptoms was reduced at higher levels of soul-building theodicy beliefs.
Discussion
The main purpose of this study was to use a community sample of
natural disaster survivors to examine whether the relationship between
belief violations and post-disaster PTSD symptoms is attenuated by
benevolent theodicies. Prior work has shown a link between belief
violations and PTSD (George et al., 2016; Park et al., 2012), as well as
between benevolent theodicies and various indices of psychological
well-being (Currier et al., 2017; Exline et al., 2017; Wilt et al.,
2016). However, most of this work has been done with undergraduate
samples dealing with nonspecific stressors (Wilt et al., 2016) or
specific individual stressors (Exline et al., 2017), so it was important
to replicate and extend this work to the role of benevolent theodicies
in buffering the negative mental health consequences of collectively
experienced traumatic events (e.g., natural disasters).
Consistent with our hypotheses and replicating and extending prior
work (George et al., 2016; Park et al., 2012), we found belief
violations had a strong positive relationship with PTSD symptoms. This
finding supports the predictions of stress response theory (Horowitz,
1986) and shattered assumptions theory (Janoff-Bulman, 1992), suggesting
that in the acute wake of disasters, the degree to which survivors
experience PTSD symptoms is heavily tied to the degree to which their
beliefs about God, the world, and themselves have been violated. Park
(2016) has made similar theoretical predictions when discussing her
meaning-making model in the context of disasters.
We also found that providence and suffering-God theodicies were
weakly to moderately related to PTSD symptoms, which may seem
counterintuitive. However, these associations correspond with findings
in prior work. The bivariate correlations can be misleading and do not
take into account the potential influence of stressor severity on
engagement with God (or another deity) as a means of coping. For
example, Wilt et al. (2016) found weak to moderate correlations between
benevolent theodicies and both greater divine struggle and greater
psychological distress, and Gerber, Boals, and Schuettler (2011) found a
weak relationship between positive religious coping and higher PTSD
symptoms. Taken together with our results, these findings suggest that,
in the midst of suffering, people in distress may tend to engage with
their theodicies about God (Wilt et al., 2016) and cope by cultivating
their perceived relationship with God (Gerber et al., 2011), even if it
means expressing some anger or disappointment toward God. This
possibility is consistent with claims that a mature religious faith
allows for some level of R/S struggle (Exline et al., 2017; Pargament,
2007; Wilt et al., 2016).
Our primary hypotheses regarding moderation were supported: the
relationship between belief violations and PTSD symptoms was moderated
by benevolent theodicies. Specifically, as belief violations increased,
flood survivors who endorsed greater belief that God was in control of
their suffering, was suffering compassionately alongside them, and was
building their character through suffering tended to report fewer
symptoms of PTSD than survivors who held less benevolent theodicies. As
suggested by previous studies, following traumatic events, survivors who
hold more benevolent theodicies may tend to engage in more positive
religious coping, which often leads to better psychological outcomes
(Park, Currier, Harris, & Slattery, 2017; cf. Exline et al., 2017;
Pargament, 2007; Wilt et al., 2016, 2017).
These results have important implications for disaster preparedness
and response. Specifically, our findings suggest there is value in
helping people cultivate spiritual resources in addition to physical
resources (e.g., shelter planning). Indeed, these findings suggest that
benevolent theodicies may serve as a protective factor for
disaster-affected people and communities. As such, benevolent theodicies
could be a potential intervention target for disaster preparedness and
response efforts. For example, preparedness efforts might involve
helping theistic religious believers prepare for disasters by
cultivating benevolent theodicies during times when life is going well.
Then, in the aftermath of a disaster, people providing disaster
spiritual and emotional care can help survivors remember and nurture
those theodicies, thereby helping buffer against common post-disaster
reactions such as PTSD, anxiety, or depression.
This possibility requires disaster care providers to be both
competent and comfortable with addressing religious and spiritual
concerns. However, commonly utilized disaster mental health
interventions (e.g., Psychological First Aid [PFA]) do not explicitly
target survivors' religion and spirituality. To fill this gap, Aten
et al. (2018) have developed a spiritually integrated disaster mental
health intervention called Spiritual First Aid (SFA). SFA addresses
survivors' immediate practical needs as well as their spiritual
distress. It assesses survivors' spiritual resources and connects
survivors with additional spiritual coping resources if needed (e.g., a
faith community; mental health care). Drawing from nearly 40 years of
psychological research on religion and spirituality, SFA is an
evidence-informed intervention designed to facilitate survivors'
holistic wellness and improve their post-disaster trajectory of
recovery. In line with results of the current study, SFA addresses
spiritual distress by adopting a strengths-based approach that helps
survivors draw upon their existing positive spiritual resources (e.g.,
benevolent theodicies) in order to prevent or mitigate post-disaster
spiritual and psychological distress.
Strengths, Limitations, and Future Research
The current study replicates and extends previous work on belief
violations, benevolent theodicies, and the role of R/S in mitigating
negative post-disaster reactions. It has several strengths, including
the use of a community sample of disaster survivors, comprised of
roughly equal numbers of men and women and encompassing a wide range of
ages. Moreover, this study exhibits high external validity in that data
was collected from survivors of a natural disaster, within one month
post-disaster, and using a highly religious (yet religiously
representative) sample.
Even so, this study is not without limitations. First, our sample
was roughly 75% White and 75% Christian, and it over-represented people
who are currently married, are of middle or upper socioeconomic status,
and/or have a bachelor's degree or higher. Therefore, it remains
unclear whether these findings are generalizable to racial/ethnic
minorities and religious minorities (e.g., people who are religiously
unaffiliated or adhere to a non-Christian faith), as well as to
individuals who are not currently married or have a lower socioeconomic
status or education level. Second, we tested our moderation hypotheses
using a cross-sectional design, but a longitudinal design would provide
stronger evidence for how belief violations and benevolent theodicies
influence the trajectory of PTSD over time. Third, we assessed PTsD
symptoms broadly, using the standard instructions of the PCL-5 (which do
not ask respondents whether their trauma symptoms are related
specifically to the disaster). Future disaster researchers might adapt
the PCL-5 instructions to ask specifically about disaster-related PTSD
symptoms. Lastly, experimental research would help clarify the pattern
of relationships we examined in this study (i.e., whether disasters
cause belief violations, which in turn cause PTSD symptoms, and whether
benevolent theodicies might attenuate this causal relationship), and
intervention research (e.g., comparing SFA and PFA) could help explore
whether interventions targeted at increasing benevolent theodicies might
be effective in mitigating post-disaster PTSD symptoms as well. Lastly,
more research is needed to replicate and extend this line of inquiry by
comparing survivors' responses to different types of disasters
(e.g., natural disasters versus human-caused disasters) in order to
determine whether various disaster-related stressors elicit different
R/S responses and/or different moderating effects of benevolent
theodicies.
Indeed, we hope the next frontier in disaster research includes a
focus on identifying factors (e.g., benevolent theodicies) that can help
protect disaster-affected individuals and communities from negative
post-disaster reactions. Similarly, researchers can work to develop and
test spiritually integrated disaster mental health interventions (e.g.,
SFA) that focus on harnessing survivors' R/S in ways that can
promote their health, well-being, and resilience. Ultimately, having a
better understanding of the ways disaster survivors might adaptively
engage their R/S can lead to interventions that are both
evidence-informed and culturally responsive.
References
AIR Worldwide: Louisiana's record rainfall caused by
low-pressure system. (2016, August 16). Insurance Journal. Retrieved
from http://www.mynewmarkets.com
Aldwin, C. M. (2007). Stress, coping, and development: An
integrative approach. New York, NY: Guildford.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders, 5th ed., VA: American Psychiatric
Association.
Aten, J. D., Shannonhouse, L., Davis, D. E., Davis, E. B., Hook, J.
N., Van Tongeren, D. R., ... Leonard, L. (2018). Spiritual first aid.
Manuscript in progress.
Belles, J., Dolce, C., & Erdman, J. (2016). 2016 hurricane
season recap: 10 things we will remember. Retrieved from:
https://weather.com/storms/hurricane/news/hurricane-season-2016-atlantic-recap.
Blevins, C. A., Weathers, F. W., Davis, M. T., Witte, T. K., &
Domino, J. L. (2015). The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for
DSM-5 (PCL-5): Development and initial psychometric evaluation. Journal
of Traumatic Stress, 28, 489-498.
Bovin, M. J., Marx, B. P., Weathers, F. W., Gallagher, M. W.,
Rodriguez, P., Schnurr, P. P., & Keane, T. M. (2016). Psychometric
properties of the PTSD Checklist for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders--Fifth Edition (PCL-5) in veterans. Psychological
Assessment, 28, 1379-1391.
Brown, R. F. (1999). Theodicy. In E. Fahlbusch & G. Bromiley
(Eds.), The encyclopedia of Christianity (Vol. 5, pp. 353-356). Grand
Rapids, MI: Eerdmans.
Bussing, A., Ostermann, T., & Matthiessen, P. F. (2005). The
role of religion and spirituality in medical patients in Germany.
Journal of Religion and Health, 44, 321-340.
Currier, J. M., Drescher, K. D., Nieuwsma, J. A., & McCormick,
W. H. (2017). Theodicies and professional quality of life in a
nationally representative sample of chaplains in the veterans'
health administration. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the
Community, 45, 286-296.
Chukwuorji, J. B. C., Ifeagwazi, C. M., & Eze, J. E. (2017).
Event centrality influences post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms via
core beliefs in internally displaced older adults. Aging & Mental
Health. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1396580
Di Liberto, T. (2016, August 19). August 2016 extreme rain and
floods along the Gulf Coast. NOAA. Retrieved from http://www.climate.gov
Exline, J. J., Hall, T. W., Pargament, K. I., & Harriott, V. A.
(2017). Predictors of growth from spiritual struggle among Christian
undergraduates: Religious coping and perceptions of helpful action by
God are both important. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 12, 501-508.
Funes, Y. (2016, August 30). Latest numbers show Louisiana flooding
was worse than initially thought. Color Lines. Retrieved from
http://www.colorlines.com
George, L. S., Park, C. L., & Chaudoir, S. R. (2016). Examining
the relationship between trauma centrality and posttraumatic stress
disorder symptoms: A moderated mediation approach. Traumatology, 22,
85-93.
Gerber, M. M., Boals, A., & Schuettler, D. (2011). The unique
contributions of positive and negative religious coping to posttraumatic
growth and PTSD. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 3, 298-307.
Hale-Smith, A., Park, C. L., & Edmondson, D. (2012). Measuring
beliefs about suffering: Development of the Views of Suffering Scale.
Psychological Assessment, 24, 855-866.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and
conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY:
Guilford Publications.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new
psychology of trauma. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Lipinski, J. (2016, August 16). For many Katrina survivors,
Louisiana Flood of 2016 revives trauma. The Time-Picayune. Retrieved
from www.nola.com
Lipka, M., & Wormald, B. (2016). How religious is your state?
Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/02/29/how-religious-is- your-state/
National Centers for Environmental Information (NCEI). (2018). U.S.
billion-dollar weather and climate disasters. Retrieved from
https://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/billions
Pargament, K. I. (2007). Spiritually integrated psychotherapy:
Understanding and addressing the sacred. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Park, C. L. (2016). Meaning making in the context of disasters.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72, 1234-1246.
Park, C., Currier, J., Harris, I., & Slattery, J. (2017).
Trauma, meaning, and spirituality: Translating research into clinical
practice. Washington, DC: APA.
Park, C. L., Mills, M. A., & Edmondson, D. (2012). PTSD as
meaning violation: Testing a cognitive worldview perspective.
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4, 66-73.
Park, C. L., Riley, K. E., George, L. S., Gutierrez, I. A., Hale,
A. E., Cho, D., & Braun, T. D. (2016). Assessing disruptions in
meaning: Development of the Global Meaning Violation Scale. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 40, 831-846.
Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., & Card, N. A. (2010). Best
practices for missing data management in counseling psychology. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 57, 1-10.
Terrell, D. (2016). The economic impact of the August 2016 floods
on the State of Louisiana: Louisiana Economic Development. Lake Charles,
LA: Lewis Terrell & Associates, LLC.
Visser, S., Jackson, A. Yan, H., & Flores, R. (2016, August
18). Louisiana flooding: Cajun Navy answers call for volunteers. CNN.
Retrieved from http://www.cnn.com
Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Grubbs, J. B., Park, C. L., &
Pargament, K. I. (2016). God's role in suffering: Theodicies,
divine struggle, and mental health. Psychology of Religion and
Spirituality, 8, 352-362.
Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Grubbs, J. B., Park, C. L., &
Pargament, K. I. (2017). Theological beliefs about suffering and
interactions with the divine. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality,
9, 137-147.
Yan, H., & Flores, R. (2016, August 19). Louisiana flood: Worst
US disaster since Hurricane Sandy, Red Cross says. CNN. Retrieved from
http://www.cnn.com
Stacey E. McElroy-Heltzel
Georgia State University
Edward B. Davis
Wheaton College
Don E. Davis
Georgia State University
Jamie D. Aten
Wheaton College
Daryl R. Van Tongeren
Hope College
Joshua N. Hook
University of North Texas
Jenny Hwang
Wheaton College
This publication was made possible through the support of a grant
from the John Templeton Foundation (Grant #44040). The opinions
expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not
necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton Foundation.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stacey
McElroy-Heltzel, Department of Counselling and Psychological Services,
Georgia State University, 30 Pryor Street, Atlanta, GA 30303;
[email protected]
Authors
Stacey E. McElroy-Heltzel (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology, Georgia
State University) is a Postdoctoral Research Associate at Georgia State
University. Dr. McElroy-Heltzel's research interests include
intercultural couples, humility, and religion/spirituality.
Edward B. Davis (Psy.D, Regent University) is an Associate
Professor of Psychology at Wheaton College (IL). His research focuses on
the psychology of religion and spirituality, especially relational
spirituality, God representations, disasters, and positive psychology.
Don E. Davis (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology) is an Associate
Professor of Psychology at Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA. His
research interests include positive psychology--especially relational
virtues of humility, forgiveness, and gratitude--as well as integration
of spirituality into counseling.
Jamie D. Aten (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology, Indiana State
University) is the Dr. Arthur P. Rech and Mrs. Jean May Rech Associate
Professor of Psychology and the Founder and Executive Director of the
Humanitarian Disaster Institute at Wheaton College. Dr. Aten's
primary professional interests include the psychology of
religion/spirituality and disasters, spiritually oriented disaster
psychology, and psychology in disaster ministry.
Joshua N. Hook (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology, Virginia
Commonwealth University) is an Associate Professor of Psychology at the
University of North Texas. Dr. Hook's research interests include
humility, religion/spirituality, and multicultural counseling. He also
blogs regularly at www.joshuanhook.com.
Daryl R. Van Tongeren (Ph.D. in Social Psychology, Virginia
Commonwealth University) is an Associate Professor of Psychology at Hope
College. He studies the psychology of meaning, religion, virtues, and
suffering.
Jenny Hwang (M.A. International Disaster Psychology, University of
Denver) is the Managing Director for the Humanitarian Disaster Institute
at Wheaton College. Her research interests includes global mental health
(in the context of disaster settings), human trafficking, cross cultural
communication, and refugee and immigrant mental health.
Caption: Figure 1. Hypothesis 2: Conceptual diagram depicting the
moderation of the effect of belief violations (X) on post-disaster PTSD
symptoms (Y) by benevolent theodicies (e.g., beliefs that God has
providential control over suffering [providence theodicy, [M.sub.1]],
willfully shares in our suffering [suffering-God theodicy, [M.sub.2]],
and uses suffering to build our character [soul-building theodicy,
[M.sub.3]]). Stated differently, benevolent views of God's role in
suffering are the boundary condition that influences how much of an
effect belief violations have on post-disaster PTSD symptoms.
Table 1
Psychometric Properties and Intercorrelations of Study Variables
Variable M SD Possible Actual
Range Range
1. Belief Violations 13.78 4.27 5-25 5-25
2. PTSD Symptoms 30.26 15.99 0-80 0-75
3. Providence Theodicy 10.64 3.60 3-18 3-18
4. Suffering-God Theodicy 11.56 3.76 3-18 3-18
5. Soul-building Theodicy 11.80 3.11 3-18 3-18
Variable 1 2 3 4 5
1. Belief Violations (.81)
2. PTSD Symptoms .62 * (.95)
3. Providence Theodicy .29 * .31 * (.80)
4. Suffering-God Theodicy .29 * .23 * .65 * (.83)
5. Soul-building Theodicy .17 * .05 .51 * .64 * (.75)
Note. N = 466. PTSD = Post-traumatic Stress Disorder. Coefficient
alphas are presented along the diagonal.
* p < .001.
Table 2
Results from Regression Analyses Examining the Moderation of the
Effect of Belief Violations (X) on Post-disaster PTSD Symptoms
(Y) by Benevolent Theodicies (M)
Coeff. SE t P
Intercept [i.sub.1] 30.94 .60 51.72 .000
Belief violations (X) [b.sub.1] 2.16 .16 13.26 .000
Providence theodicy [b.sub.2] 0.21 .26 0.81 .420
([M.sub.1])
Moderation effect [b.sub.3] -0.15 .04 -3.45 .001
(X [M.sub.1])
[R.sup.2] = .43, MSE = 146.17
F(3, 462) = 199.56, p < .001
Intercept [i.sub.1] 31.00 .61 50.51 .000
Belief violations (X) [b.sub.1] 2.19 .16 13.36 .000
Suffering-God theodicy [b.sub.2] -0.09 .20 -0.45 .649
([M.sub.2])
Moderation effect [b.sub.3] -0.16 .03 -4.62 .000
(X [M.sub.2])
[R.sup.2] = .42, MSE = 148.29
F(3, 462) = 167.83, p < .001
Intercept [i.sub.1] 30.52 .59 51.54 .000
Belief violations (X) [b.sub.1] 2.36 .15 16.13 .000
Soul-building theodicy [b.sub.2] -0.50 .23 -2.13 .034
([M.sub.3])
Moderation effect [b.sub.3] -0.12 .04 -2.79 .006
(X [M.sub.3])
[R.sup.2] = .40, MSE = 153.90
F(3, 462) = 111.80, p < .001
Note. N = 466. SE = standard error. Belief violations (X) and
benevolent theodicies (M) were mean-centered prior to analysis.
Table 3
Conditional Effects of Belief Violations (X) on Post-disaster PTSD
Symptoms (Y) at Values of the Moderator Benevolent Theodicies (M)
Moderator (M) Effect SE t
Providence theodicy (M1)
Low providence theodicy beliefs 2.71 .19 14.39
Average providence theodicy beliefs 2.16 .16 13.26
High providence theodicy beliefs 1.62 .26 6.21
Suffering-God theodicy (M2)
Low suffering-God theodicy beliefs 2.78 .18 15.71
Average suffering-God theodicy beliefs 2.18 .16 13.36
High suffering-God theodicy beliefs 1.59 .24 6.74
Soul-Building theodicy (M3)
Low soul-building theodicy beliefs 2.73 .18 15.11
Average soul-building theodicy beliefs 2.36 .15 16.13
High soul-building theodicy beliefs 2.00 .21 9.42
Moderator (M) p CI
Providence theodicy (M1)
Low providence theodicy beliefs .000 [2.34, 3.08]
Average providence theodicy beliefs .000 [1.84, 2.48]
High providence theodicy beliefs .000 [1.11, 2.13]
Suffering-God theodicy (M2)
Low suffering-God theodicy beliefs .000 [2.43, 3.12]
Average suffering-God theodicy beliefs .000 [1.86, 2.50]
High suffering-God theodicy beliefs .000 [1.12, 2.05]
Soul-Building theodicy (M3)
Low soul-building theodicy beliefs .000 [2.38, 3.09]
Average soul-building theodicy beliefs .000 [2.08, 2.65]
High soul-building theodicy beliefs .000 [1.58, 2.41]
Note. N= 466. SE= standard error; CI = confidence interval. Belief
violations (X) and benevolent theodicies (M) were mean-centered
prior to analysis. Values for each moderator are the mean and
plus/minus one standard deviation from the mean.
COPYRIGHT 2018 CAPS International (Christian Association for Psychological Studies)
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.