The effect of youth alcohol initiation on high school completion.
Koch, Steven F. ; McGeary, Kerry Anne
I. INTRODUCTION
Alcohol consumption by youths is viewed as a critical problem in
the United States, and research concerning the effects of youth alcohol
consumption has borne out this view. Grant and Dawson (1997), Moore and
Cook (1995), and Wilson et al. (2002) find that youths who drink at an
earlier age are more susceptible to future problem drinking. Figlio
(1995) finds that youthful consumers of alcohol are more likely to be
involved in traffic accidents. Furthermore, Benham and Benham (1982),
Cook and Moore (1993), Mullahy and Sindelar (1994), and Yamada et al.
(1996) find that these same youths are less likely to succeed in school.
More generally, heavy and problem drinking reduces adult earnings, as
evidenced in Harwood et al. (1984), Mullahy and Sindelar (1993), Rice et
al. (1990), and Rice (1993). The preceding results have influenced
federal legislative efforts to raise the minimum legal drinking age to
21 years, reductions in the availability of alcohol on college campuses,
increased conspicuousness of youth driver's licenses, and increased
civil and criminal penalties for adults who furnish alcohol to minors.
Although a large body of research, prime examples of which have
already been cited, shows a strong negative relationship between alcohol
consumption and measures of socioeconomic status, other researchers have
called these results into question. For example, Dee and Evans (1999)
find minimal effects of youth drinking on education. In addition,
although Burgess and Propper (1998) find detrimental future effects due
to behavioral problems in early life, early alcohol consumption would
not be defined as a behavioral problem, due to its negligible effects on
later life outcomes. Also, Koch and Ribar (2001) show that increasing
the age of alcohol consumption onset does not appreciably increase the
years of completed schooling.
It is possible that Burgess and Propper's (1998) and Koch and
Ribar's (2001) results are due to the fact that they examine
outcomes years afterward. Burgess and Propper examine productivity and
household formation 10 years after the problem is uncovered, and Koch
and Ribar examine education completion at age 25, which is 8 years
beyond the sample average alcohol consumption onset. Therefore, many of
the people in these analyses may have had trouble with alcohol during
their high school years but were able to return to a more productive
lifestyle later. In that case, these two aforementioned research
projects may not have uncovered the true effects of early alcohol use.
Although these papers, as well as Dee and Evans (1999), take care to
instrument for the simultaneity between schooling decisions and alcohol
consumption decisions, little consideration is given to the social
complementarities of alcohol consumption and schooling. For example,
Koch and Ribar (2001) use sibling data to control for within-family
similarities in the propensity to consume alcohol and obtain further
education; Burgess and Propper (1998) include few controls for
endogeneity' Dee and Evans (1999) use state-level cohort data,
where there are unlikely to be any peer effects concerning drinking and
schooling decisions.
In this article, we examine more carefully the relationship between
youthful drinking and high school success using data from the 1979-96
panels of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). High
school-aged individuals are chosen in an effort to see whether youthful
alcohol consumption initiation, rather than contemporaneous consumption,
affects education attainment in the short run. Initiation is important
because short-run adverse education consequences may result in large
cumulative effects on human capital and later socioeconomic status. The
fact that high school and the onset of drinking occur proximately for
much of the sample makes the estimation of a direct effect of alcohol
consumption onset on school progress plausible.
We find that alcohol consumption onset has a statistically
significant negative influence on high school completion. In addition,
we find that failure to control endogeneity, which could work through
the social intricacies of high school because students are likely to
form peer groups and those peer groups are likely to significantly
influence student drinking and schooling behavior, leads to results that
are largely understated. For instance, we find that after controlling
for potential social factors, a young student, who had initiated regular
alcohol consumption by the age of 14, would be nearly one-quarter less
likely to receive a high school diploma by the age of 20; young alcohol
consumers are nearly one-tenth less likely to complete high school,
either through a diploma or a General Equivalency Degree (GED) by the
age of 20. Most tellingly, youths who had begun to regularly consume
alcohol by the age of 14 and had dropped out of school were nearly
one-half as likely to complete their high school education after the age
of 20 than those who did not regularly consume alcohol by the age of 14.
These estimated negative effects are much larger than were estimated
when ignoring the endogeneity between alcohol consumption and schooling
decisions.
The rest of this article is organized as follows. In section II, we
provide economic explanations for why early drinking might be negatively
associated with progress in education and further review some of the
empirical literature on the subject. We continue in section III by
describing the statistical issues associated with modeling these
relationships and laying out our econometric approach. In section IV, we
describe the data sources and variables used in the analysis, as well as
highlight differences between and across data subsamples. Estimation
results and specification comparisons are reported and discussed in
section V. Section VI contains conclusions and recommendations for
further research.
II. BACKGROUND
The effect of alcohol consumption on schooling is examined in the
context of a simple model of human capital investment, similar to Becker
(1965) or Grossman (1972). Children are endowed at birth with a basic
level of human capital, which includes natural affinities to certain
activities (physical and intellectual), inherent healthfulness, and
other genetic information. Human capital evolves through childhood due
primarily to parental actions (or the work of others who might be
raising or surrounding the child). As children age, they start making
decisions for themselves. In this article, we consider how and for how
long adolescent decisions regarding alcohol use will affect the
child's education completion.
In addition to the assumptions concerning childhood endowments, we
assume that progress in school requires time in the form of attendance
and studying, and the consumption of alcohol is assumed to require money
and time expenditures. Assuming youths have preferences regarding
schooling, drinking, and their consumption of other goods, a direct
negative effect of drinking on schooling is plausible if it diverts
individual resources away from schooling. Drinking might have other
negative spillover effects on schooling progress if investments in
education are less productive, due to alcohol consumption. For example,
Weschler et al. (2000) report that alcohol binge drinking by college
students makes them less likely to attend class or stay current with
their studies. If there are negative psychological effects related to
consumption, alcohol consumption might lower the individual's
desire or ability to perform. For example, recent biological research by
Deas and colleagues (2000) suggests that brain function may be adversely
affected by youthful alcohol abuse.
Although contemporaneous trade-offs between studying and drinking
imply a direct adverse effect of drinking on schooling progress, the
model is also consistent with other explanations for a negative
association. School advancement and alcohol consumption, as suggested at
the outset, also depend on family background characteristics, like
parental supervision or parental attitudes to alcohol consumption.
Attitudes regarding schooling and drinking are also likely to be shaped
by peer influences and other childhood experiences. Ignoring these
relevant characteristics might lead to the estimation of a spurious negative correlation between adolescent drinking behavior and schooling
progress, or might bias the estimated correlation negatively or
positively.
It is possible however, that dropping out of school can more easily
support alcohol consumption. (1) In this case, school leaving may have
drinking consequences rather than the other way around. Accordingly, the
examination of educational progress, or any other potential
socioeconomic consequence, and alcohol consumption must consider issues
of omitted variables and endogeneity bias. In our analysis, however, we
account for alcohol consumption onset at the age of 14, the age at which
most people enter high school. Using a measure of alcohol consumption,
which predates the schooling continuation decision(s), eliminates
contemporaneous endogeneity bias. (2) However, as our model suggests,
many childhood background characteristics, which are not completely
observable, should be included in the estimation. If we are not able to
include them, it is necessary to allow for the probable correlation
between the unobservables in each equation. The complete model, which
will be discussed in section III, will allow for correlations between
the unobservable determinants of drinking onset and education
completion.
The link between alcohol consumption and education has been
examined as early as Benham and Benham (1982). Employing a sample of St.
Louis youths from the 1910s and 1920s, who were surveyed again 30 years
later, they found that drinking problems reduced schooling by about 1.5
years. Cook and Moore (1993), using the NLSY, as we do, estimated
structural and reduced form equations for education completion
(including highest year completed and college graduation) and youthful
alcohol consumption (measured by drinks per week, frequency of drinking,
and frequency of being drunk). To allay concerns over endogeneity bias,
they used state beer taxes and minimum drinking age laws as instruments
for alcohol consumption. Their instrumental variables procedures
generated large but relatively imprecise estimates of the effect of
drinking on schooling. (3) As a check, they provided reduced form
estimates showing that individuals from states with higher minimum
drinking age laws and higher beer taxes were more likely to complete
high school.
We cannot be certain, however, that Cook and Moore's (1993)
estimated effect of alcohol policy variables on schooling is, itself,
not spurious. Dee and Evans (1999) show that college entrance is less
likely in states with higher cigarette excise taxes, with lower taxes on
gasoline, without gun purchase waiting periods, with a death penalty,
and with a 65 mph speed limit on the freeway. Their results imply that
education policy and outcomes are linked to other government policies
within a state, and for that reason, the use of within-state alcohol
control policies may not be acting on education outcomes through its
effect on drinking, but rather through its relation to within state
policy goals. Dee and Evans continue by applying a two sample
instrumental variables estimator to show that education attainment is
not affected by youthful drinking.
Mullahy and Sindelar (1994) used cross-sectional data from the New
Haven site of the National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiological Catchment Area survey and found that onset of alcoholism symptoms by age
22 reduced schooling by 5%. Unfortunately, if youths in New Haven are
not representative of youths in the rest of the country, their results
cannot be generalized. (4) More important, however, they are unable to
control for potential endogeneity between schooling and alcoholism
onset, and therefore we cannot be certain whether their results
represent an upper bound or a lower bound.
Yamada et al. (1996), who also used data from the NLSY to estimate
the effects of alcohol consumption on education, found that a 10%
increase in the frequency of drinking reduced the graduation probability
by 6.5%. Their model used high school completion for the class of 1981
as the dependent variable, which we do not do. Rather, our analysis
focuses on other schooling behaviors, of which direct graduation is just
one. Therefore, we are able to examine a much larger group of high
school students. In addition, although they do model substance demand,
which they assume affects graduation, they do not allow for the
possibility that the errors across the two equations could be correlated (endogeneity). For these reasons, it is uncertain if their results can
be generalized.
Recognizing that scant attention has been paid to potential
endogeneity issues surrounding schooling and alcohol consumption by
youths, Koch and Ribar (2001) examine the effect of different
endogeneity assumptions, and their respective biases in an effort to
provide bounds on the estimates. Their analysis used same-sex sibling
alcohol consumption onset as instruments for the individual's
drinking behavior. Their analysis found that a one-year increase in the
age of alcohol consumption onset did not measurably increase the number
of years of completed schooling. The small magnitude of their results
may be due to the fact that their instruments were not strong predictors
of drinking behavior. The small magnitude could also have been obtained
because they measured completed education at the age of 25, nearly 10
years later than the average age for alcohol onset, and if youthful
alcohol consumption only causes short-term problems, their estimates may
not encompass those short-term problems. (5) Endogeneity corrections
showed that any negative effects of drinking were understated. However,
even including the endogeneity corrections, the upper bound estimate was
an increase in schooling of less than one-half year for every one-year
increase in the age of alcohol onset.
Most recently, Wolaver (2002) studied the intensive margin relating
to youthful alcohol consumption and education. Using crosssectional data
from the 1993 Harvard College Alcohol Study, she finds that if college
students are often intoxicated or are frequent binge drinkers, they
attend classes less regularly, their average grade points are lower, and
they have a higher probability of majoring in business (compared to
engineering). Her results show that treating heavy alcohol use as
exogenous to the schooling decision is inappropriate, although light
alcohol consumption may be exogenous. Moreover, not treating the
endogeneity understates the adverse effect on college education caused
by heavy alcohol consumption.
We examine the effect of early alcohol consumption on educational
attainment, as measured by high school completion (via diploma and/or
GED). Importantly, the design of our analysis, where alcohol initiation
occurs before education decisions are made, eliminates the potential
problem of simultaneity; therefore, it is not reasonable to argue that
education decisions affect early alcohol consumption decisions. However,
it is expected, as has been shown in many of the preceding publications,
that there are underlying unobservable variables impacting schooling and
alcohol consumption decisions, especially peer interactions and the
structure of social relationships during the high school years. These
unobserved influences, if ignored in the analysis, could bias the
estimates. Our expectation is that these unobservable variables
understate the negative effects of alcohol consumption onset on
education, as many of the preceding studies suggest.
III. MODEL
Bivariate Probit
In this article, we examine an empirical model, which measures the
determinants of alcohol consumption onset and high school completion.
(6) We define [E.sup.*.sub.i] as individual i's propensity to
complete high school conditional on alcohol consumption onset,
[A.sub.i], as well as other demographic and sociological factors. We
assume in particular that [E.sup.*.sub.i] is a function of these
factors, such that:
(1) [E.sup.*.sub.i] = f([A.sub.i][[beta].sub.A] + [X.sub.i][theta])
+ [[epsilon].sub.i],
where [X.sub.1] is a 1 x k matrix of observed determinants, [theta]
is a k x 1 vector of associated coefficients, [A.sub.i] is alcohol
consumption onset preceding high school, [[beta].sub.A] is the
corresponding coefficient, and [[epsilon].sub.i] is unobserved
variation. The high school completion decision for individual i is
(2) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
The different definitions of completion are discussed in section
IV.
In the same fashion, we define [A.sup.*.sub.i] as the propensity to
begin drinking by the time high school begins for individual i. We
assume that [A.sup.*.sub.i] is a function of economic and sociological
factors, such that
(3) [A.sup.*.sub.i] = g([Z.sub.i][GAMMA]) + [v.sub.i],
where [Z.sub.i] denotes a 1 x m matrix of observed explanatory variables, with an m x 1 vector of corresponding coefficients, [GAMMA],
and [v.sub.i] represents unobserved variation. Alcohol consumption
initiation preceding high school is a dichotomous variable given by (7)
(4) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
The unobserved variations in equations (1) and (3) are assumed to
be joint normally distributed:
(5) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
Therefore, high school completion is specified as a probit with
alcohol consumption initiation preceding high school as a dummy endogenous determinant.
Maximum likelihood estimation of equations (1), (3), and the
distributional assumption (5) can be performed quite readily. However,
to estimate the likelihood function, a few identification issues must be
addressed. Unfortunately, the coefficient and error variance estimates
can only be identified up to their proportions; therefore, we only know
[[beta].sub.A]/[[sigma].sub.[epsilon]], [theta]/[[sigma].sub.[epsilon]],
and [GAMMA]/[[sigma].sub.v], (8) In addition to only knowing
proportions, the effect of alcohol consumption onset on high school
completion cannot be determined without appropriate exclusion
restrictions. (9) Appropriately excluded variables from [X.sub.i] should
be related to alcohol consumption onset but unrelated to high school
completion. In our analysis, we use three such variables: the log beer
tax rate that the individual faces at the age of 14, the minimum legal
drinking age faced by the individual at the age of 14, and whether the
individual knows close relatives with an alcohol problem.
If equations (1) and (3) are specified such that there is no
possibility for correlation between their associated errors, then
endogeneity bias is not a concern, and the estimated correlation
coefficient, [rho], will be zero. In that case, separate probit
estimation of the two equations, is appropriate. Although we are
primarily concerned with the estimate of [[beta].sub.A], we provide, as
a point of comparison, the results of the probit estimates alongside the
results of the bivariate probit estimates.
IV. DATA
The data for this analysis come from the 1979-96 panels of the NLSY
(Center for Human Resource Research, 1998). The NLSY is a national
sample of 12,686 individuals, who were 14-21 years old in 1979 and have
been reinterviewed annually since then. (10) The survey contains
detailed longitudinal behavioral information, including data on
schooling progress and alcohol consumption at different ages;
unfortunately, alcohol questions were not asked yearly. Personal, peer,
family background, and local area data (which allows us to match the
data with state-level information on taxes and legal drinking ages) are
also available in the NLSY.
We have created subsamples from the NLSY based on gender and
schooling achievement. The gender subsamples are only used to portray the differences between men and women in their education success and
their alcohol consumption onset. (11) The education subsamples are the
basis of the analysis within the article.
For determining the effect of youthful alcohol consumption on
normal high school progress and completion, we primarily focused on
dichotomous measures of high school completion versus noncompletion.
However, these dichotomous measures were created from three basic
schooling completion measures: (1) receipt of a high school diploma by
the age of 20; (2) receipt of a high school diploma after the age of 20
or a GED; and (3) no receipt of a recognized high school degree within
the sample period. (12) Given the age of the NLSY respondents, the
majority of high school completions in the later years of the NLSY
survey were through the GED, although the total number of GED recipients is small (slightly less than 10% of the women, and slightly less than
11% of the men in the sample have earned a GED).
Similar to Koch and Ribar (2001), the age of alcohol consumption
onset is constructed from the data. In 1982 and 1983, individuals were
asked about the age at which they began to drink regularly; a
dichotomous measure of drinking by the age of 14 could be constructed
from the 1982 and 1983 survey questions, because the entire sample was
at least 16 years of age by the time of the 1982 panel. (13) Because
individuals were not interviewed until they were 14 years old, the
typical age of eighth- and ninth-graders, and could have been as old as
22 years, a large component of the analysis is based on retrospective
data. (14) The age of 14 was chosen because it is the average age of a
ninth-grader. Therefore, the investigation can focus on whether
drinking, preceding high school, has any effect on the individual's
normal successful completion of high school.
Alcohol Consumption Onset by Age 14 and Schooling Completion
Differences
Table 1 illustrates the detrimental effect of early alcohol
consumption onset on high school completion. Although the remainder of
the analysis is based on a pooled sample of males and females, the
schooling and alcohol onset frequencies are presented by gender in Table
1. (15) The table highlights the differences between drinking and
schooling behavior between men and women. Although the majority of
women, 71.8%, and men, 64.7%, receive their high school diploma by the
age of 20, alcohol consumption onset preceding high school is associated
with a reduced probability of completion. For example, considering only
the youths who do not initiate alcohol consumption by the age of 14,
27.1% of these women do not receive a diploma by the age of 20; on the
other hand, for the women who initiated alcohol consumption by the age
of 14, over 39.3% of them fail to complete their diplomas by the age of
20. (16) For the men, the numbers are similarly sobering. Over 47.8% of
the men who consumed alcohol regularly by the age of 14 do not complete
their diplomas by the age of 20. On the other hand, only 32.1% of the
men who have not initiated alcohol consumption by the age of 14 do not
manage to complete their high school diplomas in a timely manner. (17)
The table also shows that women are more likely to complete a diploma by
the age of 20, whereas men are more likely to initiate alcohol
consumption by the age of 14. (18) Furthermore, the hypothesis that each
of the female and male subsamples has the same likelihood of drinking by
the age of 14 is rejected by a Kruskal-Wallis test. (19)
Summary Statistics of Data Used in the Analysis
Tables 2 and 3 contain summary statistics for the: family
background characteristics of the female high school subsample, family
background characteristics of the male subsample, and local area and
other characteristics for the pooled sample. The tables also contain
definitions of all of the variables used in the analysis and are further
disaggregated by our schooling completion measure.
The data reported in Table 2 lend some credence to Becker's
(1991) theory of the family: Children in larger families have less
success in school, possibly because limited parental resources are
spread more thinly over the children, the quality of those resources may
also be lower, mothers and fathers average less completed education,
older siblings were less likely to graduate from high school, and fewer
reading materials were available to the children. The average family of
a nongraduate was intact for a shorter period of time preceding the
child's high school education than a diploma recipient. In
addition, the diploma recipient, more often, was raised in an area with
a higher high school completion rate.
The descriptive data in Table 3 show that students who did not
receive a high school diploma by the age of 20 felt less control over
their lives (as measured by self-determination), had lower ASVAB verbal
scores, (20) and, most important, were less likely to initiate regular
alcohol consumption by the age of 14. On the other hand, students who
completed their diploma by age 20 lived in states with lower beer taxes
but were less aware of relatives with a drinking problem. However, there
is no obvious pattern across education status for the state minimum
legal drinking ages. The majority of the previously discussed
differences in means across education subsamples carry through the
analysis.
V. RESULTS
In this section we discuss the results from our specifications,
which we examined over the entire sample of male and female students.
(21) The results are presented according to the identification exclusion
restriction, if one was necessary. Therefore, in the first column, we
present probit estimates for high school completion and youthful alcohol
consumption onset; these probits were estimated based on the assumption
that [rho] = 0, that is, there is no correlation between the unobserved
error terms. In the second through last column, estimates of different
bivariate probits are provided; the estimates in each column are based
on different identification exclusion restrictions. Therefore, each of
the last column headings defines which variable is used as the variable
left out of the schooling equation for identification purposes. In
addition, each of these last columns includes an estimate of the
unobserved correlation between the high school completion equation and
the alcohol consumption onset equation. Appropriate test statistics,
standard errors, and significance notes are provided in each of the
columns of each of the tables. Finally, in an effort to use space
parsimoniously, only the main results are highlighted in the tables.
(22)
Early Alcohol Consumption Initiation and High School Diploma
Recipients
Table 4 displays results from our estimates of high school
completion, defined as receiving a high school diploma by the age of 20
(rather than not graduating, receiving a GED, or receiving a diploma
later in life), and early alcohol consumption onset, defined as starting
by the age of 14. In other words, for the entire sample, (23)
(6) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
From the table, it is clear, as the discussion of the data in
section IV suggests, that the probability of receiving a diploma
declines significantly if the individual consumed alcohol on a regular
basis before the age of 14. In addition, if we ignore unobservable but
endogenous factors, the estimated impact is biased and understated; the
mean of the calculated dummy endogenous marginal effects is ~23%,
whereas the probit marginal effect estimate is only 10%. Importantly,
the choice of exclusion restriction does not greatly alter the estimated
impact of early alcohol initiation on the probability of receiving a
high school diploma. Although youthful alcohol consumption onset reduces
the probability of a diploma, the results show that males are less
negatively affected; the interaction between male and alcohol
consumption onset yields a marginally positive and significant
coefficient.
The most interesting feature of the results is the fact that the
estimated effect of unobserved determinants is positive. For both males
and females the [[chi square].sub.1] statistic rejects the null
hypothesis that the errors across the two equations are uncorrelated in
all cases. As predicted by our previous discussion, the estimated
unobserved correlation is positive, which means that unobserved factors,
which increase (decrease) a youth's propensity to embark on an
alcohol consumption path by the age of 14, also increase (decrease) the
individual's propensity to receive a diploma by the age of 20.
Given the difficulty in controlling for peer and other social effects
with the data available, the estimated [rho] > 0 is not surprising.
Alcohol consumption is a social activity, and school is a highly social
environment. The underpinning social nature of these two activities
suggests an inherent complementarity between the two activities. (24)
Otherwise, most of the results are to be expected. We find that the
decision to initiate alcohol consumption onset by the age of 14 is
decreasing in the state policy variables, like the minimum legal
drinking age in place at the time, as well as the beer tax rate at the
time. (25) In addition, we find that having knowledge of family members
with a drinking problem is highly correlated with youthful alcohol
consumption onset.
Although the rest of the results are not also reported, it is worth
noting that the remaining variables reported in the data summary tables
generally have the expected effect. Family intactness, school ability
(ASVAB), religiosity, being nonwhite, and having an older sibling who
has successfully completed high school, reduce the probability of
youthful onset and raise the probability of schooling completion.
Parental education and having access to various forms of reading
material in the household raises the probability of own schooling
success. In addition, Catholic youths with older siblings are more
likely to have consumed alcohol on a regular basis by the age of 14 than
otherwise similar youths. Finally, we find that children in larger
families are less likely to successfully complete high school.
Note that if our choice of excluded variables are themselves
endogenous, then the presented results are also biased. However, Dee and
Evans (1999) suggest that the state determinants, not just drinking
laws, are related to drinking; we also show that drinking laws are
related to drinking behavior. However, we also show that knowledge of
family alcohol problems leads to broadly similar estimated results. It
is highly unlikely that these two different types of determinants are
endogenous in the same way; therefore, we do not believe that the
concern raised by Dee and Evans is a problem here. Furthermore, it is
not clear that knowledge of family members with a drinking problem is
endogenous to the decision to initiate alcohol consumption onset, given
the fact that those family members are likely to have made their own
decisions about alcohol consumption. (26)
Association between Early Alcohol Consumption Onset and Diploma
Recipients by Age 20 Compared to GED and Later-Life Diploma Recipients
Due to differences between individuals who receive a diploma on
time and those who receive either a GED or a later life diploma, we
examine, more carefully, the differences between these two subgroups.
The results from the analysis are reported in Table 5. This analysis is
only performed on the sample of students who complete a recognizable
high school degree. (27) In other words, our education completion
variable is:
(7) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
The results presented in Table 5 are qualitatively similar to those
presented in Table 4. For example, the unobserved correlation is
positive and significant in all cases, with the exception of the state
beer tax exclusion estimate, where it is positive and insignificant. The
exclusion restrictions also produce similar estimates as reported in the
previous subsection. The primary difference between this analysis and
the one reported in the previous subsection is that the difference
between the probit marginal effect of alcohol consumption onset and the
bivariate probit marginal effect is now much smaller, despite the
positive and significant correlation between the unobserved determinants
of alcohol consumption onset and schooling completion. The estimated
probit marginal effect is a 9.5% reduction in the probability of
completing school, whereas the means of the estimated bivariate probit
marginal effects varies from an 11.2% reduction to a 13.7% reduction in
the probability of schooling completion. (28) According to our analysis,
if it were not for social factors acting positively on the young men and
women, there would be more schooling problems than are observed.
Although other results are not reported here, it is worth noting
that many of the same variables affect schooling completion and alcohol
onset the same way as mentioned in the preceding section. Generally, the
size of the parameter estimates is much lower here than in the preceding
set of estimates. An important difference (not included in the table) is
in the statistical implication that the size of the family, parental
education, and availability of reading material do not greatly improve
the likelihood that a youth receives a high school diploma by the age of
20 over the receipt of a GED or a diploma later in life. In other words,
family characteristics play an important role in pushing the youth to
conclude their education in some way or another, but not necessarily the
type of degree completed.
Early Alcohol Consumption Onset and GED or Later-Life Diploma
Attainment Compared to Nongraduates
Our last set of results is presented in Table 6. This table
displays estimates of high school success for those who initially quit
school, some of whom later returned. Therefore, high school success is
described as receiving a GED (or, in rare instances, returning for a
high school diploma) after the age of 20. The effect of alcohol
consumption onset on educational success remains the focus. Hence, for
the sample of nondiploma (by age 20) earning individuals, (29)
(8) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]
Because the results in this subsection are concerned with a smaller
sample of individuals, those who do not receive a diploma by the age of
20, the results suggest whether there are any differences in early life
circumstances between those dropouts who eventually return and those who
do not. Importantly, because return may occur at much later ages (i.e.,
late enough for many early life circumstances to be forgotten) for most
of the sample, it is surprising that the results do pinpoint the
importance of alcohol consumption onset still. However, an additional
problem is noticed in the tables: Generally, the excluded variables are
not strong and significant predictors of alcohol consumption onset. For
that reason, we do not place unshakable faith in the estimated marginal
effects from the biprobit analysis. In the first column, where the
probit estimates are presented, the estimated effect of youth alcohol
consumption onset is negligible, whereas in all of the bivariate cases,
the mean estimated marginal effect approaches a 50% reduction in the
probability of returning to school. Furthermore, the estimated
correlation between unobserved factors affecting schooling and alcohol
onset decisions approaches unity. In other words, there are too many
unobserved factors in each of the two estimated equations; the result is
the identification problems alluded to in the notes.
The difficulty we had in estimating this set of equations is,
however, not all that surprising. As we found initially, alcohol
consumption onset and family background characteristics play an
important role in an individual's schooling decisions. However,
although schooling is an investment decision, we would expect that youth
variables will have been mostly forgotten when individuals decide
whether to return to school. Alcohol consumption preceding high school
is contemporaneous and relevant, primarily because we examined high
school decisions. Further analysis into education and alcohol
consumption, especially consideration of when and if people choose to
return to school will require more contemporaneous alcohol consumption
variables.
VI. CONCLUSION
In this article, we have reported results from the examination of
the determinants of alcohol consumption onset by the age of 14 and
different measures of high school completion, where we controlled for
the endogeneity of schooling and alcohol decisions with alcohol policy
variables as well as family controls. The general conclusion to be drawn
from the analysis is that alcohol consumption onset greatly reduces the
probability of high school success, especially if high school success is
meant to include the receipt of a high school diploma by the age of 20.
The primary concern when estimating the effect of alcohol consumption on
education is the ability to control for the simultaneity of the two
decisions. Research by Cook and Moore (1993), Yamada et al. (1996), and
Moore and Cook (1995) have found significant negative effects of alcohol
and drug use on education, whereas Dee and Evans (1999) and Koch and
Ribar (2001) have found negative but insignificant or negative and
significant (but small) effects of alcohol use on education. In contrast
to the preceding publications, we have found negative significant and
large effects of alcohol consumption onset on education. Our results are
due to the fact that we look at alcohol consumption decisions that
predate the education decisions and the fact that we employ an empirical
technique that allows for the estimation of the unobserved correlation
between the two endogenously determined variables. Most previous studies
have not been able to examine the importance of the social nature of
schooling and drinking. Our research suggests that further research into
peer interactions and their effects on human capital decisions will be
important.
Like many previous researchers, we find that alcohol tax policies
affect alcohol use, and, therefore, tax policies can influence education
success for high school youths. On the other hand, like Dee and Evans
(1999), we do not find that alcohol control policies, measured by the
legal minimum purchase age law, affect alcohol consumption onset. This
finding results despite the fact that we included a family control for
older siblings, who we find to be contributing to the alcohol
consumption of their younger siblings.
In addition, we also find that individuals from families with a
history of alcohol problems are more likely to consume alcohol on a
regular basis at an early age. Although we did not have data, the
implication of the research is that alcohol treatment programs may have
a greater benefit than alcohol control or tax policies. The importance
of social factors in counterbalancing the negative effects of alcohol
consumption (although those same factors may also contribute to alcohol
consumption in practice) need to be further recognized. Appropriate
school-level programs that encourage participation in social activities
could increase the schooling success of many youths, especially because
these social activities will not promote alcohol consumption.
The results reported here suggest an interesting research agenda,
one that focuses much more carefully on duration analysis and the
hazards associated with difficulties resulting from adolescent drinking.
Our results suggest that alcohol consumption initiation at an early age
will increase the hazard of dropout, and, therefore, it may increase the
duration of school absence. (30) Our results further highlight the
importance of modeling events that are proximate in time; hazard
analysis could do this. Finally, due to the nature of social
interactions, it might be possible to examine changes in peer structures
and its effect on both alcohol consumption and schooling decisions.
Until more is understood concerning absence from school, return to
school, peer interactions, and job market behavior for youthful
consumers of alcohol, there remain many interesting questions to be
examined in this area of the literature.
ABBREVIATIONS
GED: Graduate Equivalency Degree
NLSY: National Longitudinal Study of Youth
REFERENCES
Becker, G. S. "A Theory of the Allocation of Time."
Economic Journal, 75, 1965, 493-517.
--. A Treatise on the Family, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1991.
Benham, L., and A. Benham. "Employment, Earnings and
Psychiatric Diagnosis," in Economic Aspects of Health, edited by V.
Fuchs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1982, 203-20.
Burgess, S. M., and C. Propper. "Early Health Related
Behaviours and Their Impact on Later Life Chances: Evidence from the
U.S." Health Economics, 7, 1998, 381-99.
Center for Human Resource Research, 1998, http://www.
chrr.ohio-state.edu/surveys.html (accessed July 1999).
Cook, P., and M. J. Moore. "Drinking and Schooling."
Journal of Health Economics, 12(4), 1993, 411-29.
Deas, D., P. Riggs, J. Langenbucher, M. Goldman, and S. Brown.
"Adolescents Are Not Adults: Development Considerations in Alcohol
Users." Alcoholism." Clinical and Experimental Research,
24(2), 2000, 232-37.
Dee, T. S., and W. N. Evans. "State Alcohol Policies, Teen
Drinking and Traffic Fatalities," Journal of Public Economics,
72(2), 1999, 289-315.
Figlio, D. N. "The Effect of Drinking Age Laws and
Alcohol-Related Crashes: Time-Series Evidence from Wisconsin."
Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 14(4), 1995, 555-66.
Grant, B. F., and D. A. Dawson. "Age at Onset of Alcohol Use
and Its Association with DSM-IV Alcohol Abuse and Dependence: Results
from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiological Survey."
Journal of Substance Abuse, 9, 1997, 103-10.
Grossman, M. "On the Concept of Health Capital and the Demand
for Health." Journal of Political Economy, 80(2), 1972, 223-55.
Harwood, H. J., A. Cruze, P. Kristiansen, J. Collins, and D. Jones.
"Economic Costs to Society of Alcohol and Drug Abuse and Mental
Illness: 1980. " Technical Report, Research Triangle Institute,
1984.
Koch, S. F., and K. A. McGeary. "The Hazard of Alcohol and
Other Drug Consumption on School Leaving and Return." Working
Paper, University of Pretoria, 2003.
Koch, S. F., and D. C. Ribar. "A Siblings Analysis of the
Effects of Alcohol Consumption Onset on Educational Attainment."
Contemporary Economic Policy, 19(2), 2001, 162-74.
Moore, M. J., and P. J. Cook. "Habit and Heterogeneity in the
Youthful Demand for Alcohol." NBER Working Paper No. 5152, 1995.
Mullahy, J., and J. L. Sindelar. "Alcoholism, Work and
Income." Journal of Labor Economics', 11(3), 1993, 494-520.
Mullahy, J., and J. L. Sindelar. "Alcoholism and Income: The
Role of Indirect Effects." Milbank Quarterly, 72(2), 1994, 359-75.
Ribar, D. C. "Teenage Fertility and High School
Completion." Review of Economics and Statistics, 76(3), 1994,
413-24.
Rice, D. P. "The Economic Cost of Alcohol Abuse and Alcohol
Dependence: 1990." Alcohol Health and Research World, 17(1), 1993,
10-12.
Rice, D. P., S. Kelman, L. S. Miller, and S. Dunmeyer. "The
Economic Cost of Alcohol and Drug Abuse and Mental Illness: 1985.
"Technical Report, Office of Financing and Coverage Policy.
Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, U.S. Department
of Health and Human Services, University of California-San Francisco,
San Francisco, CA.
Weschler, H., J. E. Lee, M. Kuo, and H. Lee. "College Binge
Drinking in the Late 1990s: A Continuing Problem." Technical
Report, College Studies Report, 2000.
Wilson, N., V. Battistich, S. L. Syme, and W. T. Boyce. "Does
Elementary School Alcohol, Tobacco, and Marijuana Use Increase Middle
School Risk?" Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 2002, 442-47.
Wolaver, A. M. "Effects of Heavy Drinking in College on Study
Effort, Grade Point Average, and Major Choice." Contemporary
Economic Policy, 20(4), 2002, 415-28.
Yamada, T., M. Kendix, and T. Yamada. "The Impact of Alcohol
Consumption and Marijuana Use on High School Graduation." Health
Economics, 5(1), 1996, 77-92.
(1.) Dropping out of school may allow more time for income-earning
activities, and thus increase the potential alcohol consumption set.
(2.) Importantly, even under extreme assumptions concerning
rationality, it is unlikely that high school continuation decisions
(during high school, for example) will influence the decision to drink
preceding high school.
(3.) A 95% confidence interval around their estimates suggests that
alcohol consumption could reduce postsecondary education anywhere from
not at all to up to four years.
(4.) Although we do not directly address these questions, because
we do not have exact onset of alcoholism symptoms in the NLSY, the raw
correlation between alcohol onset before the age of 14 (which means
consumption onset by 1979) and alcoholism (which is measured with the
1988 wave of the NLSY) is 0.09 and is significant at the 0.01% level.
(5.) Burgess and Propper (1998), in related work, find an increase
in earnings related to early alcohol experiences, that is, those
beginning before the age of 18, suggesting that early alcohol
experiences are not likely to have lingering negative effects.
(6.) The model, more commonly referred to as a bivariate probit,
has been used to examine a number of different issues in economics. Our
model is derived in part from the discussion in Ribar (1994).
(7.) Although it is rarely acceptable to use less information than
more (as we do here with the dichotomous measure of alcohol onset rather
than a continuous measure, which could be incorporated into the
analysis), using the dichotomous measure of alcohol consumption removes
one possible direction of causality. It is not plausible that an
individual's later schooling decisions will determine current
drinking behavior. However, using the continuous analog of drinking
would make it difficult to determine whether drinking was caused by poor
schooling performance or vice versa.
(8.) We apply the standard normalization, [[sigma].sub.[epsilon]] =
[[sigma].sub.v] = 1, for ease of interpretation.
(9.) We also apply multiple restrictions and test for
overidentification.
(10.) The NLSY contains weights, not used here, which make the
estimates more nationally representative. The survey oversampled blacks,
Hispanics, and underprivileged whites.
(11.) The total sample of females was reduced to 4,749 and the
total sample of males was reduced to 4,401 after observations without
complete information were removed.
(12.) Each year, the NLSY asked respondents whether they had
completed high school within the past year. If they had completed high
school, they were further asked whether they had received a diploma or a
GED.
(13.) There were some disagreements between the 1982 and 1983
responses. In the case of disagreement, we chose the younger response in
each of those years. As an extreme correction, we also dropped all
responses with a disagreement; the estimates were not significantly
different, so we stayed with the younger response.
(14.) One concern from using retrospective data is that recall may
be better for individuals closer to the age they are recalling. Results
of cohort analyses show unimodal distributions centered at age 18 and
mass points at 0; the mass point at 0 measures the extent of nondrinkers
in the cohort. As expected, older cohorts had smaller mass points at 0,
as older individuals have had more opportunity to begin consuming
alcohol. Similarly, younger cohorts were more likely to report more
youthful alcohol consumption onset. However, it is not apparent that the
cohort effects are inherently due to recall error or differences in the
age distributions; either interpretation could be valid.
(15.) After very careful analysis, it was determined that although
males and females differed in their drinking and schooling success, they
did not significantly differ in their responses to the exclusion
restrictions used for identification of the model. A dummy variable to
control for gender was Incorporated in the analysis to extract the
differences in behavior between men and women. We thank a reviewer for
bringing this feature to our attention.
(16.) There were 588 young females who did not receive any high
school degree, there were 585 young females who received either a GED or
a diploma at an age later than 20; that makes 1,173 out of 4,321 (or
27.1%) young females who did not consume alcohol by the age of 14 and
did not receive a diploma by the age of 20. Similarly, there were 168
out of 428 (39.3%) young females who did initiate alcohol consumption by
the age of 14 and did not receive their diplomas by the age of 20.
(17.) For the male sample, there were 593 nondegree recipient
nondrinkers and 569 GED and later-life diploma recipient nondrinkers. In
other words, 1,162 out of the 3,587 young males who did not consume
alcohol regularly by age 14 also did not receive a diploma by the age of
20. On the other hand, 390 out of the 814 young men who did consume
alcohol regularly by the age of 14 also did not complete their high
school diplomas by the age of 20.
(18.) Kruskal-Wallis tests of similarities across samples reject
that men and women drink by the same age ([[chi square].sub.1] = 61.62)
and graduate at the same rates ([[chi square].sub.1] = 33.83).
(19.) For this test, the calculated test statistic for females is
[[chi square].sub.1] = 6.96. For males, the calculated test statistic is
[[chi square.sub.1] = 32.49.
(20.) The data also contains scores on the 10 ASVAB sections.
Verbal scores were chosen due to the statistical fact that they were
highly correlated with all other ASVAB section scores; the lowest
correlation coefficient was 0.60, and the highest was 0.98. Analyses
were run with other ASVAB section scores as the control for ability, but
no obvious differences were noticed.
(21.) A large number of different specifications were examined to
determine the validity of a pooled analysis over an analysis separated
by gender. Log-likelihood ratio tests generally confirmed the results
posted in Table 1: Young men and women have different drinking and
schooling behaviors. However, log-likelihood tests also showed that men
and women did not respond differently to the identification exclusion
restrictions. For that reason, only the pooled results are reported in
the paper. The separated sample results are available on request.
(22.) The remaining results are available on request.
(23.) We also estimated the relationship dropping all GED
recipients. The results were qualitatively the same, although
quantitatively stronger, so we report only the full sample results.
(24.) Recall that we are not saying that drinking at an early age
leads to an increased probability of completing school; rather, we are
saying that the negative impact of drinking at an early age is
understated, due to the complementary social nature of schooling and
drinking. As an extreme example, if it were not for the ability to
socialize with contemporaries, it may be the case that more students
would dropout of school than currently do.
(25.) One referee suggested that the effect of the minimum legal
drinking age might be nonlinear. We investigated that possibility by
allowing for dummy variables at different age categories. The effect of
the different definitions was insignificant in all cases, and, for that
reason, we do not report those results here.
(26.) It is possible that the only reason these people know they
have relatives with a drinking problem is that they have been told by
other family members that they may turn out to be just like so-and-so.
In that sense, the knowledge is gained later and cannot be considered as
a determinant to their actual decision.
(27.) We did also analyze other subsamples; for example, we
compared diploma recipients by age 20 with those who had not completed
their diploma by the age of 20. The results presented here only pertain
to the first analysis, because the subsampling did not greatly affect
the conclusions.
(28.) The mean for the beer tax exclusion is only a 7.5% reduction;
however, the need for the bivariate probit in that case is rejected.
(29.) Other subsamples were considered. For example, we looked only
at individuals who had earned a GED compared to those who did not
complete anything. In all of these cases, identification was an issue
due to relatively small sample sizes, and, more important, due to
minimal variations in the explanatory variables; for that reason, we
focus on the sample of nondiploma earners, under which scenario we were
able to obtain estimates, although those estimates are not ideal.
(30.) In related work, Koch and McGeary (2003) are investigating
dropout hazards. Preliminary results suggest that this hazard increases
with the number of years an individual has been drinking preceding the
year of school in question. However, their analysis has not yet included
absence duration.
STEVEN F. KOCH and KERRY ANNE MCGEARY *
* We thank two anonymous referees, Paul E. Jensen, seminar
participants at the University of Pretoria and Drexel University, as
well as participants at the International Atlantic Economic Association
Meetings in Charleston and Athens, participants at the Western Economic
Association Meetings, and participants in the African Econometric
Society Modelling Conference in Kruger Park, South Africa for their
helpful comments.
Koch: Associate Professor, Department of Economics, University of
Pretoria, Pretoria 0002, Republic of South Africa. Phone 27-12-420-3468,
E-mail steve.
[email protected]
McGeary: Assistant Professor, Department of Economics and
International Business, Drexel University, Suite 503-D Matheson Hall,
32nd and Market Streets, Philadelphia, PA, 19104. Phone 1-215-895-6972,
Fax 1-215-895-6975, E-mail
[email protected]
TABLE 1
Frequency of Early Alcohol Consumption Onset and
High School Completion Measures for the Female
and Male Subsamples
Has Not HS Diploma
Received a after Age 20
HS Degree or a GED
Females
Alcohol onset after 14 588 585
% of total sample 12.38 12.32
Alcohol onset by 14 82 86
% of total sample 1.73 1.81
Cumulative schooling (b) 670 671
% of total sample 14.11 14.13
Males
Alcohol onset after 14 593 569
% of total sample 13.47 12.93
Alcohol onset by 14 213 177
% of total sample 4.84 4.02
Cumulative schooling (b) 806 746
% of total sample 18.31 16.95
High School Cumulative
Diploma Alcohol
by Age 20 Onset (a)
Females
Alcohol onset after 14 3148 4321
% of total sample 66.29 90.99
Alcohol onset by 14 260 428
% of total sample 5.47 9.01
Cumulative schooling (b) 3408 4749
% of total sample 71.76 100
Males
Alcohol onset after 14 2425 3587
% of total sample 55.10 81.50
Alcohol onset by 14 424 814
% of total sample 9.63 18.50
Cumulative schooling (b) 2849 4401
% of total sample 64.74 100
(a) Aggregation over each schooling measure leads to
total number of youths.
(b) Aggregation over each alcohol measure leads to total
number of youths who have completed a particular level
of schooling.
Source: Authors' calculations from NLSY 1979-96.
TABLE 2
Sample Statistics of Family Variables Used in the Analysis by
Measure of School Completion
No GED or
Diploma
Any Age
Variable N = 1,476
Mean SD
Male = 1 if male 0.546 0.498
Nonwhite = 1 if not white 0.486 0.500
% FamInt = proportion to age 14 boi family 0.721 0.399
was together
% FamIntMiss = 1 if % FamInt not available 0.014 0.018
Siblings = number of siblings 4.752 2.874
NotOldest = 1 if not oldest child 0.741 0.438
OlderSibHS = 1 if older sibling completed 0.380 0.486
high school
MomEd = mom years of education 8.332 4.162
MomEdMiss = 1 if mom education not available 0.108 0.311
DadEd = dad years of education 6.909 5.046
DadEdMiss = 1 if not available 0.226 0.419
Reading Available = 3 if magazine library 1.484 1.032
card and newspapers; 2 if only 2 of 3; 1
if 1 of 3; 0 if none
Raised Catholic = 1 if Catholic 0.301 0.459
Religious = 1 if attended church at least 0.344 0.475
twice per month
Urban = 1 if lived in city at age 14 0.793 0.406
%HSDip = % of local population with diploma 48.231 12.067
Diploma
after Age
20 or a GED
Variable N = 1,417
Mean SD
Male = 1 if male 0.526 0.499
Nonwhite = 1 if not white 0.519 0.500
% FamInt = proportion to age 14 boi family 0.716 0.398
was together
% FamIntMiss = 1 if % FamInt not available 0.014 0.118
Siblings = number of siblings 4.339 2.873
NotOldest = 1 if not oldest child 0.702 0.457
OlderSibHS = 1 if older sibling completed 0.440 0.497
high school
MomEd = mom years of education 9.215 4.047
MomEdMiss = 1 if mom education not available 0.077 0.267
DadEd = dad years of education 7.927 5.154
DadEdMiss = 1 if not available 0.197 0.398
Reading Available = 3 if magazine library 1.768 1.039
card and newspapers; 2 if only 2 of 3; 1
if 1 of 3; 0 if none
Raised Catholic = 1 if Catholic 0.330 0.470
Religious = 1 if attended church at least 0.368 0.483
twice per month
Urban = 1 if lived in city at age 14 0.819 0.385
%HSDip = % of local population with diploma 49.379 11.440
Diploma
Received
by Age 20
Variable N = 6,257
Mean SD
Male = 1 if male 0.455 0.498
Nonwhite = 1 if not white 0.375 0.484
% FamInt = proportion to age 14 boi family 0.838 0.331
was together
% FamIntMiss = 1 if % FamInt not available 0.009 0.094
Siblings = number of siblings 3.489 2.410
NotOldest = 1 if not oldest child 0.671 0.470
OlderSibHS = 1 if older sibling completed 0.570 0.495
high school
MomEd = mom years of education 11.047 3.600
MomEdMiss = 1 if mom education not available 0.038 0.192
DadEd = dad years of education 10.458 5.024
DadEdMiss = 1 if not available 0.105 0.307
Reading Available = 3 if magazine library 2.206 0.929
card and newspapers; 2 if only 2 of 3; 1
if 1 of 3; 0 if none
Raised Catholic = 1 if Catholic 0.335 0.472
Religious = 1 if attended church at least 0.500 0.500
twice per month
Urban = 1 if lived in city at age 14 0.780 0.414
%HSDip = % of local population with diploma 51.319 11.110
Source: Authors' calculation from NLSY 1979-96.
TABLE 3
Sample Statistics of Personal and Location Variables Used
in the Analysis by Measure of School Completion
No GED or
Diploma
by Any Age
Variable N = 1,476
Mean SD
Onset at 14 = 1 1 if initiate by 14 0.200 0.400
One Relative = 1 if knows one relative 0.512 0.500
with drinking problem
Two relatives = 1 if knows two relatives 0.254 0.435
with drinking problem
MLDA14 = Minimum legal age for purchase 19.038 1.367
when aged 14
LNtax l4 = state beer tax when aged 14 -0.766 1.036
AsvabVerb = Score on ASVAB verbal section 36.426 10.081
RottInd = index summed over 5 questions 10.583 2.258
measuring own perception of control
over own life
Diploma
after Age
20 or a GED
Variable N = 1,417
Mean SD
Onset at 14 = 1 1 if initiate by 14 0.186 0.389
One Relative = 1 if knows one relative 0.534 0.499
with drinking problem
Two relatives = 1 if knows two relatives 0.279 0.449
with drinking problem
MLDA14 = Minimum legal age for purchase 18.946 1.317
when aged 14
LNtax l4 = state beer tax when aged 14 -0.787 1.003
AsvabVerb = Score on ASVAB verbal section 41.934 10.483
RottInd = index summed over 5 questions 10.805 2.406
measuring own perception of control
over own life
Diploma
Received
by Age 20
Variable N = 6,257
Mean SD
Onset at 14 = 1 1 if initiate by 14 0.109 0.312
One Relative = 1 if knows one relative 0.494 0.500
with drinking problem
Two relatives = 1 if knows two relatives 0.237 0.425
with drinking problem
MLDA14 = Minimum legal age for purchase 18.995 1.339
when aged 14
LNtax l4 = state beer tax when aged 14 -0.816 0.955
AsvabVerb = Score on ASVAB verbal section 49.472 9.412
RottInd = index summed over 5 questions 11.533 2.382
measuring own perception of control
over own life
Source: Authors' calculation from NLSY 1979-96.
TABLE 4
Estimates of Early Alcohol Consumption (by Age 14) on
High School Diploma Receipt by Age 20
Bivariate
Probit
Specification
Exclusion
Restriction
for Onset by
Age 14
Sample N = 9150 Probit Estimate MLDA14
Effect on education by -0.3654 (a) -1.4064 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.0715) (0.1934)
Marginal effect -0.1301 (f) -0.2273 (f)
(0.0268) (0.1289)
Male 0.4887 (b) 0.7457 (a)
(0.2086) (0.2093)
Male x Education 0.0269 0.1471 (f)
Effect of onset (0.0897) (0.0870)
Effect of MLDA14 -0.018 -0.0129
On Onset by 14 (0.0134) (0.0124)
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0659 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0211)
Effect of one relative 0.1636
On Onset by 14 (0.0418)
Effect of Two relatives 0.1832
On Onset by 14 (0.0456)
Male 1.4417 (a) 1.4831 (a)
(0.2538) (0.2134)
[rho] 0 (h) 0.5320 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 21.634
Log likelihood -7,805.5 (e) -7,839.49
Bivariate Probit Specification
Exclusion Restriction for
Onset by Age 14
Sample N = 9150 LNtax14 One Rel
Effect on education by -1.3597 (a) -1.4108 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.2098) (0.1666)
Marginal effect -0.2197 (f) -0.2295 (f)
(0.1227) (0.1310)
Male 0.7319 (a) 0.7459 (a)
(0.2109) (0.2071)
Male x Education 0.1397 (f) 0.1464 (f)
Effect of onset (0.0882) (0.0858)
Effect of MLDA14
On Onset by 14
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0530 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0198)
Effect of one relative 0.2617 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0336)
Effect of Two relatives
On Onset by 14
Male 1.4921 (a) 1.4854 (a)
(0.2625) (0.2630)
[rho] 0.5093 (a) 0.5379 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 17.226 30.61
Log likelihood -7,836.42 -7,809.32
Bivariate Probit Specification
Exclusion Restriction for
Onset by Age 14
Sample N = 9150 Two Rels All Four
Effect on education by -1.4017 (a) -1.4017 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.1671) (0.1623)
Marginal effect -0.2299 (f) -0.02292 (f)
(0.1314) (0.1309)
Male 0.7474 (f) 0.7441 (a)
(0.2074) (0.2071)
Male x Education 0.1467 (f) 0.1454 (f)
Effect of onset (0.0858) (0.0857)
Effect of MLDA14 -0.0219 (c)
On Onset by 14 (0.0130)
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0624 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0207)
Effect of one relative 0.1686 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0405)
Effect of Two relatives 0.2943 (a) 0.1876 (a)
On Onset by 14 (0.0365) (0.0439)
Male 1.4848 (a) 1.5090 (a)
(0.2618) (0.2641)
[rho] 0.5394 (a) 0.5371 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 30.373 32.411
Log likelihood -7,808.8 -7,795.23
Notes: SEs are in parentheses. Other variables used in regression
and not reported in the table: Nonwhite, %FamInt, %FamIntMiss,
Siblings, NotOldest, OlderSibHS, MomEd, MomEdMiss, DadEd, DadEdMiss,
Reading Available, Raised Catholic, Religious, Urban, %HSDip, Asvab
Verb, and Rottlnd.
(a) Significant at 1%.
(b) Significant at 5%.
(c) Significant at 10%.
(d) Significant at 15%.
(e) Sum of log-likelihoods from two separate probit estimates,
one of school completion, the other from alcohol onset.
(f) Marginal probability averaged over the entire sample.
(g) SD based on sample average marginal probability.
(h) Estimation restriction sets [rho] = 0; identical to
estimating separate probits.
Source: NLSY 1979-96.
TABLE 5
Estimates of Early Alcohol Consumption (by Age 14) on
High School Completion (Diploma by Age 20 versus GED
or Later-Life Diploma)
Probit
Sample N = 7,646 Estimate MLDA 14
Effect on Education by -0.3608 (a) -0.97286 (b)
Onset at age 14 (-0.0821) (0.3902)
Marginal effect -0.0953 (a) -0.1124 (f)
(0.0242) (0.0661)
Male 0.3614 (c) 0.47666 (b)
(0.2126) (0.2221)
Male x Education Effect 0.0327 0.0999
of onset (0.1045) (0.1108)
Effect of MLDA14 on -0.018 -0.0125
Onset by 14 (0.0149) (0.0148)
Effect of LNtax14 on -0.0741 (a)
Onset by 14 (0.0234)
Effect of one relative 0.164 (a)
on Onset by 14 (0.0467)
Effect of two relatives 0.2119 (a)
on Onset by 14 (0.0506)
Male 1.2757 (a) 1.2439 (a)
(0.2701) (0.2687)
[rho] 0 (h) 0.3039 (d)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 2.365
Log likelihood -5,838.76 (e) -5,875.93
Bivariate Probit
Specification Exclusion
Restrictions for
Onset by 14
Sample N = 7,646 LNTax14 One Rel.
Effect on Education by -0.7397 (a) -1.1195 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.4574) (0.2399)
Marginal effect -0.0747 (f) -0.01346 (f)
(0.0405) (0.0837)
Male 0.4319 (c) 0.4995 (b)
(0.2278) (0.2126)
Male x Education Effect 0.0722 0.1171
of onset (0.1146) (0.1039)
Effect of MLDA14 on
Onset by 14
Effect of LNtax14 on -0.0650 (a)
Onset by 14 (0.0230)
Effect of one relative 0.2803 (a)
on Onset by 14 (0.0380)
Effect of two relatives
on Onset by 14
Male 1.2565 (a) 1.2195 (a)
(0.2711) (0.2680)
[rho] 0.1900 0.3790 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 0.696 9.783
Log likelihood -5,872.29 -5,849.36
Bivariate Probit
Specification Exclusion
Restrictions for
Onset by 14
Sample N = 7,646 Two Rels. All Four
Effect on Education by -1.1352 (a) -1.1172 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.2423) (0.2287)
Marginal effect -0.1369 (f) -0.135 (f)
(0.0856) (0.0840)
Male 0.505 (b) 0.4978 (b)
(0.2132) (0.2126)
Male x Education Effect 0.1182 0.1162
of onset (0.1035) (0.1034)
Effect of MLDA14 on -0.0224 (d)
Onset by 14 (0.0149)
Effect of LNtax14 on -0.0695 (a)
Onset by 14 (0.0236)
Effect of one relative 0.1704 (a)
on Onset by 14 (0.0461)
Effect of two relatives 0.3256 (a) 0.2179 (a)
on Onset by 14 (0.0413) (0.0498)
Male 1.2286 (a) 1.2326 (a)
(0.2667) (0.2687)
[rho] 0.3870 (a) 0.3799 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 9.783 10.798
Log likelihood -5,846.77 -5,835.23
Notes: SEs are in parentheses. Other variables used in regression
and not reported in the table: Nonwhite, %FamInt, %FamIntMiss,
Siblings, NotOldest, OlderSibHS, MomEd, MomEdMiss, DadEd,
DadEdMiss, Reading Available, Raised Catholic, Religious,
Urban, %HSDip, Asvab Verb, and RottInd.
(a) Significant at 1%.
(b) Significant at 5%.
(c) Significant at 10%.
(d) Significant at 15%.
(e) Sum of log likelihoods from two separate probit estimates,
one of school completion, the other from alcohol onset.
(f) Marginal probability averaged over the entire sample.
(g) SE based on sample average marginal probability.
(h) Estimation restriction sets [rho] = 0; identical to estimating
separate probits.
Source: NLSY 1979-96.
TABLE 6
Estimates of Early Alcohol Consumption (by Age 14) on
GED or Later-Life Diplomas Compared to Noncompletion
Bivariate
Probit
Specification
Exclusion
Restriction
for Onset by 14
Probit
Sample N = 2,893 Estimate MLDA14
Effect on education by -0.0045 -1.5789 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.1072) (0.1317)
Marginal effect -0.0018 -0.4824 (f)
(0.0428) (0.1462)
Male 0.2417 0.4721 (b)
(0.3134) (0.2332)
Male x Education -0.06858 0.1367
Effect of Onset (0.1308) (0.0978)
Effect of MLDA14 on -0.0071 0.019
Onset by 14 (0.0226) (0.0173)
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0052
on Onset by 14 (0.0345)
Effect of one relative 0.1204 (c)
on Onset by 14 (0.0689)
Effect of two relatives 0.1285 (c)
on Onset by 14 (0.0745)
Male 1.363 (a) 0.8605 (b)
(0.2292) (0.4121)
[rho] 0 (h) 0.8714 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 24.623
Log likelihood -3,186.42 (e) -3,176.78
Bivariate Probit
Specification
Exclusion Restriction
for Onset by 14
Sample N = 2,893 LNTax14 One Rel.
Effect on education by -1.5900 (a) -1.4887 (a)
Onset at age 14 (0.1255) (0.2218)
Marginal effect -0.4840 (f) -0.4664 (f)
(0.1477) (0.1347)
Male 0.4791 (b) 0.4261 (c)
(0.2326) (0.2479)
Male x Education 0.1384 0.12
Effect of Onset (0.0965) (0.1074)
Effect of MLDA14 on
Onset by 14
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0344
on Onset by 14 (0.0267)
Effect of one relative 0.1335 (b)
on Onset by 14 (0.0571)
Effect of two relatives
on Onset by 14
Male 0.8597 (b) 0.8251 (b)
(0.4124) (0.4196)
[rho] 0.8782 (a) 0.8162 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 26.017 9.783
Log likelihood -3,176.54 -3,173.81
Bivariate Probit
Specification
Exclusion Restriction
for Onset by
Sample N = 2,893 Two Rels. All Four
Effect on education by -1.4985 (a) -1.5279
Onset at age 14 (0.2221) (0.1840)
Marginal effect -0.4683 (f) -0.4741 (f)
(0.1361) (0.1404)
Male 0.4313 (c) 0.4459 (c)
(0.2479) (0.2479)
Male x Education 0.1225 0.1265
Effect of Onset (0.1075) (0.1032)
Effect of MLDA14 on 0.012
Onset by 14 (0.0187)
Effect of LNtax14 -0.0295
on Onset by 14 (0.0293)
Effect of one relative 0.0907 (d)
on Onset by 14 (0.0610)
Effect of two relatives 0.1368 (b) 0.0785
on Onset by 14 (0.0606) (0.0642)
Male 0.8332 (b) 0.8012 (c)
(0.4182) (0.4158)
[rho] 0.8222 (a) 0.8417 (a)
[[chi].sup.2.sub.1] 9.535 12.958
Log likelihood -3,174.26 -3,172.04
Notes: SEs are in parentheses. Other variables used in regression
and not reported in the table: Nonwhite, %FamInt, FamIntMiss,
Siblings, NotOldest, OlderSibHS, MomEd, MomEdMiss, DadEd, DadEdMiss,
Reading Available, Raised Catholic, Religious, Urban, %HSDip,
Asvab Verb, and RottInd.
(a) Significant at 1%.
(b) Significant at 5%.
(c) Significant at 10%.
(d) Significant at 15%.
(e) Sum of Log-likelihoods from two separate probit estimates,
one of school completion, the other from alcohol onset.
(f) Marginal probability averaged over the entire sample.
(g) SD based on sample average marginal probability.
(h) Estimation restriction sets [rho] = 0; identical to
estimating separate probits.
Source: NLSY 1979-96.