Investing in health: the long-term impact of head start on smoking.
Anderson, Kathryn H. ; Foster, James E. ; Frisvold, David E. 等
I. INTRODUCTION
There is a strong evidence that early childhood socioeconomic
conditions have long-term economic consequences, reinforcing and
sustaining disparities over the lifecourse (1) This observation provides
a compelling rationale for public investments targeted toward
disadvantaged children to expand opportunities and break the cycle of
poverty. Head Start is the principal federally funded program through
which the United States invests directly in the human capital of
disadvantaged preschool children. Since its inception in 1965 as part of
President Lyndon Johnson's "War on Poverty," Head Start
has provided services to more than 23 million preschool children; in
2005, there were more than 900,000 children enrolled in the program at a
total cost of $6.8 billion (Office of Head Start 2006a). To achieve its
overall goal of increasing the school readiness of participants, Head
Start provides a comprehensive set of services including education,
health, nutritional, and social services to participants and their
families. Research has shown that Head Start has positive impacts on
participants' human capital, both in childhood and in adulthood.
(2)
In recent years, questions have arisen about the relative
effectiveness of Head Start and whether it should be continued in its
present form (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2003). Much
of the debate has centered on the magnitude and persistence of the
cognitive achievements of Head Start participants. However, given the
link between education and health, (3) and the comprehensive nature of
Head Start (with substantial health components), one could reasonably
expect it to have a favorable impact on participants' health as
well as education. If significant health effects were found to persist
into adulthood, this in itself could alter the evaluation of Head Start
and have important implications for the associated policy debate.
Smoking is the leading preventable cause of mortality in the United
States (Mokdad et al. 2004). It is linked to an extensive list of
diseases (Chaloupka and Warner 2000) and imposes large economic costs on
society (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2002). Tobacco use
generally begins before individuals graduate high school and, as an
addictive behavior, youth smoking is linked to adult smoking (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services 1994). Adolescents from low
socioeconomic status households with low academic achievement are more
likely to use tobacco products (U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services 1994). Thus, comprehensive development programs targeted toward
disadvantaged youths that are designed to improve child outcomes have
the potential to influence smoking, improve health over the lifecourse,
and reduce the social costs from smoking. Suggestive evidence that Head
Start might influence the smoking behavior of participants comes from
evaluations of other preschool programs that targeted low-income
children. Participants in both the Carolina Abecedarian preschool
program and the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program were much less likely
to smoke as adults (Barnett and Masse 2007; Belfield et al. 2006).
In this article, we assess the impact of Head Start participation
on adult smoking behavior using data from the Panel Study of Income
Dynamics (PSID) and its supplements on early childhood education and
health. We examine the cohort of adults (above age 21 in 1999) whose age
is low enough to have potentially participated in a Head Start program.
Following Currie and Thomas (1995) and Garces, Thomas, and Currie
(2002), we employ a sibling-based model to control for unobservable
family characteristics that may affect smoking or the decision to
participate in Head Start.
Our results show that Head Start participation significantly
influences smoking-related behavior. We estimate that if Head Start
participants had not enrolled in the program, these individuals would be
approximately 20 percentage points more likely to smoke cigarettes
beyond age 25. The magnitude of the impact of Head Start participation
is considerable and similar to the impact from the randomized evaluations of the Perry Preschool and Carolina Abecedarian early
childhood development programs that target similar populations. Based on
these results, Head Start participation may yield significant
improvements in health over the lifecourse through its impact on smoking
behavior. These results have relevance for the policy debate surrounding Head Start; they suggest that public investment in disadvantaged
children can have a lasting impact. We provide the first systematic
evidence in the literature that Head Start participation is associated
with improvement in adult health outcomes.
II. BACKGROUND
Head Start is a comprehensive, national, and federally funded
program that provides early childhood developmental services to
disadvantaged children and their families. Federal guidelines state that
at least 90% of the children enrolled in each of the Head Start centers
must be from families whose total annual income before taxes is less
than or equal to the poverty line and at least 10% of the participants
must be children with disabilities (Office of Head Start 2006b). Of the
906,993 children enrolled in the 19,800 centers throughout the country
in 2005, 52% were 4 yr old and 34% were 3 yr old (Office of Head Start
2006a). Thirty five percent of Head Start children in 2005 were white
and 31% were black. Thirty three percent of children also classified
their ethnicity as Hispanic (Office of Head Start 2006a).
In light of the overall focus of the Head Start program on
improving school readiness, most evaluations of the program assessed the
impact of Head Start on test scores or other measures of educational
success. Head Start participation is generally believed to be associated
with short-term cognitive benefits; however, researchers have also found
that after a few years, these benefits begin to fade (Aughinbaugh 2001;
Lee, Brooks-Gunn, and Schnur 1988; Lee et al. 1990; McKey et al. 1985).
On the other hand, Head Start participation leads to sizeable increases
in educational attainment. White Head Start participants are 40
percentage points less likely to repeat a grade in school (Currie and
Thomas 1995), 22 percentage points more likely to graduate high school
(Garces, Thomas, and Currie 2002), and 19 percentage points more likely
to attend college (Garces, Thomas, and Currie 2002). Additionally,
Ludwig and Miller (2007) provide evidence that Head Start participation
increases the educational attainment of both white and black
participants.
Recent evidence from the randomized evaluation of Head Start
demonstrates that Head Start participation increases cognitive abilities
in reading, writing, literacy, and vocabulary (U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services 2005). The magnitude of the estimated impacts has led
to calls for greater emphasis in the program on literacy and increasing
cognitive achievement (Besharov 2005). (4)
Increasing the cognitive achievement of the disadvantaged children
in the Head Start program is clearly an important goal. However, because
of the comprehensive services provided in the program, greater cognitive
ability is likely to be only one of many outcomes. Considering the
importance of early childhood conditions on social and economic
outcomes, early childhood investment programs such as Head Start have
the potential to substantially improve broader indicators of social
welfare. To understand the value of the Head Start program to society,
it is necessary to assess the lasting impact of the program on a wide
array of outcomes. (5)
Previous research has demonstrated that Head Start participation
results in positive short-term health benefits for children in terms of
general physical health, motor skills, and nutrition (McKey et al.
1985). Head Start participants are more likely to receive
age-appropriate health screenings or dental examinations (Hale, Seitz,
and Zigler 1990) and be immunized for the measles (Currie and Thomas
1995). Additionally, Head Start participants are less likely to be obese
in later childhood (Frisvold 2007). Head Start participation has also
led to a substantial decline in child mortality rates (Ludwig and Miller
2007).
In this article, we analyze the impact of Head Start participation
on adult smoking. Head Start may influence smoking behavior for a
variety of reasons. As discussed by Heckman and Masterov (2007),
noncognitive skills during childhood are important predictors of a
variety of adult outcomes, including smoking. Enhancing the noncognitive
skills or social development of participants is a key component of the
mission of the Head Start program. Thus, one potential mechanism is that
Head Start participation may influence noncognitive outcomes that are
associated with smoking. For example, Head Start participation reduces
hyperactive behavior and behavior problems (U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services 2005). Hyperactivity is related to youth smoking
(Milberger et al. 1997), and behavior problems are associated with
smoking initiation and persistence (Griesler, Kandel, and Davies 2002).
Head Start may influence individuals' preferences for the future,
self-image, self-esteem, confidence, and impulse control, as well as
peer group formation--all of which are related to smoking use (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services 1994).
Another potential mechanism is that Head Start participation may
influence smoking as a result of increases in educational attainment
(Garces, Thomas, and Currie 2002). More educated individuals make better
decisions about their health and are less likely to engage in behaviors
that reduce their health. In particular, education has a negative
influence on the likelihood that an adult smokes (Gilleskie and Harrison
1998; Kabat and Wynder 1987; Kenkel 1991; Sander 1995).
More directly, Head Start educates children and parents about the
health consequences of smoking and increases in health knowledge reduce
smoking (Hsieh et al. 1996). Sixty-two percent of Head Start health
coordinators report discussing the consequences of tobacco use with
children and 78% of Head Start parents continue this discussion at home
with their children (Keane et al. 1996). The health education in Head
Start may directly influence participants' behavior and indirectly
influence children by influencing parents' behavior. Not only might
parents of Head Start children who smoke quit but parents are also
likely to reinforce positive health behaviors with their children.
Parenting behavior toward smoking has a significant influence on youth
smoking, and parents' smoking status is strongly related to
children's smoking behavior (Powell and Chaloupka 2005).
III. ESTIMATION STRATEGY
The ideal design for evaluating the effect of Head Start
participation on smoking would begin with a random assignment of
children to a treatment group that attends Head Start and a control
group that does not attend any form of preschool. The behavior of
individuals would then be monitored over time, and the observed
difference in smoking behavior across the two groups would provide an
estimate of the impact of attending Head Start. However, in the absence
of such a randomized experiment, we analyze existing survey data and
attempt to account for several important deviations from this ideal. (6)
Observable characteristics of families that are associated with the
selection of participants into Head Start could also be associated with
smoking behavior. For example, Harrell et al. (1998) find that children
in low-socioeconomic status families are more likely to begin smoking.
To allow for the options that individuals attend Head Start, other forms
of preschool, or no preschool, the relationship between Head Start and
smoking controlling for individual and family background characteristics
during the preschool years can be modeled as:
(1) [S.sub.i] = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1]H[S.sub.i] +
[[beta],.sub.2]P[S.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.3][X.sub.i] + [[eta].sub.i],
where [S.sub.i] represents the current smoking status of individual
i, HS and PS are indicator variables for participation in Head Start or
other preschool programs, X includes exogenous family background and
individual characteristics, and [eta] is random error. The coefficient [[beta].sub.1] is the marginal impact of attending Head Start,
[[beta].sub.2] is the impact of attending other pre school programs, and
[[beta].sub.3] is the impact of childhood conditions on health behavior.
Unobservable characteristics of families that influence smoking may
be correlated with Head Start attendance. More than 890,000 parents
volunteer in Head Start (Office of Head Start 2006a); these parents may
choose to make other investments in their child that could positively
influence behavior. Failure to account for such unobserved variables
could lead to an overestimate of the impact of Head Start participation.
Alternatively, the most disadvantaged of eligible applicants are
selected for Head Start by the program administrators. Head Start
programs are required by federal guidelines to establish a formal
selection mechanism to determine which eligible children are admitted,
and children with the greatest need for Head Start services are required
to be chosen by program administrators (Office of Head Start 2006b). (7)
Thus, children in families with incomes farthest below the poverty line
are most likely to be chosen to enroll in the program. Also, children in
high-risk families are preferentially admitted into the program.
Although high risk may be defined differently across programs, this
category can include children in families with substance abuse or
domestic violence; children in families afflicted by a crisis such as
death, separation, terminal illness, or chronic health issues; children
referred into Head Start by a community agency; or other special
circumstances. Based on the Head Start selection criteria, Head Start
participants are disadvantaged according to characteristics that are
likely to be unobserved, especially family characteristics. Felitti et
al. (1998) demonstrate that children raised in dysfunctional households
characterized by abuse are more likely to smoke. The failure to control
for unobserved characteristics related to Head Start attendance that are
also determinants of smoking will lead to an underestimate of the impact
of Head Start participation.
Following established methodology by Currie and Thomas (1995) and
Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002), we compare sibling outcomes to
determine the effects of Head Start participation. We restrict the
sample to individuals with at least one sibling and include a
family-specific fixed effect. The fixed-effects controls for fixed
unobservable family characteristics that affect individuals'
smoking decisions and are correlated with the decision to participate in
Head Start. The smoking behavior of an individual is estimated as:
(2) [S.sub.if] = [[delta].sub.0]+[[delta].sub.1]
H[S.sub.if]+[[delta].sub.2]P[S.sub.if]+[[delta].sub.3][X.sub.if] +
[[phi].sub.f]+[v.sub.if],
where [S.sub.if] is now a measure of smoking for individual i in
family f, while [phi] is the mother-specific fixed effect and v is
random error. The coefficient [[delta].sub.1] is the marginal impact of
Head Start, [[delta].sub.2] is the impact of other preschool programs,
and [[delta].sub.3] is the impact of childhood conditions on smoking
behavior.
Equation (2) determines the impact of Head Start participation by
comparing the smoking status of siblings who attended Head Start to the
behavior of siblings who did not attend any form of preschool. The
identifying assumption is that the underlying reasons that some siblings
attended Head Start and others did not are uncorrelated with smoking.
Thus, to assess the validity of this empirical strategy, it is important
to consider why siblings differ in their attendance in the Head Start
program.
One reason that the early childhood experiences of siblings might
differ is that differences in family income or size could mean that some
siblings are eligible for Head Start during the preschool years, while
others are not. Therefore, we control for the family background
characteristics that are specific to each child during the ages in which
children are eligible for Head Start.
If the unobserved household characteristics that are related to
selection into Head Start are constant across siblings then
[[delta].sub.1] is a consistent estimate of the impact of Head Start
participation. However, it is possible that parents decide whether their
children will attend Head Start based on characteristics that are
related to future outcomes but are unobservable to researchers. For
example, if parents choose to invest more in the health or human capital
of a sibling with higher ability by sending that child to Head Start but
not sending other siblings then they may also choose to make other
investments in that child that positively influence his or her behavior.
As discussed by Currie and Thomas (1995), parental favoritism could
result in differential investments among siblings. The estimate of the
impact of Head Start participation would be confounded by the unobserved
family characteristics that influence early childhood experience and
later adult behavior. Selection on unobservable characteristics is not
likely to be restricted to Head Start attendance; similar parental
decisions that lead to differences in Head Start participation among
siblings are also likely to lead to differences in attendance in other
preschools. In fact, Currie and Thomas (1995) suggest that other
preschool children are more likely to benefit from favoritism than Head
Start children. To assess the potential that differential parental
investments lead to differences in Head Start participation, we compare
the individual characteristics of siblings who attended Head Start to
those who did not and examine differences in characteristics such as
birth order that are related to parental investments (Price 2008).
Within our multivariate model, to reduce the bias from unobserved family
characteristics that are not constant across siblings, we compare the
effects of participation in Head Start to participation in other
preschools ([[delta].sub.1] - [[delta].sub.2]). The difference in
coefficients is a consistent estimator of the effect of Head Start if
parents view Head Start and other preschools as of comparable quality.
If parents think that other preschools are of higher quality than Head
Start (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2001) and send their
preferred children to the other preschools, the difference estimate
([[delta].sub.1] - [[delta].sub.2]) is likely to be a lower bound
estimate of the impact of Head Start (Currie and Thomas 1995).
Our estimation strategy is subject to additional limitations.
First, although the fixed-effects strategy is implemented to reduce the
endogenous variation in Head Start participation, it also reduces the
exogenous variation in participation (Bound and Solon 1999; Griliches
1979). Second, the fixed-effects strategy exaggerates measurement error,
which biases the estimates toward zero and contributes to the
underestimation of the effect of Head Start participation.
Third, spillover benefits from Head Start participants to siblings
could result in an underestimate of the effect of Head Start
participation (Barnett and Hustedt 2005; Garces, Thomas, and Currie
2002). To explore the possibility of sibling spillovers from Head Start
participation, we follow Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002) and modify
Equation (3) to include an interaction term for Head Start participation
and birth order and whether the child is the oldest sibling. We do not
find evidence to support the possibility of sibling spillovers for
smoking status.
Fourth, the fixed-effects strategy might lead to an underestimate
of the effect of Head Start participation because of compensating
behaviors by parents to equalize outcomes among siblings (Barnett and
Hustedt 2005). Even if the reasons that lead parents to differentially
send siblings to Head Start are exogenous, if parents seek to compensate
the children who did not attend then the resulting reduced-form
estimates of the effect of Head Start may be less than the actual impact
of the program. We do not find evidence to support this possibility;
former Head Start participants and their siblings, for example, are
equally likely to receive an inheritance from their caregivers. However,
we cannot rule out the possibility that there are other unobservable
parental investments that are designed to equalize sibling outcomes.
Finally, changes in mothers' smoking status due to Head Start
participation are not likely to contribute to the estimated impact of
Head Start in the fixed-effects specification. (8) To explore the
importance of mothers' smoking behavior as a mechanism through
which Head Start might influence smoking, we examine mothers'
smoking status before and after the ages of Head Start eligibility.
Mothers' smoking status by children's age is constructed from
survey responses to current smoking status, age at initiation, and
quitting age in the 1999 wave of the PSID. We find that mothers of Head
Start children smoke less after their children attended Head Start than
before; however, the rate of decline is similar to the decline
experienced by all mothers. (9) Thus, while children's Head Start
attendance could influence mothers' smoking behavior, it does not
appear that estimates based on sibling comparisons that ignore this
potential mechanism underestimate the impact of Head Start
participation. (10)
In summary, because of possible sibling spillovers, compensating
behaviors by parents, measurement error, and negative selection into
Head Start within families, we emphasize that our estimates of the
impact of Head Start participation are potentially lower bound estimates
of the treatment effect on the treated.
IV. DATA
Our research evaluates the impact of Head Start on smoking using
data from the PSID. The PSID is a longitudinal study of U.S. households
and individuals that began in 1968 with a national sample of
approximately 4,800 households. Members of these households, their
offspring, and current coresidents have been interviewed on an annual or
biennial basis since the inception of the PSID.
Supplements to the PSID occasionally expand the information
collected. In 1995, additional questions were asked of interviewees that
related to early childhood education, (11) This included whether or not
an individual attended Head Start or a preschool, nursery school, or day
care center other than Head Start. In 1999, health-related questions
were asked of adult members of the household, including whether or not
the individual currently smokes cigarettes. (12) This question was
repeated in 2003. The advantage of using the PSID for this analysis is
that it brings together information on early childhood education and on
adult smoking, along with other important socioeconomic variables. One
of the key questions arising in studies of this type is whether impacts
found for a group of people at a given date might dissipate or disappear
over time (Currie and Thomas 1995). To address this concern, we selected
a cohort from the PSID whose birth years are late enough to be
consistent with participation in Head Start (which occurs mainly during
ages 3 and 4) and early enough to be an adult (which in the present
study is taken to be over age 21) at the two dates the data on smoking
behavior were gathered. We analyze the impact of Head Start on smoking
for this cohort using the 1999 smoking data and then follow-up 4 yr
later to see whether the original findings are sustained.
The family background variables available in the PSID include the
average total family income between ages 3 and 6, (13) the mother's
average years of formal schooling completed between ages 3 and 6, the
average family size between ages 3 and 6, whether the father was present
between ages 3 and 6, and disability status (14) during childhood.
Controlling for family background throughout the period of early
childhood education (ages 3-6), as opposed to capturing a snapshot of
the family environment at the most common age of preschool attendance
(age 4), minimizes measurement error, reduces missing data, and provides
a more accurate description of the family environment during the early
childhood years. Variables that measure individual characteristics
include age, gender, race (black, white, and other race), marital
status, birth order, and whether the individual is the oldest child. We
also include two dummy variables that are interactions between birth
prior to 1966 and participation in Head Start or other preschool. (15)
Individuals with missing values for any variable in the 1999 sample
are excluded from the analysis using the 1999 smoking data. (16) A
similar restriction is imposed on the 2003 sample. Because the PSID
began in 1968, individuals who were older than 5 yr in 1968 are excluded
from both samples. This restriction ensures that family background
characteristics are available during the ages of Head Start eligibility.
Thus, the oldest individuals included are 36 yr old in 1999. Individuals
in the 1999 sample attended Head Start between 1968 and 1982. The
descriptive statistics for the 1999 sample are provided in Table l and
include the means and standard errors for the early childhood education
variables, smoking behavior, and individual and family background
characteristics of PSID respondents. (17) The descriptive statistics for
the 2003 sample are included in Table A2.
Because a drawback of the fixed-effects methodology is the sample
restriction to individuals with at least one sibling, Table 1 includes
the descriptive statistics for all individuals and the subset of
individuals who have at least one sibling in the sample. The descriptive
statistics reveal that while the sample size is dramatically reduced,
restricting the analysis to individuals with at least one sibling does
not change the overall characteristics of the sample.
The Head Start, preschool, and neither samples are subsets of
individuals within the sibling sample who participated in Head Start; a
preschool program, nursery school, or day care center besides Head
Start; or none of the above, respectively. These descriptive statistics
highlight the disadvantaged background of Head Start participants and
the importance of controlling for the home environment in estimating the
impact of Head Start participation. Former Head Start participants are
more likely to be from larger families with less income and to be the
younger children in the family. These families are also less likely to
have had a father present in the home and mothers had less education on
average. Additionally, Head Start participants are more likely to be
black and less likely to be currently married.
While the fixed-effects model in Equation (2) restricts the sample
to individuals with at least one sibling, the Head Start coefficient in
the equation ([[delta].sub.1] is estimated from the sample of sibling
pairs where one sibling attended Head Start and the other siblings did
not. The columns under the heading "Effective Sample for Head
Start" summarize the sample of individuals who contribute to the
estimation of the Head Start coefficient. The sample of all Head Start
participants is similar to the sample of Head Start participants who
contribute to the estimation of the Head Start coefficient with two
exceptions--the effective sample has a larger percent of individuals who
are female and a smaller percent who are black. The number of
individuals who contribute to the estimation of 81 is 136 (in 52
families), and 62 of these individuals attended Head Start. (18) While
this sample is small, the sample size is comparable to many of the model
early childhood program evaluations described in Barnett (1995). This
sample size is smaller than the effective sample size in Garces, Thomas,
and Currie (2002) of 255 individuals in 100 families because smoking
behavior is not asked of all individuals within households in the PSID.
The sample size influences the precision but not the magnitude of the
estimated impact of Head Start.
Comparisons of means between Head Start participants and their
siblings yield no statistically significant differences in individual
characteristics, with the exception of age. Head Start participants are
younger than their siblings who did not attend Head Start and are of
higher birth order. Siblings of a higher birth order generally have
worse education and labor market outcomes (Black, Devereux, and Salvanes
2005); one explanation for this is that parents spend less time with
later-born children (Price 2008). Based on this descriptive comparison,
it does not seem likely that parents choose which child to send to Head
Start based on observed individual characteristics associated with adult
success or greater parental investment. Additionally, a fixed-effects
regression, similar to Equation (2), of individual and family
characteristics on Head Start participation suggests that a decrease in
family income influences the likelihood that a sibling will attend Head
Start. (19) On the other hand, child-specific characteristics such as
age, gender, birth order, and disability status do not influence which
sibling will attend Head Start.
V. RESULTS
Table 2 displays the estimation results, based on linear
probability models, of the relationship between Head Start participation
and whether an individual currently smokes cigarettes. (20) The first
two columns display the results for the 1999 sample of individuals. (21)
The first column displays the results of the estimates of Equation (1),
which controls for individual and family background characteristics to
account for observable selection into Head Start. The coefficient for
Head Start demonstrates that Head Start participants are 9.7 percentage
points less likely to smoke than their siblings who did not attend Head
Start, and this estimate is statistically significant at the 10% level.
The coefficient for other preschools is negative but small in magnitude
and statistically insignificant. The difference estimate suggests that
Head Start participants are 8.4 percentage points less likely to smoke,
although this estimate is not measured precisely.
The estimates of the mother-specific fixed-effects model, Equation
(2), are displayed in the next column. After accounting for unobservable
selection into the program, the Head Start coefficient becomes even more
negative and is statistically significant at the 5% level. This change
suggests that Head Start participants are significantly disadvantaged
across unobserved characteristics and is consist with previous research
on the impact of Head Start participation (Frisvold 2007; Garces,
Thomas, and Currie 2002). The coefficient for other preschools becomes
positive but is not statistically significant after accounting for
unobservable selection into other preschools, which suggests that
children sent to other preschools are relatively advantaged in
unobserved characteristics. Because the unobservable household
characteristics that determine the selection decisions associated with
early childhood education may not be fixed across siblings, the
difference in smoking between Head Start participants and their siblings
is compared to the difference in smoking between other preschool
participants and their siblings. This reveals a negative and
statistically significant effect of Head Start participation on smoking.
Head Start participation reduces the probability that an adult will
smoke cigarettes by 0.248.
The last two columns in Table 2 contain the results for the 2003
sample. Comparing the estimates from the last two columns to those in
the first two columns provides evidence on the persistence of the impact
of Head Start participation. After controlling for observed and
unobserved selection with mother-specific fixed effects and the
difference in the Head Start and other preschool coefficients, Head
Start participation now reduces the likelihood that an adult will smoke
by 19.4 percentage points. While the impact of Head Start participation
is smaller for the 2003 sample, this estimated impact remains
substantial and statistically significant.
VI. DISCUSSION
We estimated the impact of Head Start participation on the smoking
behavior of adults using data from the PSID and found, for this cohort,
that adults who attended Head Start were less likely to smoke as adults
than adults who attended other preschools or did not attend preschool.
(22) For this cohort, the smoking benefit from attending Head Start
diminished but persisted at least over the next 4 yr.
Given the sizeable impact of Head Start participation on
educational outcomes, the large estimated impact on smoking is
consistent with our expectations. The magnitude of the persistent impact
of Head Start participation on smoking is comparable to the impact of
other early childhood education programs on adult smoking (Barnett and
Masse 2007) and the quit rates obtained through traditional intensive
smoking cessation programs (Anthonisen et al. 2005; Cutler 2002). The
estimated 0.194 reduction in the probability of smoking implies that the
probability that a Head Start participant would smoke had he or she not
participated in Head Start would be 0.418 in 2003. (23) To assess the
plausibility of our results, we compare this estimated counterfactual
probability to various comparison groups that are similar to Head Start
participants. The estimated counterfactual for Head Start participants
is higher than the prevalence of smoking of 30.5% among adults below the
poverty line in 2003 (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2005).
However, individuals who attend Head Start are likely to be more
disadvantaged than the average individual with income below the poverty
line because Head Start program administrators select the most
disadvantaged of the eligible applicants. More relevant comparison
groups are the control groups in the randomized evaluations of the
Carolina Abecedarian preschool program and the High/Scope Perry
Preschool Program because both of these programs targeted populations
similar to Head Start participants. The prevalence of smoking in the
control groups for both of these evaluations was 55% (Barnett and Masse
2007), well above the estimated counterfactual of Head Start
participants. The most relevant comparison group, however, is likely to
be Head Start participants' own siblings. In the PSID sample used
in this analysis, 39% of Head Start siblings smoked in 1999 and 37% of
Head Start siblings smoked in 2003. These comparisons suggest that the
large estimated magnitude of the impact of Head Start participation is
plausible.
Head Start participation can influence adult smoking for a variety
of reasons. (24) We explore two potential mechanisms: (1) Head Start
increases educational attainment and thus decreases smoking and (2) Head
Start improves noncognitive skills and thus decreases smoking. For the
first path, we are able to use data from the PSID to explore its
importance. For the second path, measures of noncognitive skills are not
available; we rely on existing research to understand its importance.
To examine the influence of educational attainment on the
relationship between Head Start participation and smoking, we estimate
Equation (2) including the years of completed schooling as an additional
explanatory variable. (25) The results are displayed in Table 3.
Comparing the results from Table 3 to the results in Table 2
demonstrates the importance of completed schooling as a pathway through
which Head Start can influence smoking. Introducing educational
attainment as an additional regressor has little influence on the
estimated impact of Head Start participation on smoking using the 1999
sample. This lack of influence may result because not all individuals
had completed schooling in the sample. For the 2003 sample, including
completed education in the model decreases the estimated impact of Head
Start participation to -0.146, and this estimate is no longer
statistically significant at conventional levels. Thus, there is some
evidence that Head Start influences the smoking behavior of participants
through increased educational attainment. However, education does not
completely explain the relationship between Head Start participation and
smoking.
An additional mechanism through which Head Start may be influencing
adult smoking is through increases in noncognitive skills, such as sense
of control, discipline, motivation, self-esteem, and social skills.
Noncognitive skills are important determinants of a variety of
behavioral outcomes and are as important as cognitive skills in
predicting smoking behavior (Heckman, Stixrud, and Urzua 2006; Heckman
and Masterov 2007). An increase from the 25th percentile to the 75th
percentile of the distribution of noncognitive skills, measured by locus
of control, leads to approximately a 15 percentage point decrease in the
probability of smoking (Heckman, Stixrud, and Urzua 2006). Longitudinal
studies in the medical literature show that hyperactivity in adolescence leads to greater smoking initiation (Hartsough and Lambert 1987;
Milberger et al. 1997) and smoking initiation at earlier ages (Milberger
et al. 1997). Problem behaviors in adolescence also predict smoking
initiation (Griesler, Kandel, and Davies 2002) and becoming a daily
smoker (Kandel, Kiros, Schaffran, and Hu 2004).
Evidence from experimental evaluations demonstrates that Head Start
participation improves noncognitive skills. The Head Start Impact Study
found that program participation reduced hyperactive behavior by 0.20
standard deviations and reduced total problem behavior by 0.16 standard
deviations (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2005). (26,27)
Alternatively, participation in other preschools does not lead to
similar increases in noncognitive skills. Magnuson, Ruhm, and Waldfogel
(2007) demonstrate that prekindergarten attendance not only improves
cognitive skills but also increases behavioral problems. Child care
attendance is associated with behavioral problems (National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network
2003) and hyperactivity and aggressiveness (Baker, Gruber, and Milligan
2005). (28) Based on the body of evidence from this existing literature,
the increase in noncognitive skills from Head Start participation likely
contributes to the measured impact on smoking in our analysis. In
summary, multiple pathways, including educational attainment and the
development of noncognitive skills, generate the impact of Head Start
participation on smoking.
VII. CONCLUSIONS
Most evaluations of Head Start have emphasized the cognitive and
educational benefits to participants. In this article, we argue that
Head Start also impacts participants' health by altering their
likelihood of smoking. We estimated a fixed-effects model for a cohort
of young adults using data from the PSID and found a substantial and
sustained reduction in smoking associated with Head Start participation.
One traditional way of interpreting our result is to convert the
estimate into dollar terms. Between 1995 and 1999, the estimated annual
cost of smoking was $3,391 per smoker, which included excess medical
expenditures and lost productivity (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention 2002). The present value of a 19.4% reduction in smoking,
assuming a 3% real discount rate, is $9,967 per each Head Start
participant entering the program at 4 yr of age in 2003. (29)
Alternatively, using a 7% real discount rate, the present value becomes
$2,563. For comparison purposes, the average cost of each Head Start
participant in 2003 was $7,092. The value of the reduction in smoking
associated with Head Start, then, represents 36%-141% of the program
costs for an average Head Start participant. (30) Since this is only one
of the many outcomes from participating in Head Start, these results
suggest that there are significant personal and social benefits
associated with the Head Start program.
ABBREVIATION
PSID: Panel Study of Income Dynamics
doi: 10.1111/j.1465-7295.2008.00202.x
APPENDIX
TABLE A1
Means of Mothers' Smoking Behavior by Child's Preschool Attendance
Child Attended Child Attended
Head Start Other Preschool
(a) Mother smoked after age 6 0.363 (0.055) 0.314 (0.030)
(b) Mother smoked prior to age 3 0.447 (0.056 0.424 (0.032)
Difference (a--b) -0.084 (0.029) -0.110 (0.020)
Sample size 163 357
Child Attended
No Preschool
(a) Mother smoked after age 6 0.376 (0.020)
(b) Mother smoked prior to age 3 0.447 (0.020)
Difference (a--b) -0.072 (0.011)
Sample size 883
Notes: The means and standard errors (in parentheses) are
weighted by the 1999 PSID individual weights.
Source: PSID.
TABLE A2
Descriptive Statistics for the 2003 Sample
Entire Sample
All Head Start Preschool
Smoke 0.231 (0.013) 0.191 (0.035) 0.188 (0.022)
Head Start 0.065 (0.007) 1.000 (0.000) 0.027 (0.008)
Preschool 0.308 (0.014) 0.129 (0.033) 1.000 (0.000)
Age 33.357 (0.125) 33.595 (0.414) 31.926 (0.222)
Female 0.511 (0.015) 0.561 (0.052) 0.527 (0.028)
Black 0.116 (0.008) 0.704 (0.048) 0.096 (0.014)
Other race 0.043 (0.007) 0.036 (0.021) 0.056 (0.015)
Birth order 2.478 (0.057) 4.079 (0.419) 1.978 (0.079)
Oldest 0.317 (0.014) 0.216 (0.037) 0.398 (0.027)
Married 0.633 (0.014) 0.385 (0.051) 0.634 (0.027)
Disabled 0.039 (0.006) 0.064 (0.024) 0.030 (0.010)
Family size 4.846 (0.051) 6.354 (0.397) 4.329 (0.071)
Father not 0.132 (0.010) 0.361 (0.048) 0.135 (0.019)
present
Mother's 12.248 (0.067) 10.770 (0.229) 13.244 (0.119)
education
Family income 55.048 (0.946) 33.109 (1.698) 64.275 (2.144)
Sample size 1,757 258 465
Sibling Sample
All Head Start
Smoke 0.233 (0.016) 0.194 (0.043)
Head Start 0.069 (0.009) 1.000 (0.000)
Preschool 0.303 (0.018) 0.110 (0.041)
Age 33.333 (0.154) 33.539 (0.548)
Female 0.499 (0.019) 0.530 (0.065)
Black 0.101 (0.010) 0.670 (0.062)
Other race 0.047 (0.009) 0.042 (0.030)
Birth order 2.593 (0.076) 4.838 (0.536)
Oldest 0.274 (0.017) 0.170 (0.042)
Married 0.650 (0.018) 0.401 (0.064)
Disabled 0.034 (0.007) 0.038 (0.026)
Family size 5.080 (0.067) 7.065 (0.512)
Father not 0.111 (0.012) 0.333 (0.057)
present
Mother's 12.370 (0.088) 10.737 (0.285)
education
Family income 55.383 (1.217) 33.904 (1.927)
Sample size 1,005 156
Sibling Sample
Preschool Neither
Smoke 0.200 (0.028) 0.259 (0.020)
Head Start 0.024 (0.009) 0.000 (0.000)
Preschool 1.000 (0.000) 0.000 (0.000)
Age 31.803 (0.271) 33.544 (0.182)
Female 0.499 (0.034) 0.488 (0.023)
Black 0.078 (0.017) 0.064 (0.008)
Other race 0.060 (0.019) 0.039 (0.010)
Birth order 2.049 (0.096) 2.509 (0.078)
Oldest 0.347 (0.032) 0.302 (0.020)
Married 0.665 (0.032) 0.647 (0.022)
Disabled 0.033 (0.013) 0.030 (0.008)
Family size 4.506 (0.089) 5.082 (0.066)
Father not 0.110 (0.021) 0.097 (0.013)
present
Mother's 13.377 (0.149) 11.984 (0.096)
education
Family income 66.069 (2.873) 50.879 (1.011)
Sample size 297 699
Effective Sample
for Head Start
Head Start Head Start
All Participants Siblings
Smoke 0.305 (0.055) 0.224 (0.068) 0.370 (0.078)
Head Start 0.444 (0.061) 1.000 (0.000) 0.000 (0.000)
Preschool 0.150 (0.040) 0.091 (0.041) 0.197 (0.062)
Age 34.314 (0.528) 33.232 (0.833) 35.179 (0.599)
Female 0.649 (0.055) 0.687 (0.078) 0.619 (0.077)
Black 0.494 (0.061) 0.458 (0.093) 0.523 (0.081)
Other race 0.032 (0.022) 0.037 (0.036) 0.028 (0.028)
Birth order 3.908 (0.323) 4.228 (0.524) 3.652 (0.382)
Oldest 0.167 (0.042) 0.100 (0.044) 0.221 (0.065)
Married 0.541 (0.061) 0.475 (0.094) 0.593 (0.077)
Disabled 0.033 (0.016) 0.028 (0.018) 0.036 (0.026)
Family size 6.081 (0.218) 6.090 (0.328) 6.074 (0.292)
Father not 0.316 (0.058) 0.350 (0.089) 0.289 (0.077)
present
Mother's 11.067 (0.247) 11.291 (0.358) 10.888 (0.336)
education
Family income 36.752 (2.060) 37.436 (3.286) 36.205 (2.615)
Sample size 144 65 79
Notes: The means and standard errors (in parentheses) are
weighted by the PSID sample weights to be representative of the
national population. The entire sample is the sample of all
individuals in the PSID older than 21 yr in 1999 with complete
information on individual and family characteristics. The sibling
sample is the subset of individuals who have at least one sibling
in the sample. The Head Start, preschool, and neither samples are
subsets of individuals who participated in Head Start; a
preschool program, nursery school, or day care center besides
Head Start; or none of the above, respectively. The effective
sample for Head Start is the sample of individuals who contribute
to the identification of the Head Start coefficient in the
fixed-effects estimation. Family income, family size, and mother's
education are averaged over the ages 3-6. Father not present
measures that the father was not a family member at some point
during the ages 3-6.
Source: PSID.
TABLE A3
Determinants of Head Start Participation among Siblings
Head Start
Outcome Participation
Age 0.002 (0.009)
Female 0.012 (0.022)
Birth order 0.029 (0.023)
Oldest 0.011 (0.027)
Disabled 0.038 (0.057)
Family income (In) -0.118 ** (0.053)
Family size 0.017 (0.024)
Father not present 0.106 ** (0.052)
Mother's education 0.039 (0.022)
Constant -0.139 (0.510)
Sample size 922
Number of families 401
Notes: Estimates and standard errors (in parentheses)
are based on a mother-specific fixed-effects specification
from the 1999 sample.
Source: PSID.
** Significant at the 5% level.
REFERENCES
Andreski, P., E. Gouskova, K. McGonagle, and B. Schoeni. "An
Analysis of the Quality of the Health Data in the Panel Study of Income
Dynamics: 1999-2003." Unpublished manuscript, University of
Michigan, 2005.
Anthonisen, N. R., M. A. Skeans, R. A. Wise, and J. Manfreda.
"The Effects of a Smoking Cessation Intervention on 14.5-Year
Mortality: A Randomized Clinical Trial." Annals of Internal
Medicine, 142, 2005, 233-39.
Aughinbaugh, A. "Does Head Start Yield Long-Term
Benefits?" Journal of Human Resources, 36, 2001, 641-65.
Baker, M., J. Gruber, and K. Milligan. "Universal Childcare,
Maternal Labor Supply and Family WellBeing." National Bureau of
Economic Research Working Paper No. 11832, 2005.
Barnett, W. S. "Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Programs
on Cognitive and School Outcomes." Future of Children, 5(3), 1995,
25-50.
Barnett, W. S., and J. T. Hustedt. "Head Start's Lasting
Benefits." Infants and Young Children, 18, 2005, 16-24.
Barnett, W. S., and L. N. Masse. "Comparative Benefit-Cost
Analysis of the Abecedarian Program and its Policy Implications."
Economics of Education Review, 26, 2007, 113-25.
Belfield, C. R., M. Notes, S. Barnett, and L. Schweinhart.
"The High/Scope Perry Preschool Program: Cost-Benefit Analysis
Using Data from the Age-40 Follow-up." Journal of Human Resources,
41, 2006, 162-90.
Besharov, D. J. "Head Start's Broken Promise."
American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research: On the Issues,
October 25, 2005, 1-2.
Black, S. E., P. J. Devereux, and K. G. Salvanes. "The More
the Merrier? The Effect of Family Size and Birth Order on
Children's Education." Quarterly Journal of Economics, 120,
2005, 669-700.
Bound, J., and G. Solon. "Double Trouble: On the Value of
Twins-Based Estimation of the Return to Schooling." Economics of
Education Review, 18, 1999, 169-82.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. "Annual
Smoking-Attributable Mortality, Years of Potential Life Lost, and
Economic Costs--United States, 1995-1999." Morbidity and Mortality
Weekly Report, 51, 2002, 300-03.
--. "Cigarette Smoking Among Adults-United States, 2003."
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 54, 2005, 509-13.
Chaloupka, F. J., and K. E. Warner. "The Economics of
Smoking," in Handbook of Health Economics, edited by A. J. Culyer
and J. P. Newhouse. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland, 2000,
1539-627.
Currie, J. "Early Childhood Education Programs." Journal
of Economic Perspectives, 15, 2001, 213-38.
Currie, J., and D. Thomas. "Does Head Start Make a
Difference?" American Economic Review, 85, 1995, 341-64.
Cutler, D. M. "Behavioral Health Interventions: What Works and
Why?" Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University, 2002.
Cutler, D., and A. Lleras-Muney. "Education and Health:
Evaluating Theories and Evidence." National Bureau of Economic
Research Working Paper No. 12352, 2006.
Felitti, V., R. Anda, D. Nordenberg, D. Williamson, A. Spitz, V.
Edwards, M. Koss, and J. Marks. "Relationship of Childhood Abuse
and Household Dysfunction to Many of the Leading Causes of Death in
Adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study." American
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 1998, 245-58.
Frisvold, D. "Head Start Participation and Childhood
Obesity." Early Childhood Research Collaborative Discussion Paper
No. 108, 2007.
Frisvold, D., and J. Lumeng. "Expanding Exposure: Can
Increasing the Daily Duration of Head Start Reduce Childhood
Obesity?" Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, 2008.
Garces, E., D. Thomas, and J. Currie. "Longer-Term Effects of
Head Start." American Economic Review, 9, 2002, 999-1012.
Gilleskie, D., and A. Harrison. "The Effect of Endogenous
Health Inputs on the Relationship between Health and Education."
Economics of Education Review, 17, 1998, 279-97.
Griesler, P. C., D. B. Kandel, and M. Davies. "Ethnic
Differences in Predictors of Initiation and Persistence of Adolescent Cigarette Smoking in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth."
Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 4, 2002, 79-93.
Griliches, Z. "Sibling Models and Data in Economics:
Beginnings of a Survey." Journal of Political Economy, 87, 1979,
S37-S64.
Grossman, M., and R. Kaestner. "Effects of Education on
Health," in The Social Benefits of Education, edited by J. Behrman
and N. Stacey. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press, 1997,
69-123.
Hale, B., V. Seitz, and E. Zigler. "Health Services and Head
Start: A Forgotten Formula." Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 11, 1990, 447-58.
Harrell, J. S., S. I. Bandgiwala, S. Deng, J. P. Webb, and C.
Bradley. "Smoking Initiation in Youth: The Roles of Gender, Race,
Socioeconomics, and Developmental Status." Journal of Adolescent
Health, 23, 1998, 271-79.
Hartsough, C. S., and N. M. Lambert. "Pattern and Progression
of Drug Use among Hyperactives and Controls: A Prospective Short-Term
Longitudinal Study." Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
Allied Disciplines, 28, 1987, 543-53.
Haveman, R., and B. Wolfe. "The Determinants of
Children's Attainments: A Review of Methods and Findings."
Journal of Economic Literature, 33, 1995, 1829-78.
Hayward, M. D., and B. K. Gorman. "The Long Arm of Childhood:
The Influence of Early-Life Social Conditions on Men's
Mortality." Demography, 41, 2004, 87-107.
Heckman, J. J., and D. V. Masterov. "The Productivity Argument
for Investing in Children." National Bureau of Economic Research
Working Paper No. 13016, 2007.
Heckman, J. J., J. Stixrud, and S. Urzua. "The Effects of
Cognitive and Noncognitive Abilities on Labor Market Outcomes and Social
Behavior." Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 2006, 411-482.
Heeringa, S. G., and J. H. Connor. "1997 Panel Study of Income
Dynamics Analysis Weights for Sample Families and Individuals."
Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, 1999.
Hsieh, C., L. Yen, J. Liu, and C. J. Lin. "Smoking, Health
Knowledge, and Anti-Smoking Campaigns: An Empirical Study in
Taiwan." Journal of Health Economies, 15, 1996, 87-104.
Kabat, G. C., and E. L. Wynder. "Determinants of Quitting
Smoking." American Journal of Public Health, 77, 1987, 1301-05.
Kandel, D. B., G. E. Kiros, C. Schaffran, and M. C. Hu.
"Racial/Ethnic Differences in Cigarette Smoking Initiation and
Progression to Daily Smoking: A Multilevel Analysis." American
Journal of Public Health, 94, 2004, 128-35.
Keane, M. J., R. W. O'Brien, D. C. Connell, and N. C. Close. A
Descriptive Study of the Head Start Health Component. Washington, DC:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996.
Kenkel, D. S. "Health Behavior, Health Knowledge, and
Schooling." Journal of Political Economy, 99, 1991, 287-305.
Lee, V., J. Brooks-Gunn, and E. Schnur. "Does Head Start Work?
A l-Year Follow-Up Comparison of Disadvantaged Children Attending Head
Start, No Preschool, and Other Preschool Programs." Developmental
Psychology, 24, 1988, 210-222.
Lee, V., J. Brooks-Gunn, E. Schnur, and F. Liaw. "Are Head
Start Effects Sustained? A Longitudinal Follow-up Comparison of
Disadvantaged Children Attending Head Start, No Preschool, and Other
Preschool Programs." Child Development, 61, 1990, 495-507.
Love, J. M., L. Harrison, A. Sagi-Schwartz, M. H. van Ijzendoorn,
C. Ross, J. A. Ungerer, H. Raikes, C. Brady-Smith, K. Boiler, J.
Brooks-Gunn, J. Constantine, E. E. Kisker, D. Paulsell, and R.
Chazan-Coben. "Child Care Quality Matters: How Conclusions May Vary
with Context." Child Development, 74, 2003, 1021-33.
Ludwig, J., and D. L. Miller. "Does Head Start Improve
Children's Life Chances? Evidence from a Regression Discontinuity Design." Quarterly Journal of Economies, 122, 2007, 159-208.
Ludwig, J., and D. Phillips. "The Benefits and Costs of Head
Start." National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper No.
12973, 2007.
Magnuson, K. A, C. Ruhm, and J. Waldfogel. "Does
Pre-kindergarten Improve School Preparation and Performance?"
Economics of Education Review, 26, 2007, 33-51.
McKey, R. H., L. Condelli, H. Ganson, B. J. Barrett, C. McConkey,
and M. C. Plantz. The Impact of Head Start on Children, Families. and
Communities: Final Report of the Head Start Evaluation Synthesis and
Utilization Project. Washington, DC: CSR Inc., 1985.
Milberger, S., J. Biederman, S. V. Faraone, L. Chen, and J. Jones.
"ADHD is Associated with Early Initiation of Cigarette Smoking in
Children and Adolescents." Journal of the American Academy of Child
& Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1997, 37-44.
Mokdad, A. H., J. S. Marks, D. F. Stroup, and J. L. Gerberding.
"Actual Causes of Death in the United States, 2000." Journal
of the American Medical Association, 291, 2004, 1238-45.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. "Does Amount of Time Spent in Child
Care Predict Socioemotional Adjustment During the Transition to
Kindergarten?" Child Development, 74, 2003, 976-1005.
Office of Head Start. Head Start Program Fact Sheet. Washington,
DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2006a.
--. Head Start Program Performance Standards and Other Regulations.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2006b.
Powell, L. M., and F. J. Chaloupka. "Parents, Public Policy,
and Youth Smoking." Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 24,
2005, 93-112.
Price, J. "Parent-Child Quality Time: Does Birth Order
Matter?" Journal of Human Resources, 43, 2008, 240-65.
Sander, W. "Schooling and Smoking." Economics of
Education Review, 14, 1995, 23-33.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Preventing Tobacco
Use Among Young People.. A Report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service,
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic
Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health,
1994.
--. "Reaching Out to Families: Head Start Recruitment and
Enrollment Practices." 2001. Accessed August 24, 2007.
http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/opre/hs/faces/reports/
reaching_out_families/reach_ing_title.html
--. "Strengthening Head Start: What the Evidence Shows."
2003. http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/Strengthen HeadStart03/index.htm
--. Head Start Impact Study: First Year Findings. Washington, DC:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for
Children and Families, 2005.
KATHRYN H. ANDERSON, JAMES E. FOSTER and DAVID E. FRISVOLD *
* The authors thank Jeremy Attack, Alison Aughinbaugh, Dale Ballou,
J. S. Butler, William Collins, Robert Margo, participants at the
American Economic Association Annual Conference, Econometric Society World Congress, Society of Labor Economists Annual Meetings, and
Southern Economic Association Annual Conference, and seminar
participants at Vanderbilt University for helpful comments. Financial
support for this research was provided by the National Institute for
Child Health and Human Development (1 R03 HD045547-01A1) and the Robert
Wood Johnson Foundation (Frisvold and Foster).
Anderson. Professor, Department of Economics, Vanderbilt
University, Nashville, TN 37235-1819. Phone 1-615-322-0263, Fax
1-615-343-8495, E-mail kathryn.
[email protected]
Foster. Professor, Department of Economics, Vanderbilt University,
Nashville, TN 37235-1819. Phone 1-615-322-2192, Fax 1-615-343-8495,
E-mail
[email protected]
Frisvold: Assistant Professor, Department of Economics, Emory
University, Atlanta, GA 30322-2240. Phone 1-404-727-7833, Fax
1-404-727-4639, E-mail david.
[email protected]
(1.) See, for example, Haveman and Wolfe (1995) and Hayward and
Gorman (2004).
(2.) See Currie and Thomas (1995), Garces, Thomas, and Currie
(2002), and the references provided in the next section.
(3.) See, for example, Grossman and Kaestner (1997) and Cutler and
Lleras-Muney (2006).
(4.) However, Ludwig and Phillips (2007) emphasize that the
estimates from the Head Start Impact Study are intent to treat effects,
not estimates of the treatment effect on the treated. Additionally,
Ludwig and Phillips (2007) suggest that the expected benefits based on
the short-term experimental estimates from the Head Start Impact Study
are likely to exceed the costs of the program.
(5.) For a review of the costs and benefits of Head Start, see
Ludwig and Phillips (2007).
(6.) In the fall of 2002, as part of the congressionally mandated
evaluation of Head Start, Head Start eligible children were randomly
assigned into Head Start or a non-Head Start control group (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services 2005). These children will not
be evaluated into adulthood. The Head Start Impact Study will allow for
the estimation of the short-term benefits of Head Start participation in
a randomized setting but not the long-term benefits.
(7.) Programs are also required to obtain more applications than
the anticipated number of enrollment opportunities, so that program
administrators are able to choose those children with the greatest need
for Head Start services from the eligible applicants to enroll (Office
of Head Start 2006b).
(8.) The mother-specific fixed effect in Equation (3) also controls
for fathers' characteristics if the siblings have the same father.
(9.) These results are shown in Table A1.
(10.) Additionally, including whether mothers smoked prior to age 3
and whether mothers quit smoking during the preschool ages in Equation
(1) does not influence the results of the baseline specification shown
in Table 2.
(11.) Collecting information retrospectively about early childhood
education experiences can lead to recall error. After comparing reported
enrollment rates, racial composition, and family income from the PSID to
the national Head Start data, Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002) validate the quality of the data from the 1966 birth cohort onward. Data from the
1964 to 1965 birth cohorts are not as reliable because participation
rates in the PSID are significantly lower than the national rates.
However, their results (based on data from the 1966 to 1977 birth
cohorts) are robust to the inclusion of the 1964-1965 birth cohorts
(personal communication with Garces).
(12.) In their assessment of the quality of the smoking data in the
PSID, Andreski et al. (2005) document that for both 1999 and 2003, the
smoking rates in the PSID are comparable to those in the National Health
Interview Survey.
(13.) Total family income includes the taxable income and transfer
income, which includes public assistance, of all household members.
Taxable income includes labor, asset, rental, interest, and dividend
income. Income is converted into 1999 or 2003 prices using the consumer
price index.
(14.) Whether an individual is disabled or requires extra care is
only assessed from 1969 to 1972 and 1976 to 1978 in the PSID.
Additionally, in 1999 and 2003, individuals were asked to provide a
self-assessment of their health during childhood. Individuals are
considered disabled during childhood if they report a disability or
requiring extra care during the ages 3-6 or if they report their health
status as poor during childhood.
(15.) These two variables are included to allow for the possibility
that recall error influenced the responses of individuals born prior to
1966 as suggested by Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002). The estimated
influence of early childhood experiences on smoking does not vary
according to whether an individual was born before or after 1966.
(16.) Keeping individuals with missing values in the sample and
imputing the mean value of the sample and adding an indicator variable
to reflect that the values were imputed does not impact the results
reported below. After imposing the age restrictions, 158 individuals are
removed from the sample because of a missing value for at least one
variable, which includes 17 individuals with missing values for race,
111 individuals for birth order, 76 for oldest child, 2 for marital
status, 18 for family income, 8 for family size, 10 for father not
present, and 24 for mother's education.
(17.) The means and standard errors are weighted by the PSID sample
weights to be representative of the national population. These weights
account for the initial oversampling of low-income households, changes
in family composition, and differential attrition. These weights also
reflect the addition of a nationally representative sample of post-1968
immigrant households in the PSID and the poststratification adjustments
of the weights to the Current Population Survey by race, metropolitan
status, and Census region (Heeringa and Connor 1999).
(18.) For the 2003 sample, 144 individuals in 55 families
contribute to the estimation of the Head Start coefficient.
(19.) These results are shown in Table A3. Whether the father is
present is also related to Head Start attendance. Because the most
disadvantaged children from the eligible applicants are admitted to Head
Start by program administrators, children with one parent are more
likely to be accepted than children with two parents.
(20.) Results from conditional logit models are qualitatively
similar.
(21.) These columns correspond to columns (3) and (4) of Table 2 in
Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002). In unreported results, we find a
similar pattern of results to the findings from the specifications in
Table 2 in Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002). The baseline results are
similar for all PSID respondents and the sibling sample. The estimates
are also influenced by the inclusion of family background
characteristics. For example, the difference estimate for the 1999
sample changes from -0.018 to -0.084 when we include family background
characteristics. When we estimate the fixed-effects specification by
race, similar to columns (5) and (6) of Table 2 in Garces, Thomas, and
Currie (2002), we find large negative results for both whites and
blacks, but only the estimate for whites is statistically significant.
However, the sample size is greatly reduced when we divide the sample by
race. Including the interaction of black and Head Start in the
fixed-effects specification suggests that the impact of Head Start
participation does not vary by race.
(22.) Although we focus on smoking, which is the leading
preventable cause of mortality (Mokdad et al. 2004), Head Start
participation may influence other health behaviors as well. Poor diet
and physical inactivity form the second leading preventable cause of
mortality (Mokdad et al. 2004); we find evidence that Head Start
participation does not influence physical activity but potentially
influences diet. There is no statistically significant impact on
engaging in heavy or light exercise. We find that Head Start
participation increases height by nearly 1 inch, which can be influenced
through nutrition, but do not find any impact on weight or body mass
index. For more on the impact of Head Start participation on obesity for
more recent cohorts of participants, see Frisvold (2007) and Frisvold
and Lumeng (2008). Alcohol is the third leading preventable cause of
mortality (Mokdad et al. 2004); we do not find an influence of Head
Start on excessive drinking. However, only 3% of adults in the sibling
sample report excessive consumption of alcohol. Information about
illicit drug use is not available in the PSID main files.
(23.) The average treatment effect on the treated is the difference
between the probability that an individual who attended Head Start
smokes and the probability that that individual would have smoked had he
or she not attended Head Start. The estimated treatment effect on the
treated is -0.194, and the sample average smoking rate for Head Start
participants in the effective sample for 2003 of 0.224 (in Table A2) is
an estimate of the probability that an individual who attended Head
Start smokes. Thus, the counterfactual estimate of the probability that
an individual would have smoked had he or she not attended Head Start is
(0.224 + 0.194) or 0.418.
(24.) As discussed previously, changes in parental smoking are a
potential mechanism through which Head Start participation may influence
smoking, but this mechanism does not contribute to the estimates in our
fixed-effects specification.
(25.) An additional possibility is that increased income can
explain the relationship between Head Start participation and smoking.
Estimating Equation (2) with current total family income included and
with years of schooling completed and current total family income
included as additional explanatory variables demonstrates that there is
no influence of income on the relationship between Head Start
participation and smoking.
(26.) These figures are the average treatment on the treated
results for 3 yr olds shown in the Appendix of the study, as opposed to
the intent to treat effects shown in the main body of the report.
(27.) Garces, Thomas, and Currie (2002) provide evidence consistent
with a long-term impact of Head Start participation on noncognitive
skills; these authors show that Head Start participation reduces
criminal activity in young adults.
(28.) One potential reason for the difference in the impact on
noncognitive skills between Head Start and other care arrangements is
that Head Start classrooms are of higher quality on average than other
preschool programs and child care centers (Currie 2001), and higher
quality child care attendance is associated with less aggressive
behavior (Love et al. 2003). An additional possibility is that the Head
Start curriculum offers a wider array of services to participants and
their families than other child care arrangements.
(29.) The economic costs are converted into 2003 dollars using the
consumer price index of all items for all urban consumers (current
series). This calculation assumes that the reduction in smoking begins
at age 25 and lasts until death at age 70.
(30.) Of course, these estimates are meant to be merely indicative
of the economic benefits predicted by our results. The estimated cost
per smoker may vary across socioeconomic groups or over time, and
different cohorts may have different impacts from Head Start.
Preliminary analysis of younger Head Start participants who grew up
during the peak of youth smoking in the late 1990s suggests that their
smoking behavior may not have been significantly altered by Head Start.
However, an updated analysis of younger cohorts will require additional
data.
TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics for the 1999 Sample
Entire Sample
All Head Start Preschool
Smoke 0.245 (0.013) 0.228 (0.040) 0.207 (0.024)
Head Start 0.064 (0.007) 1.000 (0.000) 0.027 (0.008)
Preschool 0.278 (0.014) 0.118 (0.034) 1.000 (0.000)
Age 29.804 (0.120) 29.732 (0.450) 28.505 (0.221)
Female 0.525 (0.015) 0.597 (0.055) 0.551 (0.030)
Black 0.112 (0.008) 0.632 (0.054) 0.101 (0.016)
Other race 0.046 (0.007) 0.050 (0.026) 0.060 (0.016)
Birth order 2.531 (0.060) 4.340 (0.447) 2.042 (0.089)
Oldest 0.313 (0.014) 0.186 (0.038) 0.394 (0.029)
Married 0.579 (0.015) 0.381 (0.053) 0.498 (0.030)
Disabled 0.042 (0.006) 0.064 (0.025) 0.036 (0.011)
Family size 4.910 (0.053) 6.558 (0.415) 4.387 (0.082)
Father not 0.137 (0.010) 0.358 (0.051) 0.143 (0.021)
present
Mother's 12.141 (0.067) 10.680 (0.246) 13.185 (0.126)
education
Family income 51.500 (0.935) 30.849 (1.620) 60.702 (2.220)
Sample size 1,638 227 405
Sibling Sample
All Head Start
Smoke 0.232 (0.016) 0.192 (0.046)
Head Start 0.072 (0.010) 1.000 (0.000)
Preschool 0.268 (0.018) 0.110 (0.044)
Age 29.826 (0.146) 29.527 (0.585)
Female 0.522 (0.020) 0.589 (0.069)
Black 0.099 (0.011) 0.576 (0.069)
Other race 0.055 (0.010) 0.062 (0.036)
Birth order 2.659 (0.081) 4.997 (0.560)
Oldest 0.267 (0.017) 0.151 (0.043)
Married 0.592 (0.020) 0.422 (0.068)
Disabled 0.041 (0.008) 0.040 (0.028)
Family size 5.154 (0.071) 7.193 (0.534)
Father not 0.122 (0.013) 0.336 (0.061)
present
Mother's 12.226 (0.089) 10.668 (0.308)
education
Family income 51.895 (1.266) 31.607 (1.707)
Sample size 922 130
Sibling Sample
Preschool Neither
Smoke 0.211 (0.031) 0.257 (0.020)
Head Start 0.029 (0.012) 0.000 (0.000)
Preschool 1.000 (0.000) 0.000 (0.000)
Age 28.745 (0.267) 30.129 (0.175)
Female 0.527 (0.038) 0.507 (0.024)
Black 0.088 (0.020) 0.060 (0.009)
Other race 0.062 (0.022) 0.050 (0.011)
Birth order 2.214 (0.123) 2.572 (0.081)
Oldest 0.312 (0.035) 0.276 (0.021)
Married 0.489 (0.038) 0.632 (0.023)
Disabled 0.039 (0.015) 0.043 (0.010)
Family size 4.592 (0.112) 5.167 (0.067)
Father not 0.131 (0.025) 0.104 (0.014)
present
Mother's 13.337 (0.167) 11.889 (0.097)
education
Family income 64.101 (3.307) 48.095 (1.129)
Sample size 214 593
Effective Sample for Head Start
Head Start Head Start
All Participants Siblings
Smoke 0.309 (0.059) 0.205 (0.071) 0.392 (0.084)
Head Start 0.445 (0.065) 1.000 (0.000) 0.000 (0.000)
Preschool 0.152 (0.041) 0.092 (0.042) 0.201 (0.064)
Age 30.329 (0.550) 29.151 (0.878) 31.272 (0.587)
Female 0.682 (0.056) 0.747 (0.073) 0.630 (0.079)
Black 0.461 (0.064) 0.422 (0.097) 0.492 (0.085)
Other race 0.047 (0.027) 0.074 (0.051) 0.025 (0.025)
Birth order 3.715 (0.327) 4.216 (0.537) 3.314 (0.364)
Oldest 0.164 (0.043) 0.103 (0.045) 0.212 (0.066)
Married 0.585 (0.063) 0.504 (0.099) 0.650 (0.079)
Disabled 0.048 (0.023) 0.026 (0.017) 0.066 (0.038)
Family size 5.937 (0.216) 6.068 (0.329) 5.832 (0.282)
Father not 0.338 (0.062) 0.374 (0.094) 0.309 (0.083)
present
Mother's 11.101 (0.252) 11.273 (0.390) 10.963 (0.324)
education
Family income 32.893 (1.893) 32.679 (2.932) 33.064 (2.472)
Sample size 136 62 74
Notes: The means and standard errors (in parentheses) are
weighted by the PSID sample weights to be representative of the
national population. The entire sample is the sample of all
individuals in the PSID older than 21 yr in 1999 with complete
information on individual and family characteristics. The sibling
sample is the subset of individuals who have at least one sibling
in the sample. The Head Start, preschool, and neither samples are
subsets of individuals who participated in Head Start; a
preschool program, nursery school, or day care center besides
Head Start; or none of the above, respectively. The effective
sample for Head Start is the sample of individuals who contribute
to the identification of the Head Start coefficient in the
fixed-effects estimation. Family income, family size, and mother's
education are averaged over the ages 3-6. Father not present
measures that the father was not a family member at some point
during the ages 3-6.
Source: PSID.
TABLE 2
The Relationship between Head Start Participation and Smoking
1999 Sample
Equation (1) (2)
Head Start (HS) -0.097 * (0.053) -0.173 ** (0.082)
Other preschool (PS) -0.013 (0.034) 0.075 (0.053)
Difference (HS--PS) -0.084 (0.060) -0.248 *** (0.095)
Sample size 922 922
Mother fixed effects X
2003 Sample
Equation (1) (2)
Head Start (HS) -0.078 * (0.047) -0.116 (0.082)
Other preschool (PS) -0.005 (0.033) 0.077 (0.053)
Difference (HS--PS) -0.073 (0.054) -0.194 ** (0.094)
Sample size 1,005 1,005
Mother fixed effects X
Notes: Standard errors (in parentheses) allow for household
clustering and heteroskedasticity in ordinary least squares
regressions. Individual characteristics include age, female,
black, other race, birth order, whether the individual is the
oldest child, marital status, and disability status. Family
background characteristics include average total family income
between ages 3 and 6, the mother's average years of formal
schooling completed between ages 3 and 6, the average family size
between ages 3 and 6, and whether the father was present between
ages 3 and 6. Additional variables include whether a Head Start
participant was born prior to 1966 and whether a participant of
other preschool was born prior to 1966.
Source: PSID.
* Significant at the 10% level; ** significant at the 5% level;
and *** significant at the 1% level.
TABLE 3
Assessing the Role of Completed Education
on the Impact of Head Start on Smoking
1999 Sample 2003 Sample
Head Start (HS) -0.162 ** (0.080) -0.102 (0.085)
Other preschool 0.089 * (0.054) 0.044 (0.059)
(PS)
Difference -0.250 *** (0.094) -0.146 (0.099)
(HS--PS)
Sample size 883 903
Notes: This table repeats the analysis in Table 2 with
the mother-specific fixed effects with the addition of years
of completed education included as an additional explan
atory variable.
Source: PSID.
* Significant at the 10% level; ** significant at the 5%,
level; and *** significant at the 1% level.