RC-MAPS: bridging the comprehension gap in EAP reading.
Sterzik, Angela Meyer ; Fraser, Carol
Introduction
Reading is an essential academic skill, but what is reading and how
does one do it? I have asked my TESL class these questions, and although
they generally agreed on a definition of what reading is, the how
appeared to be much more difficult to answer. The general consensus when
asked "What do you do when you read?" was "I don't
know; I just read." In order to teach students how to read,
teachers need to be able to articulate not only what is required, but
more importantly, how to do it.
In academic environments, reading is the basis for much of the
knowledge that both first-language (L1) and second-language (L2)
students require to succeed. Students are expected to read not only
multiple texts and comprehend them in the sense of determining the
writer's intended meaning, but also to interpret, that is, analyze,
synthesize, and evaluate the texts, in order to develop their knowledge
base (Grabe, 2009; Grabe & Stoller, 2002). These tasks require
interaction with texts and the integration of information in and between
texts (Grabe; Grabe & Stoller; Kintsch, 1998). Before interaction
and integration can occur, however, readers must understand the
author's meaning or achieve what Kintsch in his
Construction-Integration Model of Discourse Processing has labeled a
text-based representation of meaning. The focus/goal of this text-based
representation is to identify and organize main ideas and key support in
a hierarchical structure. Kintsch (1986) associates this with
remembering a text. Armed with a solid text-based level of
comprehension, readers are then prepared to interact with and integrate
the newly acquired knowledge into their own knowledge base to achieve a
more interpretative, situation-based representation of meaning (Kintsch,
1998). At the level of situation-based comprehension, readers transform
information in the text in terms of their own purpose and knowledge base
(Kintsch, 1986).This level of comprehension is associated with learning
from a text (Kintsch, 1986, 1998); unfortunately, these interpretative
comprehension skills are challenging reading skills for many university
students.
Most students in English for Academic Purposes (EAP) reading
classes at the college and university levels are fluent readers in their
L1s. They are able to interact with texts and integrate the information
when it is in their mother tongue, so one would expect them to be able
to transfer these skills to their L2. In our experience, however, this
is often not the case. Through the explicit instruction and practice of
strategies such as context clue use, rhetorical pattern recognition, and
macro- and micro-idea identification, students are usually able to
tackle challenging texts successfully and independently and achieve
solid text-based comprehension. Yet many still have difficulty bridging
the gap from a more literal text-based comprehension to a more
interpretive, situation-based comprehension even when what is expected
is made explicit.
Through her academic journey and research, the first author
realized that the problem did not lie in the students'
understanding of what was being asked of them, but rather in how to do
it. The students knew where they needed to go; they simply did not have
a map to get there. With this realization, she embarked on a quest for a
possible solution, out of which emerged Reading Comprehension MAP for
Situation-based comprehension (RC-MAPS). RC-MAPS is a pedagogical tool
intended to be used in EAP classrooms to facilitate and foster
interpretative reading comprehension skills in L2 learners. RC-MAPS is a
strategy training procedure to help students to cope with reading
challenging texts and to develop strategies for future academic work. It
consists of simple, task-specific questioning guidelines, along with
graphic organizers to assist L2 readers in bridging the gap between
text-based and situation-based reading comprehension in academic tasks.
RC-MAPS is intended to be used as a modeling tool during explicit
questioning instruction and as scaffolding to assist L2 readers in
moving from awareness, to practice, to routine use of task-specific,
metacognitive questioning strategies.
An important factor in text-based comprehension is an awareness of
how texts are structured and organized (Grabe, 2004; Jiang & Grabe,
2007). Graphic organizers (see Appendix A) are visual representations of
the organization and interrelationships of the macro- and
micro-hierarchical structures of texts. The use of graphic organizers in
both L1 and L2 research has demonstrated their positive effects in
learners' construction of a text-based level of comprehension
(Jiang & Grabe). As such, they are commonly used in reading
classrooms to facilitate the instruction of identifying discourse
patterns as a reading strategy (Jiang & Grabe). To date, graphic
organizers have been used successfully to foster text-based
comprehension (Jiang & Grabe), but they have not been applied to
situation-based comprehension. Moreover, although situation-based
comprehension is required for academic tasks (Grabe, 2009; Grabe &
Stoller, 2002), we are unaware of a pedagogical tool with a specific
strategic focus on fostering situation-based comprehension; however,
RC-MAPS fills this gap.
In the context of RC-MAPS, graphic organizers are first used as a
visual representation of the organizational structure of texts.
Furthermore, as Jiang and Grabe (2007) suggest, the graphic organizers
used in RC-MAPS are specific to rhetorical patterns (see Appendix A).
RC-MAPS provides a map to guide students from text-based to
situation-based comprehension by using graphic organizers in two stages:
1. To scaffold and demonstrate text-based comprehension by
paraphrasing and organizing the author's ideas into their intended
hierarchy (thesis, main ideas, supporting details).
2. To scaffold and develop situation-based comprehension through
critical questioning and responding to questions.
By integrating the strategy of critical questioning, RC-MAPS
extends the use of graphic organizers from promoting text-based
comprehension to also fostering situation-based comprehension in L2
readers.
Theoretical Frameworks
Construction-Integration Model
Reading comprehension is often seen as consisting of two main
processing categories: decoding and comprehending (Grabe & Stoller,
2002; Macaro & Erler, 2008). Decoding refers to the identification
and processing of basic linguistic units (letter patterns, words,
syntactic patterns, and semantic propositions), whereas comprehending
involves the integration of information from these knowledge sources to
construct a mental representation of the entire text (Fraser, 2004;
Grabe, 2009; Grabe & Stoller, 2002; Kintsch, 1998; Macaro &
Erler, 2008). In comprehending, fluent readers remember the prior
propositions and mentally attach them to the new propositions to create
a mental representation of the author's meaning (Kintsch; Koda,
2005; Macaro & Erler; Urquhart & Weir, 1998). Kintsch refers to
this type of understanding as constructing a text-based representation
of meaning. Text-based comprehension is associated with comprehending
the words and sentences; it underlies the ability to recall and
summarize information given by the author. Text-based comprehension is
typically required in academic settings, but more importantly, it is
also required for students to move from summary-type tasks to more
difficult interpretative academic tasks such as analyzing, assessing,
and synthesizing information (Grabe; Kintsch).
In academic environments, reading is assigned not simply to
transmit information; students are required to take the information and
based on the task set by the instructor, assess, analyze, and critique
it in relation to personal experiences, prior knowledge, and/or other
readings (Grabe, 2009). Thus text-based comprehension alone is not
sufficient for academic success. Top-down processing is also required,
and this involves applying prior knowledge to define purpose(s), make
and verify hypotheses, infer to fill gaps, and question content (Macaro
& Erler, 2008; Urquhart & Weir, 1991). Readers must be able to
integrate and apply prior knowledge to their developing text-based
understanding in order to interpret a text appropriataely (Grabe;
Kintsch, 1998; Koda, 2005). Kintsch describes the integration of the
text-based understanding and the reader's background knowledge as
creating a situation-based representation of meaning. This integration
is associated with learning, and the recall of information is
reconstructive and conceptually driven. This interpretive process
underlies the ability to analyze, synthesize, and evaluate information,
all typical tasks in academic settings (Grabe; Kintsch). Furthermore, as
Grabe notes, this integration is often achieved through critical
questioning.
It is important to note that the quality of the situation-based
representation depends not only on the reader's knowledge base, but
also on the quality (i.e., accuracy and completeness) of the text-based
comprehension (Koda, 2005). Moreover, Grabe (2009) states that to
achieve an effective integration of text-based with situation-based
representations, skilled readers use strategies to engage their personal
needs and goals when reading a text. For example, while reading a
difficult text, skilled readers employ multiple strategies, often
concurrently, such as reflecting on the content, making inferences to
close gaps, and interpreting the text (Grabe). Unfortunately, because of
the language issues that L2 readers face, even those who are fluent,
strategic readers in their L1 do not necessarily transfer L1 strategies
to the L2 reading context (Clarke, 1980; Heeney, 2005; Macaro &
Erler, 2008; Shih, 1992). ESL students often complain that applying what
they read to academic tasks is one of their most difficult tasks
(Fraser, 1989). Thus teaching students strategies to bridge the gap
between constructing a text-based understanding of text and developing
an interpretative situation-based understanding is a worthy
instructional goal in the EAP reading class.
Direct Strategy Instruction
Strategies are deliberate, controlled, selected actions that
readers employ to achieve comprehension goals effectively and
efficiently (Allen, 2003; Carrell, Gajdusek, & Wise, 1998; Heeney,
2005; Macaro & Erler, 2008; Zhang, 2007). Cognitive reading
strategies include actions such as paraphrasing, summarizing,
elaborating, inferencing, and questioning to enhance comprehension
(Allen; Grabe, 2009; Heeney; Shih, 1992; Yang, 2006). Implicated in this
strategy use is metacognitive knowledge or readers' awareness of
the strategic choices available and their deliberate use of one or more
strategies to attend to comprehension breakdowns as well as to check,
monitor, evaluate, revise, and select cognitive strategies (Salataci
& Akyel, 2002).
The two aspects of metacognition are knowledge and regulation
(Carrell et al., 1998). Knowledge incorporates knowing which strategies
are available, how to perform them, and why to choose a specific
strategy. Regulation is the control of the strategies and includes
planning, monitoring, testing, revising, and evaluating the
effectiveness of strategies (Carrell et al.). Skilled readers employ
cognitive strategies automatically, but they also engage in
metacognition they perceive reading as challenging (Clarke, 1980; Grabe,
2009; Koda, 2005). L2 students in academic settings are often given
readings that they find difficult. Reading instructors can develop
learners' metacognitive awareness, along with their repertoire of
cognitive strategies, to help students succeed in academic environments
(Clarke). Research (Fraser, 1989; Grabe, 2009; Koda) indicates that the
most effective approach to strategy training is direct teaching,
"the explicit explanation, demonstration, and practice of selected
strategies" (Fraser, p. 76).
RC-MAPS: Metacognitive Strategy Training Technique
RC-MAPS makes use of dual-purpose graphic organizers to guide
students first to demonstrate text-based comprehension and then to
develop and expand situation-based comprehension. Once students have
been introduced to and have practiced filling in scaffolded (i.e.,
partly completed by the teacher) graphic organizers that represent the
specific discourse structures of the texts (see Appendix A), they are
instructed to create the appropriate style of graphic organizer for a
text (see Jiang & Grabe, 2007, for more detail). Students who create
a graphic organizer themselves have been shown to perform better on
content-recall tasks than those who are given one generated by a
teacher.
A Text-Based Comprehension Lesson with RC-MAPS
Overall, text-based comprehension requires students to remember
propositions (i.e., ideas) and to attach them to new propositions as
they read (Grabe, 2009; Grabe & Stoller, 2002; Kintsch, 1998). If
students focus on decoding, they tend to take longer to read, and hence
they forget prior propositions (Grabe). RC-MAPS allows students to read
and write at their own pace to create a written record to which to
refer. Using RC-MAPS, students graphically organize the macro- and
micro-propositions (main ideas, supporting details, etc.) put forth by
the author, both during and after reading the text. They are encouraged
to paraphrase the original text and to summarize it in point form in
their graphic organizers. Having such a written record helps them to
integrate and recall information and ideas.
The following lesson has been used with advanced EAP reading
classes. It can easily be modified for lower levels by choosing a
level-appropriate text (e.g., instead of a 5-paragraph expository text,
beginners could use an 8-10sentence paragraph). When introducing RC-MAPS
for the first time, the reading should be at or slightly below
students' proficiency level, as
limited language proficiency appears to exert a powerful effect on
the behaviours utilized by readers ... [and] ... limited control over
the language "short circuits" the good reader's system,
causing him/her to revert to poor reader strategies when confronted with
a difficult ... task in the second language. (Clarke, 1980, p. 206)
In addition, the text should be on a topic previously studied to
ensure appropriate background knowledge. Last, students should have been
taught and should have practiced how to identify and highlight the main
ideas and supporting details, as well as how to make paraphrased,
point-form marginal notes.
As the topic of this reading is homeopathy (see Appendix B),
students' background knowledge can be activated with a class
discussion on current medical issues and trends. For example, the
instructor might ask, "What do you know about H1N1? SARS? The
common cold? How do doctors treat these illnesses? How do people treat
themselves? What are the treatment options you are aware of, both here
and in your home countries?" After the prereading discussion, the
instructor should distribute the reading and ask students to preview the
text for the main topic and purpose of the text; this should be followed
by a short discussion. This initial reading activity can also be given
as homework in the preceding class.
The students' next task is to read the text more carefully and
to underline or highlight the main ideas and supporting details in each
paragraph. This could also have been done as a homework assignment. In
order to monitor and assess the students' text-based comprehension
of the text before they use their RC-MAPS, instructors may ask students
to work in pairs or small groups to compare and discuss the macro- and
micro-propositions that each student identified in the text while
creating paraphrased marginal notes for each paragraph. This follow-up
activity follows Vygotsky's (1978) Zone of Proximal Development
(ZPD) theory in which non-experts can learn from interactions with other
non-experts. As well, the teacher can circulate, helping groups to reach
consensus on comprehension problems; this extension activity also places
students in the ZPD because of the interaction with an expert
(Vygotsky). Finally, students' comprehension and highlighting can
be assessed by the teacher with each pair/group or as a class, using the
visual of a "correctly" highlighted text for comparison and
discussion. The collaborative discussion of the highlighting also serves
as a means to teach the students to monitor their own understanding of
the hierarchical structure of information in the text.
Next, students are instructed to use their marginal notes and
highlighting to create a text-based RC-MAPS. Depending on how familiar
students are with graphic organizers, scaffolded versions may be used in
which students need to fill in only some of the missing information, as
other details are already present, thereby allowing more checks of their
hierarchal structure of text propositions. For example, some information
has been provided in the Chart Style graphic organizer (see Appendix A)
for the Homeopathy text. Finally, to demonstrate clearly text-based
comprehension and to provide academic task practice, students should
write a summary of the article based on their RC-MAPS. These summaries
may be peer-edited, collaboratively written, and/or submitted for
assessment and feedback.
RC-MAPS: Situational Comprehension Through Questioning
When students have constructed a text-based representation, the EAP
reading classroom can begin to focus on situation-based comprehension.
Once general information about questioning has been presented, students
are introduced to the specific RC-MAPS Questioning Strategy.
First, they are introduced to the concept of questioning texts and
how the reader's purpose and the academic task can affect the types
of questions readers want or need to ask (Day & Park, 2005; Grabe,
2009). Readers with varied purposes typically focus on and interpret
text information variably: the questions each asks and the answers to
them will differ (Grabe). To illustrate, one can imagine how differently
a house-hunter and a thief will read and interpret an MLA house-for-sale
listing; information that the house is at the end of a cul-de-sac could
be interpreted by the house-buyer as indicating privacy and quiet, but
by the thief as isolation and lack of traffic at night.
Similarly, academic tasks can require varied approaches. For
example, in a personal response task, the questions asked by the reader
will relate to opinions that are based on personal experiences as well
as on prior knowledge acquired from friends, family members, and the
media. In a between-text comparison task, however, there should be no
mention of personal experiences; the questions should relate only to the
texts in the task description. In experiences with RC-MAPS, personal
response and position papers have been found to be the best tasks for
introducing situational RC-MAPS because the students need only to look
at one text in conjunction with information that they already have.
Next, students need to be made aware of the RC-MAPS Questioning
Strategy: what questioning is, why it is used, and when. Questioning
written texts is located in cultural domains (Grabe, 2009; Zhang, 2007);
many ESL/EFL students come from cultures in which the Western notion of
questioning experts is not necessarily promoted. Therefore, questioning
rules must first be outlined by the teacher and then modeled on the
RC-MAPS to provide direction and scaffolding for the strategy. The
RC-MAPS Questioning Strategy consists of the two rules below, based on
instructors' experience.
Often ESL students will either give a personal response that does
not address specific information or ideas from the text, but only the
topic of the text based on their own knowledge; or they will discuss the
text with no reference to their own knowledge. Situation-based reading
comprehension and academic reading tasks that demand critical thinking
require both. RC-MAPS Questioning Strategy
RULE 1: The questions must directly relate to the specific content
of the text
* RULE 2: The answers must be in 2 parts: one that uses information
from the text and another that uses 'outside' information
* Outside source(s) are task-dependent; use your knowledge and/or
experiences for a personal response task, but use only another text for
a cross-text comparison.
A Situation-Based Comprehension Lesson with RC-MAPS
In order to provide scaffolding, students initially complete the
situation-based RC-MAPS with the teacher as a class, then in small
groups and/or pairs, and finally individually. The directions given to
students should highlight and reiterate the rules so that they adhere to
them in their questions and answers. First, students are asked to
display their text-based RC-MAPS. Then they are shown how to create a
space for questions for each paragraph on their RCMAPS. This is a simple
modification: if students have used an outline style, they should simply
make a margin on the left or right (see Appendix C). For charts, they
will need to add one more column on the left or right of the existing
chart (see Appendix D). With Mind Maps, students need add only a bubble
(in another shape or color) to the existing bubbles (see Appendix E).
Second, the RC-MAPS Questioning Strategy should be explicitly
presented to and modeled for students. Teachers explain the types of
questions and responses that are expected and appropriate for the set
task and topic, and they model their own cognitive processes of
questioning. Typically, instructors should read the first two sentences
aloud and pause, then begin to ask (again, aloud) the questions that are
applicable to the task. They should also answer the questions, making
sure to refer to both the text and their personal knowledge. Using the
first paragraph of the Homeopathy text (Appendix B), for example,
instructors might say, "The law of similars gives a small dose to
treat symptoms. Do I know of any other medical practice that is similar
to that? What about vaccines?" Students write the questions asked
on their RCMAPS next to the appropriate propositions (see Appendixes
C-E). Typically, teachers should model the first paragraph, and then the
second paragraph should be completed by the class with the
instructors' guidance. The teacher reads the first sentence or two
and then asks the class for a question. The suggestions given by the
students are discussed with the class: do they follow the two rules? If
so, the students write them on their RC-MAPS. If not, the class suggests
how to fix the proposed questions before they are recorded. In small
groups or pairs, students are then assigned the task of continuing to
create questions for the text. They discuss and create at least two
questions per paragraph and monitor that they answer them by referring
to the text and their own knowledge. Before the students begin to
question on their own, the teacher reiterates the two rules that need to
be followed. As students become more familiar and comfortable with
questioning, teachers' scaffolding can be gradually reduced so that
students can move independently from text-based to situational-based
comprehension activities.
Research in strategy instruction indicates that in addition,
evaluation of strategy use is important for its successful
implementation (Carrell et al., 1998; Grabe, 2009; Yang, 2002).
Evaluation is also a skill, so some focus on teaching students how to
evaluate their work independently is necessary. RC-MAPS assists in the
development of evaluation skills because there is a written record that
can be reviewed and discussed. Once the questions and answers have been
completed, they can be posted for the class to see, or groups can
exchange papers. Students are then given instructions to assess the
questions and answers: Do they follow the rules? If not, how do they
violate the rules, and how can they be fixed? These assessments and
suggestions should be shared with the class as a whole, with the teacher
providing additional input and explicit feedback.
The final task for students to demonstrate their situation-based
comprehension is to include some of their questions and answers in a
personal response paper. This is written using the RC-MAPS and submitted
for the teacher's assessment and feedback. For the rest of the
semester, the instructor should generally require that situational
RC-MAPS be handed in with all written assignments based on readings.
This gives the students regular practice and over time enhances the
fluency and skill (or efficiency and effectiveness) with which they can
apply questioning strategies to diverse texts and tasks in the future.
Conclusion
L2 reading is a complex, interactive, and integrative process
(Heeney, 2005; Kintsch, 1998; Koda, 2005; Urquhart & Weir, 1998).
Academic reading often requires more than a text-based understanding
because students are asked to apply a text to diverse tasks (Heeney;
Shih, 2002; Yang, 2006; Zhang, 2007). Research has demonstrated not only
that metacognitive strategy instruction improves reading comprehension
(Allen, 2003; Carrell et al., 1998; Fraser, 1989, 1999; Grabe, 2009;
Heeney; Macaro & Erler, 2008; Yang, 2002, 2006; Zhang), but that
there is a best practice for teachers to follow, namely, using a
direct-strategy teaching approach that explicitly focuses on what the
strategy is, why it is important, how it is used, when and where it is
applied, and how it is evaluated (Allen; Carrell et al.; Grabe; Heeney;
Shih, 1992; Yang, 2002, 2006; Zhang). Although research has given
teachers direction regarding the approach to use when providing strategy
instruction in their classrooms, it has not provided them with the
required teaching tools to achieve this goal. RC-MAPS represents one
instructional technique that provides teachers with an easily modified
tool to assist them in developing the situation-based comprehension
skills of L2 readers in academic environments.
It is important to note that RC-MAPS was developed through
teachers' observation of a gap in students' reading
comprehension. Currently, RCMAPS has been implemented in only two
academic institutions, and the evidence is anecdotal and based only on
teachers' and students' observations and comments. Initial
responses have, however, been positive. In the classes in which we have
used RC-MAPS, we have noted that the quality of written responses to
readings has improved for most students. The students who use the
RC-MAPS with the Questioning Strategy rules tend to have task-specific
and appropriate questions that integrate the text with their own
knowledge, and they seem to have more confidence in their abilities to
complete academic tasks based on readings. These preliminary
observations demonstrate a need for further research on specific
pedagogical tools that focus on teaching critical questioning strategies
in the EAP reading class. Finally, we recognize the need for empirical
research on the proposed RC-MAPS technique, and we welcome readers'
feedback.
The Authors
Angela Meyer Sterzik is a doctoral student in applied linguistics
at York University in Toronto. She has taught EFL, community ESL, EAP,
and TESL. Currently, she teaches EAP at Fanshawe College, and her
research interests include academic reading and writing, EAP, and
teacher training.
Carol Fraser is an associate professor at York University. She
teaches EAP and undergraduate and graduate courses in L2 language
learning and teaching, bilingualism, and L2-reading and writing skills.
She has published articles on curriculum/test development, reading and
writing instruction, the role of language knowledge in SLA, learning
vocabulary through reading, and the development of reading fluency in an
L2.
Appendix A: RC-MAPS: Text-Based Comprehension Options
1. Mind Map Styles (for description and classification texts as per
Jiang & Grabe's, 2007, suggestions).
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
2. Outline Style (For narrative and time-line texts (Jiang &
Grabe).
A.--
i. --
B. --
ii. --
iii.--
3. Chart Style (for cause-effect, problem-solution, description,
classification, and for-against texts, as per Jiang & Grabe, 2007)
Paragraph Main idea Support(s)
1 Homeopathy developed in 1700s-- Based on 3
didn't like current medical practices. principles: law of
similars, minimum
dose, and single
remedy.
2
3
4
5
Appendix B: Homeopathy
Homeopathy was developed in the 18th century by the German
physician Samuel Hahnemann because he was not happy with the most
commonly used medical treatments of the time. His theory of medicine was
based on three principles: the law of similars, the minimum dose, and
the single remedy. The law of similars came as a result of observation;
he noticed that he developed symptoms of malaria after taking a strong
dose of the malaria treatment quinine. This led him to believe that if a
large amount causes symptoms in a healthy person, then smaller amounts
could treat those same symptoms in an ill person.
Homeopathic medicine involves prescribing drugs that duplicate the
symptoms of an illness. A homeopathic doctor will prescribe a drug made
from plants, herbs, or other natural materials that would cause the same
symptoms the patient is suffering in a healthy person. The classic
recipe is one grain of the required herb mixed with 99 parts of milk
sugar. The solution is diluted further by the homeopathic doctor with
milk sugar until he reaches the 30th time.
Many scientists dispute the validity of homeopathic remedies, but
clinical trials have provided some empirical evidence that homeopathic
patients can show positive results. Practitioners and patients do not
care about the physiological mechanisms behind this phenomenon: they
simply care that it works for them.
Allopathic (conventional) medicine tends to attempt to create
effects that are different from a disease or an illness, and many
practitioners of allopathic medicine have rejected homeopathy as sham
treatment. However, not all conventional treatments work, so many people
accept homeopathy as a valid alternative.
Homeopathy is practiced worldwide, and the number of homeopaths has
increased in the US to approximately 3,000 in the late 1990s from fewer
than 200 in the 1970s. Homeopathy, like conventional medicine, has
empirical support, anecdotal evidence, and can cure ailments; it is a
valid course of treatment.
(Adapted from Frazier & Leeming, 2007)
Appendix C: Situational RC-MAP Outline Style
Questions and Answers Text Information
I.--
A.--
B.--
i.--
ii.--
iii.--
C.--
Appendix D: Situational RC-MAP Chart Style
Main Ideas Support
Homeopathy developed Based on 3 principles:
in 1700s--didn't like law of similars, minimum
current medical practices. dose, and single remedy.
Questions
1. Does one ingredient actually
fight all symptoms--some
diseases have many symptoms
(e.g., the common cold)
--
2. How has medicine has
changed since the 1700s?
3. Write the answers to all your questions from above.
1.--
2.--
3.--
4.--
5.--
6.--
7.--
8.--
Appendix E: Situational RC-MAP Mind Map Style
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
References
Allen, S. (2003). An analytic comparison of three models of reading
strategy instruction. IRAL, 41(4), 319-338.
Carrell, P.L., Gajdusek, L., & Wise, T. (1998). Metacognition
and EFL/ESL reading. Instructional Science, 26, 97-112.
Clarke, M.A. (1980). The short circuit hypothesis of ESL reading Or
when language competence interferes with reading performance. Modern
Language Journal, 64, 203-209.
Day, R.R., & Park, J. (2005). Developing reading comprehension
questions. Reading in a Foreign Language, 17(1), 60-73.
Fraser, C.A. (1989). Reading for independence. TESL Canada Journal,
6(2), 74-86.
Fraser, C.A. (1999). Lexical processing strategy use and vocabulary
learning in reading. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21,
225-241.
Fraser, C.A. (2004). Reading fluency in a second language. Canadian
Modern Language Review, 61, 135-160.
Frazier, L., & Leeming, S. (2007). Lecture ready 3: Strategies
for academic listening, note-taking, and discussion. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Grabe, W. (2004). Research on teaching reading. Annual Review of
Applied Linguistics, 24, 44-69.
Grabe, W. (2009). Reading in a second language. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Grabe, W., & Stoller, F. (2002). The nature of reading
abilities. In W. Grabe & F. Stoller (Eds.), Teaching and researching
reading (pp. 9-39). London, UK: Pearson.
Heeney, M. (2005). Collaborative awareness reading training (CART):
Student and teacher perceptions of the reciprocal teaching approach.
Unpublished master's thesis, York University.
Jiang, X., & Grabe, W. (2007). Graphic organizers in reading
instruction: Research findings and issues. Reading in a Foreign
Language, 19(1), 34-55.
Kintsch, W. (1986). Learning from text. Cognition and Instruction,
3(2), 87-108.
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A
cross-linguistic approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Macaro, E., & Erler, L. (2008). Raising the achievement of
young-beginner readers of French through strategy instruction. Applied
Linguistics, 29(1), 90-119.
Salataci, R., & Akyel, A. (2002). Possible effects of strategy
instruction on L1 and L2 reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 14(1),
1-17.
Shih, M. (1992). Beyond comprehension exercises in the ESL academic
reading class. TESOL Quarterly, 26, 289-318.
Urquhart, S., & Weir, C. (1998). Reading in a second language:
Process, product, and practice (pp. 37-46). London: Longman.
Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher
psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Yang, Y. F. (2002).
Reassessing readers' comprehension monitoring. Reading in a Foreign
Language, 14(1), 18-42.
Yang, Y.F. (2006). Reading strategies or comprehension monitoring
strategies? Reading Psychology, 27(4), 313-343.
Zhang, L.J. (2007). Constructivist pedagogy in strategic reading
instruction: Exploring pathways to learner development in the English as
a second language (ESL) classroom. Instructional Science, 36(2), 89-116.