Behavioral economics and drinking behavior: preliminary results from an Irish college study.
Delaney, Liam ; Harmon, Colm ; Wall, Patrick 等
1. INTRODUCTION
This article provides an examination of alcohol consumption among a
sample of students at an Irish university. We examine the role of key
demographic factors such as gender, age, year in college, housing
tenure, and parental socioeconomic circumstances in determining
students' alcohol consumption. Moreover, we attempt to measure and
model behavioral parameters such as time preferences, risk perception,
and personality as direct influences on consumption. We also examine the
effects of peer, sibling, and parental drinking.
II. POTENTIAL DETERMINANTS OF ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION--BEHAVIORAL
DRIVERS
The literature on alcohol consumption has identified a number of
key influences. (1) However, the role of individual differences in
personality merits consideration in the examination of health risk
behavior patterns.
The most validated personality assessment tool currently used is
based on the "Big-Five" personality framework, a
multidimensional typology assessing personality on five
dimensions--extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional
stability, and openness to experience. For example, a study of
university students by LemosGiraldez and FidalgoAliste (1997) found that
"conscientiousness" and "agreeableness" measures
were significant predictors of health-related behaviors and attitudes
regarding smoking and alcohol consumption. "Low Agreeableness"
(which indicates, e.g., hostility) has been linked with poor health
behaviors (Smith and Christensen 1992). (2)
Parental and sibling factors have also been investigated as
determinants of alcohol consumption patterns (e.g., Windle 2000). These
effects could operate through a number of channels. The alcohol
consumption patterns of family members may be reflective of genetic
predispositions to alcohol consumption. Individual consumption patterns
may be reflective of imitative behavior or parental/sibling alcohol use
(e.g., Brody et al. 2000). Different home environments may also be
conducive to differential exposure to alcohol. Parents and siblings may
also form part of an individual's budget and time constraints. (3)
Moreover, parent and sibling effects may operate at a lag. Thus, while
the literature emphasizes the importance of peer as opposed to parental
effects on current alcohol consumption, this is mainly examined through
the lagged effect of family alcohol patterns on current alcohol
consumption, with childhood exposure to alcohol predictive of later
alcohol consumption patterns (e.g., Webster et al. 1989).
Many studies have shown that peers exert a decisive influence over
adolescent risk-taking behavior, with a greater influence than parental
effects (Allen et al. 2003; Gamier and Stein 2002; Urberg,
Decegirmencioglu, and Pilgrim 1997). Peers can shape participation in
risk-taking behaviors, such as alcohol or drug use, through a number of
means--by influencing attitudes, norms, and values; by modeling the
behavior; and by offering opportunity and support for the behavior
(Bauman and Ennett 1996). Alexander et al. (2001) emphasized how
"best friends" or close friendships may have a greater impact
on behavior than a larger peer network due to the level of contact such
relationships provide. They found that the risk for regular smoking was
increased if the individual had one or two very close friends who were
also regular smokers.
Finally, the age at which the person begins to consume alcohol has
been implicated in later patterns of heavy alcohol use by a number of
articles. This could be due to common unobserved factors affecting both
onset and later alcohol use. However, there is strong evidence that
alcohol consumption is habitual and highly persistent. Grant (1997)
examined interview data with current and former drinkers from the 1992
National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey and assessed the
probability of alcohol abuse and dependence as a function of the age at
which the individual began to consume alcohol. While only 4% of those
who began to drink after the age of 20 experienced lifetime alcohol
abuse, this figure rose to 11% for those who began drinking at 16 yr or
younger. Similarly, with regards to prevalence rates for lifetime
alcohol dependence, it was found to be 10% in those who began drinking
at 20 yr and older and more than 40% in those who began at 14 yr or
younger. The authors concluded that for each increasing year of age of
alcohol initiation, the probability of lifetime alcohol abuse declined
by 8%, and the probability of lifetime dependence declined by 14%.
The perception and judgment of risk are also central to any
theoretical model of health risk behavior on the belief that an
individual's behavior is influenced by how they perceive the
consequences of their actions and whether they believe themselves to be
vulnerable to these consequences (Millstein and Halpern-Felsher 2002).
As outlined by Slovic (1987), psychological research on risk perception
developed from studies of probability assessment and decision-making
processes. A body of literature has demonstrated that individuals
systematically misperceive risk and that the degree of misperception can
be reliably predicted by a number of factors including the salience of
the risk, its immediacy, and several other factors. This work implied
that the risks of alcohol consumption may be underestimated, as the main
consequences may not be revealed for a number of years; the risks are
largely self-imposed and relatively predictable.
Alcohol consumption may also be viewed as a manifestation of
underlying time preferences. The question of how individuals process
future priorities is interesting in this context--alcohol consumption
has frequently been viewed as myopic and indicating a high rate of time
preference. This has recently attracted the attention of economists
(e.g., Fehr 2002; O'Donoghue and Rabin 2000). There have been some
attempts to integrate survey measures of time preferences as independent
variables, explaining different types of health risks. For example,
Borghans and Golsteyn (2005) concluded that survey measures of discount
rates can explain some of the variance in body mass index (BMI), though
they found no evidence for changing discount rates being a driver for
increases in obesity rates. Henson et al. (2006) found strong
associations between future time orientation (as measured by the
Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory), higher engagement in health
protective behavior, and lower engagement in health risk behavior.
III. UCD GEARY INSTITUTE HEALTH BEHAVIOUR STUDY
The UCD Geary Institute Health Behaviour Study is planned as a
major longitudinal study on a number of diverse populations. In the
current phase, all the students of a large Irish university were
contacted via e-mail and asked to participate in a Web-based survey.
The literature points to several advantages of our approach in
terms of data collection. However, achieving high response rates is
difficult with this format. To encourage participation, we offered an
incentive of a lottery with 10 prizes of 1,000 [euro] (approximately
$1,300 at current rates). The current pilot study is based on a sample
of 4,500 students, which represents approximately 20% of the total body
of 20,000 students. This response is relatively low, taking the
population as a whole. However, statistics provided from the University
suggest that only half of the student body use the college e-mail
system, which would imply that our total sampling frame population is
closer to 10,000 (leading to response rate of 50%). More convincing is
that the mean outcomes from this data closely align with the
administrative records in terms of distribution across degree programs,
course year, and other demographics such as age and gender.
The survey was divided into nine modules: personal information such
as gender and age, physical health and psychological well-being, alcohol
consumption patterns, personality as measured by a short Big-Five
inventory (Gosling, Rentfrow, and Swann 2003), vignettes surrounding
occasional alcohol consumption, risk perceptions and other risk
behaviors, anchoring vignettes, questions on time management and time
preferences, and further demographic and family background questions.
Drinking behavior was assessed using a number of measures. First, we
examined monthly expenditure on alcohol. We also administered the World
Health Organization (WHO) Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) examination--a screening test for alcohol misuse that includes
several questions on different aspects of alcohol-related behavior. (4)
Descriptive statistics are displayed in Table 1. (5)
IV. RESULTS
In the empirical model, individuals maximize intertemporal utility
subject to their budget constraint. Standard preference parameters are
included such as measures of time preferences (as measured by survey
scales) and risk tolerance (as measured by smoking). As in many
different behavioral models, consumption can be generated by lack of
information about risk. Drinking patterns are assumed to be influenced
exogenously by peer groups and parents. (6) Age of onset influences
alcohol consumption through the effects of persistence and habit. Table
2 displays the results of multiple regression models assessing the
determinants of participation, expenditure, and scores on the AUDIT
scale.
Participation (i.e., whether a person drinks at all as opposed to
abstaining) is determined by a number of variables. Males are less
likely to participate than females controlling for other factors.
Foreign students are not significantly more or less likely to
participate than Irish students. Higher parental income makes one more
likely to participate. With regards to effects of parental and peer
drinking, we find little evidence that parental drinking influences the
decision to participate in alcohol consumption. Participation is related
to closest peer and outside college peer drinking, though not to college
friend drinking.
Second, we examine the determinants of scores on the WHO AUDIT
scale. The time preferences scale substantially predicts higher drinking
levels across all specifications. The results reveal a substantial
effect of peer group drinking but very little effect of parent drinking.
Indeed, parental variables in general are poor explanatory variables in
explaining AUDIT scores, with neither parental income nor parental
education having an effect on individual AUDIT scores. The drinking
levels of the individuals' closest peer are most predictive of own
drinking, with the drinking behavior of friends outside college more
predictive than the drinking levels of college friends. In terms of
personality variables, conscientiousness predicts lower scores on the
AUDIT, while extraversion predicts higher scores. There is a slight
relationship between openness to experience and lower scores and no
discernible relationship between scores and measures of agreeableness or
"nervousness." High perception of risks related to drinking
predicts lower AUDIT scores. Consistent with the previous literature,
AUDIT scores are higher for those who begin drinking at an earlier age.
Both cannabis usage and ecstasy usage predict higher scores on the
AUDIT, pointing toward complementarities between consumption of alcohol
and illegal drugs. However, even after controlling for all these
factors, males score substantially higher than females on the AUDIT. The
drinking behavior of domestic students in college dorms is more
pronounced than the other groups. In fact, the raw correlation between
living in a student dorm and drinking is actually negative, but this
reflects the higher number of foreign students who live in dorm
accommodation. Most interestingly from an economic perspective, high
time preferences (i.e., lower patience scores) increase AUDIT scores,
but scores are not related to personal disposable income.
Third, we examine the determinants of alcohol expenditures. Alcohol
expenditures and consumption are not necessarily strongly related,
particularly among students as students may access cheap alternatives if
their income is not high. This is borne out by the fact that our results
demonstrate that disposable income does not have an effect on the AUDIT
score, but it does have an effect on alcohol expenditures. Moreover,
while older students do not score higher on the AUDIT, they do spend
more on alcohol and nights out. This points to a substitution toward
more expensive types of drinking occasions as both income and age
increase. Once again, we find very little evidence for parental effects
on alcohol expenditures in terms of parental education, parental income,
or parental drinking. Those living away from home spend more on alcohol
than those residing at home. In this instance, the drinking behavior of
the individuals' closest friend, average friend at college, and
average friend outside college all have a similar positive effect on
alcohol expenditures. Higher perceptions of risks from drinking do not
have an effect on alcohol expenditures. High time preferences predict
higher alcohol expenditure. Both cannabis and ecstasy usage predict
alcohol expenditures, suggesting consumption complementarities.
V. CONCLUSIONS
This article is an initial attempt to incorporate several important
economic and psychological parameters into the study of alcohol
consumption and provides a useful baseline study for future research in
this area. The results provide evidence that income is a very weak
explanatory factor for alcohol consumption patterns and that higher
income students instead of consuming more alcohol tend to consume more
expensive alcohol. Alcohol consumption is better explained by
personality and peer factors than by parental resources, family
background, or disposable income. In terms of individual psychological
and economic parameters, time preferences are strongly related to
alcohol consumption, and we also find an effect of extraversion,
conscientiousness, and levels of well-being.
Exploring the use of psychometric measures of time preferences in
explaining risk behavior is an important future question for this study.
The exploration of the interplay between parental, peer, and sibling
effects is also a high priority for future research. While the models
outlined in this article indicate that drinking behavior by close
friends affects one's own drinking and that peer and sibling
drinking have much bigger effects than parental drinking, more work
needs to be done to examine the transmission of parental drinking to
peer selection and the endogeneity of peer effects.
ABBREVIATIONS
AUDIT: Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
BMI: Body Mass Index
WHO: World Health Organization
REFERENCES
Alexander, C., M. Piazza, D. Mekos, and T. Valente. "Peers,
Schools and Adolescent Cigarette Smoking." Journal of Adolescent
Health, 29, 2001, 22-30.
Allen, M., W. A. Donohue, A. Griffin, D. Ryan, and M. M. M. Turner.
"Comparing the Influence of Parents and Peers on the Choice to Use
Drugs." Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 2003, 163-86.
Babor, T. F., J. C. Higgins-Biddle, J. B. Saunders, and M. G.
Monteiro. AUDIT The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test:
Guidelines for use in Primary Health Care. Geneva, Switzerland: World
Health Organization, 2001.
Bauman, K. E., and S. T. Ennett. "On the Importance of Peer
Influence for Adolescent Drug Use: Commonly Neglected
Considerations." Addiction, 91, 1996, 185-98.
Borghans, L., and B. H. H. Golsteyn. Time Discounting and the Body
Mass Index, IZA Discussion Papers 1597. Bonn: Institute for the Study of
Labor (IZA), 2005.
Brody, G. H., X. Ge, J. Katz, and I. Arias. "A Longitudinal
Analysis of Internalization of Parental Alcohol-Use Norms and Adolescent
Alcohol Use." Applied Developmental Science, 4, 2000, 71-9.
Courtenay, W. H. "Behavioural Factors Associated with Disease,
Injury and Death among Men: Evidence and Implications for
Prevention." The Journal of Men's Studies, 9, 2000, 81-142.
Fehr, E. "The Economics of Impatience." Nature, 415,
2002, 269-72.
Grant, B. F. "Age at Onset of Alcohol Use and its Association
with DSM-IV Alcohol Abuse and Dependence: Results from the National
Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey." Journal of Substance
Abuse, 9, 1997, 103-10.
Garnier, H. E., and J. A. Stein. "An 18-year Model of Family
and Peer Effects on Adolescent Drug Use and Delinquency." Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 2002, 45-56.
Gosling, S. D., P. J. Rentfrow, and W. B. Swann. "A Very Brief
Measure of the Big-Five Personality Domains." Journal of Research
in Personality, 37, 2003, 504-28.
Henson, J. M., M. P. Carey, K. B. Carey, and S. A. Maisto.
"Associations Among Health Behaviors and Time Perspective in Young
Adults: Model Testing with Boot-Strapping Replication." Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 29, 2006, 127-37.
Kubicka, L., Z. Matejcek, Z. Dytrych, and Z. Roth. "IQ and
Personality Traits Assessed in Childhood as Predictors of Drinking and
Smoking Behaviour in Middle-aged Adults: A
24-year-follow-up-study." Addiction, 96, 2001, 1615-28.
LemosGiraldez, S., and A. M. FidalgoAliste. "Personality
Disposition and Health-related Habits and Attitudes: A Cross-sectional
Study." European Journal of Personality, 11, 1997, 197-209.
Millstein, S. G., and B. L. Halpern-Felsher. "Perceptions of
Risk and Vulnerability." Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 2002,
10-27.
O'Donoghue, T., and M. Rabin. "The Economics of Immediate
Gratification." Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 13, 2000,
233-50.
Slovic, P. "Perception of Risk." Science, 236, 1987,
280-5.
Smith, T. W., and A. J. Christensen. "Hostility, Health, and
Social context," in Hostility, Coping, and Health, edited by H.
Friedman. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1992,
33-48.
Urberg, K. A., S. M. Decegirmencioglu, and C. Pilgrim. "Close
Friend and Group Influence on Adolescent Cigarette Smoking and Alcohol
use." Developmental Psychology, 33, 1997, 834-44.
Webster, D. W., E. Harburg, L. Gleiberman, A. Schork, and W.
Difranceisco. "Familial Transmission of Alcohol Use: Parent and
Adult Offspring Alcohol Use over 17 Years." Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, 50, 1989. 557-66.
Windle, M. "Parental, Sibling and Peer Influences on
Adolescent Substance Use and Alcohol Problems." Applied
Developmental Science, 4, 2000, 98-110.
(1.) For example, the international literature on health risk
behaviors reflects consistent gender differences in alcohol consumption
and frequency of use. For example, Courtenay (2000) reviewed a
substantial body of national data and meta-analyses and concluded that
males of all ages are more likely than females to engage in behaviors
that increase the risk of disease, injury, and death, many of which are
preventable.
(2.) Kubicka et al. (2001) examined whether childhood personality
ratings on three of the Big-Five dimensions would predict adult drinking
and smoking behavior and showed that low levels of conscientiousness
emerged as a significant predictor in adult smoking and heavy episodic drinking, while those exhibiting high "extraversion" show
higher daily levels of alcohol consumption (Kubicka et al. 2001).
(3.) For example, it is important to assess for college students
whether the student is still living at home with their parents, in which
case one might expect that their behavior would be more constrained by
family norms regarding alcohol consumption.
(4.) The AUDIT was developed and validated over the past two
decades by the WHO as a simple screening instrument for excessive
drinking (Babor et al. 2001). Initially designed for use in primary
healthcare settings, it can also be self-administered or used by
non-health professionals to identify alcohol dependence and a number of
specific negative consequences of drinking. The AUDIT explicitly focuses
on symptoms within the past year. It is the only alcohol screening test
designed for international use; its use with primary healthcare patients
has been validated in six countries (Babor et al. 2001).
(5.) Statistics are displayed both for all people who responded to
the given question and for all people who responded to every single
question. This gives some indication of the nature and scope of
potential biases related to partial response. As can be seen, those who
completed the survey fully tended to be slightly younger, with lower
parental income, and were less likely to be smokers. The effects are not
substantial but do give a useful clue as to the potential direction of
survey biases.
(6.) This will be tested in later work through the gathering of
more detailed information on family background.
LIAM DELANEY, COLM HARMON and PATRICK WALL *
* We thank Arnaud Chevalier, Kevin Denny, Orla Doyle, Arie Kapteyn,
James Smith, and Ian Walker for their useful comments in both the design
and the execution of this study and this article. Lorna Sweeney, Martin
Ryan, Claire Milner, Maurice Collins, Fearghal O hAodha worked superbly
on various aspects of the project. Bas Weerman and Tim Colvin of RAND
programmed the Web-based survey. UCD Geary Institute is a recipient of
funding from Diageo Ireland PLC, a major drinks company.
Delaney: Lecturer in Economics and Public Health, UCD Geary
Institute, UCD School of Economics and UCD School of Public Health and
Population Science, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4,
Ireland. E-mail
[email protected]
Harmon: Professor of Economics, UCD Geary Institute, University
College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland & Institute for the
Study of Labour, Schaumburg-Lippe-Str. 5-9 D-53113 Bonn, Germany.
Wall: Associate Professor of Public Health, UCD Geary Institute and
UCD School of Public Health and Population Science, University College
Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland.
TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics
Observation Used in the Full
Regression Analysis
Variable Observation Mean SD
AUDIT 1,647 10.81526 5.97641
Age 1,647 20.85356 2.338071
Male 1,647 0.422987 0.494178
Cannabis 1,647 2.106745 1.227275
Ecstasy 1,647 1.179835 0.588057
Father drinking 1,647 3.534884 1.514687
Mother drinking 1,647 3.144282 1.485554
Close friend drinking 1,647 3.911267 1.136818
College friend drinking 1,647 4.187281 0.871446
Home friend drinking 1,647 4.056042 0.949243
Parental income 1,647 4.876896 1.679349
Time preferences 1,647 52.50351 9.127586
GHQ-12 1,647 29.13843 5.466925
Openness 1,647 10.48909 2.121953
Conscientiousness 1,647 9.837168 2.614222
Extraversion 1,647 8.823703 2.891579
Agreeableness 1,647 10.116 2.11491
Nervousness 1,647 6.684055 2.829748
Disposable income 1,647 848.9627 729.1022
Religiosity 1,647 3.325554 1.067577
Risk perception 1,647 22.21762 23.36467
Age started drinking 1,647 17.09102 1.590214
Private renting 1,647 0.357643 0.479446
Student residences 1,647 0.151692 0.358827
Own property 1,647 0.017503 0.131174
Foreign full--time student 1,647 0.063011 0.243053
Foreign visiting student 1,647 0.025671 0.158198
Never smoked 1,647 0.625438 0.484151
Father lower secondary 1,647 0.210035 0.407452
Father upper secondary 1,647 0.26021 0.438877
Father university 1,647 0.443991 0.496998
Mother lower secondary 1,647 0.181447 0.385501
Mother upper secondary 1,647 0.342474 0.474676
Mother university 1,647 0.432322 0.495543
Parents separated 1,647 0.129019 0.314201
Observation Used in the Full
Regression Analysis
Variable Minimum Maximum
AUDIT 0 41
Age 17 29
Male 0 1
Cannabis 1 6
Ecstasy 1 5
Father drinking 1 6
Mother drinking 1 6
Close friend drinking 1 6
College friend drinking 1 6
Home friend drinking 1 9
Parental income 1 7
Time preferences 11 77
GHQ-12 12 48
Openness 3 14
Conscientiousness 2 14
Extraversion 2 14
Agreeableness 3 14
Nervousness 2 14
Disposable income 5 8,550
Religiosity 1 5
Risk perception 1 100
Age started drinking 1 27
Private renting 0 1
Student residences 0 1
Own property 0 1
Foreign full--time student 0 1
Foreign visiting student 0 1
Never smoked 0 1
Father lower secondary 0 1
Father upper secondary 0 1
Father university 0 1
Mother lower secondary 0 1
Mother upper secondary 0 1
Mother university 0 1
Parents separated 0 1
Total Observed on Each Variable
Variable Observation Mean SD
AUDIT 3,980 11.36432 6.41615
Age 4,446 21.5578 4.33041
Male 4,440 0.453153 0.497857
Cannabis 3,492 2.130584 1.218878
Ecstasy 3,471 1.187842 0.597673
Father drinking 3,381 3.558119 1.547648
Mother drinking 3,419 3.169348 1.533224
Close friend drinking 3,437 3.939773 1.181017
College friend drinking 3,433 4.197786 0.916487
Home friend drinking 3,428 4.081389 1.000189
Parental income 2,258 4.769708 1.72452
Time preferences 3,436 53.16473 9.252668
GHQ-12 4,037 29.41243 5.361055
Openness 3,533 10.52335 2.144637
Conscientiousness 3,530 9.984986 2.625496
Extraversion 3,539 9.072337 2.914627
Agreeableness 3,507 10.07015 2.183976
Nervousness 3,532 6.492922 2.819141
Disposable income 3,334 919.7682 786.075
Religiosity 3,422 3.310929 1.094373
Risk perception 3,330 23.02372 37.57011
Age started drinking 2,980 17.59329 18.56875
Private renting 4,423 0.330771 0.470544
Student residences 4,423 0.140176 0.347209
Own property 4,423 0.046575 0.21075
Foreign full--time student 4,343 0.072991 0.260152
Foreign visiting student 4,343 0.022795 0.149268
Never smoked 3,510 0.580057 0.49362
Father lower secondary 3,416 0.16452 0.370801
Father upper secondary 3,416 0.229216 0.42039
Father university 3,416 0.537178 0.498689
Mother lower secondary 3,413 0.140639 0.347699
Mother upper secondary 3,413 0.323469 0.467869
Mother university 3,413 0.494287 0.500041
Parents separated 3,415 0.103075 0.295899
Total Observed on Each Variable
Variable Minimum Maximum
AUDIT 0 41
Age 12 61
Male 0 1
Cannabis 1 6
Ecstasy 1 6
Father drinking 1 6
Mother drinking 1 6
Close friend drinking 1 6
College friend drinking 1 6
Home friend drinking 1 6
Parental income 1 7
Time preferences 11 77
GHQ-12 12 48
Openness 3 14
Conscientiousness 2 14
Extraversion 2 14
Agreeableness 2 14
Nervousness 2 14
Disposable income 5 8,550
Religiosity 1 5
Risk perception 1 100
Age started drinking 1 27
Private renting 0 1
Student residences 0 1
Own property 0 1
Foreign full--time student 0 1
Foreign visiting student 0 1
Never smoked 0 1
Father lower secondary 0 1
Father upper secondary 0 1
Father university 0 1
Mother lower secondary 0 1
Mother upper secondary 0 1
Mother university 0 1
Parents separated 0 1
Notes: The highest level of nonresponse was on the parental income
question. This generates the bulk of the disparity between the
observed sample and those used in the full regression models.
A number of other observations were discarded due to implausibility.
The sample is also restricted to those aged younger than 30 yr. GHQ,
General Health Questionnaire.
TABLE 2
Determinants of Alcohol Expenditure, WHO AUDIT, and Alcohol
Participation
Alcohol
Variable Expenditure
Age 2.27 *** (0.87)
Male 6.59 *** (4.07)
Lodgings/renting -12.27 *** (4.46)
College dorm -6.08 (5.82)
Own property -20.36 (15.01)
Foreign full-time student -28.55 *** (7.69)
Foreign visiting student -32.08 *** (11.27)
Never smoked -19.81 *** (6.59)
Cannabis use (1-6 scale) 6.88 *** (2.04)
Ecstasy use (1-6 scale) 12.89 *** (3.75)
Mothers drinking (1-6 scale) -2.26 (1.41)
Fathers drinking (1-6 scale) -0.66 (1.46)
Close friend drinking (1-6 scale) 13.52 *** (1.83)
College friends drinking (1-6 scale) 9.44 *** (2.41)
Outside college friends drinking 9.70 *** (2.35)
(1-6 scale)
Father lower secondary 10.50 (7.67)
Father upper secondary 4.96 (7.69)
Father higher education 4.47 (7.63)
Mother lower secondary 10.70 (9.71)
Mother upper secondary -4.22 (9.40)
Mother higher education -12.97 (9.55)
Parental income (1-7 scale) 0.20 (0.15)
Parents separated -8.93 (6.03)
Time preferences -1.19 *** (0.23)
Well-being (GHQ-12, 12-48 0.63 ** (0.37)
positive scale)
Openness -1.74 * (0.95)
Conscientiousness -0.56 (0.80)
Extraversion 3.70 *** (0.71)
Agreeableness 1.42 (0.90)
Neuroticism -0.39 (0.76)
Disposable income (in [euro]) 0.02 *** (0.00)
Religiosity (1-6 scale from religious 1.25 (1.84)
to not religious)
Risk perception (1-100 scale) -0.15 *** (0.06)
Age started drinking -1.01 ** (0.46)
Constant -79.28 (33.15)
N 1,647
[R.sup.2]/pseudo [R.sup.2] 0.28
WHO
Variable AUDIT
Age -0.15 *** (0.05)
Male 1.83 *** (0.25)
Lodgings/renting 0.57 ** (0.28)
College dorm 1.64 *** (0.37)
Own property 0.03 (0.89)
Foreign full-time student -1.95 *** (0.51)
Foreign visiting student -2.50 *** (0.74)
Never smoked -1.31 *** (0.40)
Cannabis use (1-6 scale) 0.82 *** (0.12)
Ecstasy use (1-6 scale) 0.94 *** (0.22)
Mothers drinking (1-6 scale) 0.07 (0.09)
Fathers drinking (1-6 scale) -0.05 (0.09)
Close friend drinking (1-6 scale) 0.84 *** (0.12)
College friends drinking (1-6 scale) 0.26 ** (0.16)
Outside college friends drinking 0.67 *** (0.15)
(1-6 scale)
Father lower secondary -0.16 (0.48)
Father upper secondary -0.23 (0.48)
Father higher education -0.44 (0.48)
Mother lower secondary 0.20 (0.62)
Mother upper secondary -0.04 (0.61)
Mother higher education -0.58 (0.62)
Parental income (1-7 scale) 0.01 (0.01)
Parents separated -0.79 (0.37)
Time preferences -0.07 *** (0.01)
Well-being (GHQ-12, 12-48 -0.07 *** (0.02)
positive scale)
Openness -0.10 * (0.06)
Conscientiousness -0.24 *** (0.05)
Extraversion 0.26 *** (0.04)
Agreeableness 0.04 (0.06)
Neuroticism 0.00 (0.05)
Disposable income (in [euro]) 0.00 (0.00)
Religiosity (1-6 scale from religious 0.03 (0.12)
to not religious)
Risk perception (1-100 scale) -0.01 *** (0.00)
Age started drinking -0.08 ** (0.04)
Constant 10.78 (2.08)
N 1,647
[R.sup.2]/pseudo [R.sup.2] 0.40
Alcohol
Variable Participation
Age 0.00 (0.00)
Male -0.02 *** (0.01)
Lodgings/renting 0.00 (0.01)
College dorm -0.02 ** (0.01)
Own property --
Foreign full-time student -0.02 (0.02)
Foreign visiting student -0.02 (0.02)
Never smoked -0.01 (0.01)
Cannabis use (1-6 scale) 0.03 *** (0.01)
Ecstasy use (1-6 scale) -0.02 (0.01)
Mothers drinking (1-6 scale) 0.00 (0.00)
Fathers drinking (1-6 scale) 0.00 (0.00)
Close friend drinking (1-6 scale) 0.01 *** (0.00)
College friends drinking (1-6 scale) 0.01 ** (0.00)
Outside college friends drinking 0.01 (0.00)
(1-6 scale)
Father lower secondary -0.01 (0.01)
Father upper secondary 0.00 (0.01)
Father higher education -0.02 (0.01)
Mother lower secondary 0.02 ** (0.01)
Mother upper secondary 0.02 (0.01)
Mother higher education 0.03 ** (0.01)
Parental income (1-7 scale) 0.00 (0.00)
Parents separated -0.01 (0.00)
Time preferences 0.001 *** (0.00)
Well-being (GHQ-12, 12-48 0.00 (0.00)
positive scale)
Openness 0.00 (0.00)
Conscientiousness 0.00 (0.00)
Extraversion 0.01 ** (0.00)
Agreeableness 0.01 ** (0.00)
Neuroticism 0.00 (0.00)
Disposable income (in [euro]) 0.00 (0.00)
Religiosity (1-6 scale from religious 0.01 ** (0.00)
to not religious)
Risk perception (1-100 scale) 0.00 (0.00)
Age started drinking --
Constant --
N 1,647
[R.sup.2]/pseudo [R.sup.2] 0.29
Notes: Robust standard errors are given in parentheses following
coefficient. The base category for "lodging/renting," "college dorm,"
and "own property" is whether the individual lives with their parents.
The base category for "foreign full-time student" and "foreign
visiting student" is "Irish student." Openness, conscientiousness,
extraversion, agreeableness, and nervousness are constructed by
summing two items for each variable derived from the Gosling,
Rentfrow, and Swann (2003) brief measure of the Big Five. Risk
perception was elicited by asking respondents to assign a probability
of dying from alcohol-related diseases consequent on drinking-specified
quantities of alcohol over time. Marginal effects are reported for the
participation equation. Well-being was measured by coding and summing
the 12 items of the GHQ, giving a scale from 12 (lowest well-being)
to 48 (highest well-being). The authors have estimated significant
numbers of alternative specifications which are available from the
authors on request. GHQ, General Health Questionnaire.
*** Significant level at 1%; ** significant level at 5%; * significant
level at 10%.