Is the aboriginal landscape sentient? Animism, the new animism and the Warlpiri.
Peterson, Nicolas
It is increasingly common for anthropologists writing about
Aboriginal people's relationships to land to use phrases like
'the sentient landscape' (eg, Biddle 2007:13; Poirier 2005:10,
57, 120,153; Povinelli 1993:139, 1995: 133; Rose 1996: 7) or the
'land is sentient' (Povinelli 1995:507). This may be partly
influenced by the renewed interest in animism among anthropologists more
generally (Bird-David 1999; Clammer, Poirier and Schwimmer 2004; Descola
1992; Viveiros de Castro 1998) which in turn is not unrelated to the
rise of interest, outside anthropology, in the relations between humans
and material objects in science and technology studies, and actor
network theory. These latter studies, of which Bruno Latour is a key
exponent, ascribe agency, but not intention, to material objects (eg,
Latour 2005). Another group interested in animism, is also outside
anthropology, and sometimes they refer to themselves as the new animists
(Harvey 2005). These new animists draw directly on a wide body of
ethnographic literature, some of which refers specifically to central
Australia.
The new animists' use of anthropological writings raises at
least two questions: how accurate are the anthropological accounts they
draw on in translating the views and understandings of the people they
are working with; and whether there is an unwitting collusion between
the anthropologists and the new animists. While the anthropologists do
not refer to the new animists' work, some of them seem to derive
inspiration from common influences such as the deep ecology movement,
and other anti-modernist views.
Here I want to set out these new animist views and then consider
how they relate to central Australian and Warlpiri ethnography. First,
however, I will provide a few examples of the anthropological usages in
respect of Australia.
THE SENTIENT LANDSCAPE
What is meant by phrases like the 'sentient landscape' is
often unclear and is variable between authors. Jennifer Biddle uses the
phrase in writing about Warlpiri speakers:
Central Desert art commands our attention in explicit ways. An
energy emanates from contemporary artworks by women, a life force that
is irreducibly bodily, palpably visceral, mesmerising in its effects.
Such effects are not only the result of culturally distinctive
sensibilities but are being harnessed, honed even, by contemporary
painting techniques. As I explore, such bodily responses are crucial to
the intercultural work that these paintings perform.
In effect I am not saying anything that hasn't been said
countless times by Aboriginal people, as well as anthropologists and art
advisors: that country is sentient, alive, sensuous to those who can
recognise it and know it (Biddle 2007:12 -13).
No illumination is provided as to what is meant by country being
'sentient, alive, or sensuous' to selected humans, nor whether
this view implies an animated flora, fauna and/or topography, or whether
this should be understood simply as literary licence.
On the other hand Sylvie Poirier, writing about the neighbouring
Kukatja is explicit: 'When it is said that some rocks at Kutal are
the ears of the ancestors that dwell there, this should be understood in
a literal sense and not as a metaphor' (2005:153). Since rocks are
rocks and not ears although they might be the transformation of ears,
the implication of this statement, it seems, is that the Aboriginal
people believe that rocks can hear and are therefore animated.
Elizabeth Povinelli is clearer about the situation among the people
of Cox Peninsula west of Darwin when she writes: 'The everydayness
of their labor-action is swept within the suprahuman realm of a sentient
landscape populated with ancestors and totemic beings' (Povinelli
1993: 133). That is, when she refers to a sentient landscape, what she
is referring to is a landscape believed by the people to be populated by
ancestral spirits with human-like form that live among the rocks and
trees. However, in other places the phrase seems to have different
implications. Thus, 'Because the country is sentient, the ground,
for Belyuen Aborigines, is always potentially liable to act for its own
reasons' (1993:150).
This ambiguousness is a common feature of such writings, nowhere
more so than in the following, taken from Deborah Rose's book
Nourishing Terrains:
Country in Aboriginal English is not only a common noun but also a
proper noun. People talk about country in the same way that they
would talk about a person: they speak to country, sing to country,
visit country, worry about country, feel sorry for country, and
long for country. People say that country knows, hears, smells,
takes notice, takes care, is sorry or happy.... country is a living
entity with a yesterday, today and tomorrow, with a consciousness,
and a will toward life' (1996:7).
Consequently one may ask whether this implies an Indigenous animist
view or is it simply an impressionistic and metaphorically rich account
seeking to create a certain aura. If the country does hear and smell, is
it the soil, the flora, the fauna or all of them and is the 'will
toward life' something other than what is commonly understood when
plants and animals are spoken of as 'being alive'? There is
ample scope here for varied interpretations.
THE NEW ANIMISM
New animists are people who Graham Harvey, an academic at the Open
University in Britain, says, recognise that the world is full of
persons, only some of whom are human (2005:xi). It follows that there
are fish, bird, and plant persons, and even it seems stone persons
(Harvey 2005:102-106) and there could be kettle persons too in some
circumstances (2005:110-111). Indeed the March 2007 issue of the Journal
of the Royal Anthropological Institute (2007:230) carries a review of a
book titled, Do glaciers listen? which speaks of sentient glaciers
(Cruikshank 2005) echoing the title of Elizabeth Povinelli's paper,
'Do rocks listen?' (1995). Chapter a of Harvey's book is
devoted to Australia with an emphasis on the desert (2005).
Harvey makes a distinction between the old animists, of which more
later, and the new animists. According to him the new animists are
concerned with knowing how to behave appropriately towards persons who
are not human: that is, it is about respecting the living world. The new
animism refers to:
the widespread indigenous and increasingly popular 'alternative'
understanding that humans share this world with a wide range of
persons, only some of whom are human. While it may be important to
know whether one is encountering a person or an object, the really
significant question for animists of the 'new' kind is how persons
are to be treated or acted towards. Discussion of these discourses,
points of view, practices and possibilities aids attempts to
understand worldviews and lifeways that are different in various
ways from those typically inculcated and more or less taken for
granted in Western modernity (2005:xi-xii).
It will come as no surprise that people who do not share these new
animist views are considered not only to have the facts wrong but also
to carry assumptions that preserve colonialist and dualist views of the
world (Harvey 2005:xii).
According to Harvey, persons are:
... those with whom other persons interact with varying degrees of
reciprocity. Persons may be spoken with. Objects, by contrast, are
usually spoken about. Persons are volitional, relational, cultural
and social beings. They demonstrate intentionality and agency with
varying degrees of autonomy and freedom. That some persons look
like objects is of little more value to an understanding of animism
than the notion that some acts, characteristics, qualia and so on
may appear human-like to some observers. Neither material form nor
spiritual or mental faculties are definitive (except in the 'old
animism' where they are the problem). People become animists by
learning how to recognise persons and, far more important, how to
engage with them. The ubiquity of terms like respect and
reciprocity in animist discourse demonstrates that the key
identifier of a person is someone who responds to or initiates
approaches to other persons (2005:xvii).
For those not versed in the Australian literature on totemism, a
superficial knowledge of accounts of a foundation period, in which
ancestral beings are identified with human and nonhuman organic and
inorganic forms, could be seen to support the foregoing views. (1)
THE OLD ANIMISM
For Edward Tylor (1929(1): 425) 'animism' simply
designated the belief in the existence of spiritual beings, and thus
included 'belief in souls ... in controlling deities and
subordinate spirits ... [that were the focus of] some kind of active
worship' (1871 see 1929 (1): 427). Tylor found, from his survey of
the literature, that virtually all peoples had a belief in ghost-souls,
or spirits (1929 (1): 428-457). He goes on to say that 'Plants,
partaking with animals [in] the phenomena of life and death, health and
sickness, not unnaturally have some kind of soul ascribed to them'
(474) and among the simplest animistic religions the theory of the soul
extends to inanimate objects (477).
As Brian Morris points out (1987:100), in Tylor's view, ideas
about soul/spirits were not the outcome of irrational thinking, although
he did see animism and science as mutually exclusive, but as George
Stocking points out Tylor thought animism was fundamentally in error
(Stocking 1987: 192). Anthropologists like Nurit Bird-David, and
non-anthropological new animists like Harvey believe on the contrary
that it is Tylor who is in error.
ANTHROPOLOGICAL REVIVAL OF INTEREST IN ANIMISM
Nurit Bird-David's paper, 'Animism revisited: personhood,
environment and relational epistemology' (1999) not only stirred
the interest of some anthropologists, but also of Graham Harvey, who has
drawn upon it extensively. (2) In this paper, Bird-David reconfigures
animism in to what she calls a 'relational epistemology'.
Bird-David reports that the Nayaka of southern India, a group of less
than 70 people, who hunt, gather and trade in forest products (1999:
s72) have a belief in devaru. Devaru objectify sharing relationships
between Nayaka and other 'beings'. Thus a hill devaru
objectifies Nayaka relations with the hill, making known the
relationships between Nayaka and a particular hill (1999: s73). She goes
on to say:
Nayaka maintain social relationships with other [mainly inanimate]
beings not because, as Tylor holds, they a priori consider them
persons. As and when and because they engage in and maintain
relationships with other beings, they constitute them as kinds of
person: they make them "relatives" by sharing with them and thus
make them persons (1999:s73).
Bird-David expands upon the notion of relating to the environment:
If "cutting trees into parts" epitomizes the modernist
epistemology, "talking with trees", I argue, epitomizes Nayaka
animistic epistemology. "Talking" is shorthand for a two-way
responsive relatedness with a tree--rather than "speaking" one-way
to it, as if it could listen and understand. "Talking with" stands
for attentiveness to variances and invariances in behaviour and
response of things in states of relatedness and for getting to know
such things as they change through the vicissitudes over time of
the engagement with them. To "talk with a tree"--rather than "cut
it down"--is to perceive what it does as one acts towards it,
being aware concurrently of changes in oneself and the tree. It is
expecting response and responding, growing into mutual
responsiveness and, furthermore, possibly into mutual
responsibility (1999:s77).
She goes on to say that framing the environment relationally is not
the only way Nayaka know their environment but it does enjoy an
authoritative status, not just among them, she believes, but among
hunter-gatherers generally, with the apparent, single exception of
Indigenous Australians (1999:s78). Near the end of her paper she has
this to say:
Furthermore, relational epistemologies function in diverse contexts
where other epistemologies enjoy authority, including Western
contexts (to a much greater extent than the authoritative status of
science permits). When ... we animate the computers we use, the
plants we grow, and the cars we drive, we relationally frame them.
We learn what they do in relation to what we do, how they respond
to our behaviour, how they act towards us, what their situational
and emergent behaviour (rather than their constitutive matter) is.
As Nayaka get to know animated aspects of their environment, so we
get to know these animated things by focusing on our relatedness
with them within the confines of that relatedness from a relational
viewpoint (1999:s78).
It is an empirical question as to whether the Nayaka, or any other
group, has a modernist understanding of nature and the person, but I
find this account a little confusing, especially Bird-David's
attempt to clarify the nature of relational epistemologies by reference
to Western contexts. Nowhere in her paper does she give any
consideration as to whether the Nayaka are speaking figuratively or
metaphorically.
It is fascinating too that once again (see Murdock 1968:336)
Indigenous Australians appear to be an exception to a broad
generalisation about foragers, as far as Bird-David is
concerned. They are not listed by her as animists, yet they were
amongst the classic cases of the old animism because of their elaborate
totemic rituals, and they surely manifest what she calls relational
epistemologies or ontologies that are central to this new animism. (3)
Nurit Bird-David and Graham Harvey, like Sylvie Poirier, Jennifer
Biddle and Debbie Rose (1998), are not only concerned with deep ecology,
but with what they see as the problematic aspects of influential
accounts of Aboriginal religion. While the latter group are dissatisfied
with WEH Stanner's (1966) structuralist attempts to understand
Aboriginal religion on its own terms (Morphy 1988:243), Bird-David and
Harvey are also critical of anthropological accounts of Aboriginal
ontology such as that by Nancy Munn. In particular they are critical of
her paper, 'The transformation of subjects into objects'
(1970) in which she examines the way ancestral beings, in her
understanding of Warlpiri and Pitjantjatjara understandings, transform
themselves into features of the landscape through the processes of
metamorphosis, imprinting and externalisation (1970:142). Munn argues
that any feature or object created by an ancestor in this way is:
thought to contain something of [the ancestor] himself within it,
and the various creative modes all imply a consubstantial
relationship between the ancestor and his objectifications....The
number of distinctive ancestors is indefinite, and the objects they
create constitute, in effect, the total, non-sentient environment
as epitomized by the country itself (1970:142-143).
Harvey comments that Munn seems to find it difficult to
'escape seeing land, boards, paintings and so on as objects. It
seems easier', he says, ' to convert indigenous language into
the language of symbol and metaphor, in which a hill symbolises an
ancestor, a board is a sign of their presence and stories are
metaphorical' (2005:73). He then goes on to quote Bird-David as
saying, 'certainly the "native [Australian] experience"
is fundamentally rooted "in a view of the country and the material
world as animate entities"' (2005:74) preferring her
assertions about a part of the world in which she has not done research
to Munn's ethnographically based work.
In Munn's view what is being inherited via ancestral
transformation is not simply the moral order and an authority structure
associated with the superordinate beings that have come before, but the
a priori grounds upon which the possibilities of this order are built.
That is the fundamental:
mode of orientation to objects in which experience of self is
firmly anchored in objective forms incorporating moral constraints,
that is being transmitted down the generations ... in which symbols
of collectivity are constantly recharged with intimations of self
... How then shall we interpret the psychological grounds of this
orientational mode? An older Anthropology regarded orientations of
this type as indicative of a 'confusion of categories' or a failure
to distinguish the subjective from the objective, the self from the
object world. On the contrary, it should be apparent from the
present analysis that this orientation is grounded in the awareness
of subject-object distinctions (1970:157-158).
Thus Munn sees her analysis as rescuing Warlpiri and Pitjantjatjara
people from misconceived 19th century ideas about hunter-gatherers,
while Graham Harvey and Nurit Bird-David see her as separating them from
19th century insights, with her modernist dualisms. One might ignore the
criticism because it is based in assertion and lacking in evidence, but
for the spate of ethnographers who use phrases like the sentient
environment when writing about the Warlpiri and their close neighbours
(eg, Biddle 2007; Poirier 2005; Rose 1998).
None of the above recent writers engages with the work of Francesca
Merlan who although not writing about people in the Warlpiri region
provides a fascinating account of Mangarrayi people's relationship,
mainly with trees, in the Roper River area in a way that is similar to
the Warlpiri practice of identifying some individuals with particular
trees, but not as elaborated as among the Mangarrayi. The Mangarrayi
directly link trees, at localities made by particular creator figures,
to living members of patrilines of a particular semi-moiety (1982:147).
Merlan makes the L6vi-Straussian point that while a totemic creator
being may loosely be considered as a prototype of a species, people made
a clear distinction between the totemic creator and the existence of
instances of that species on the ground; these in turn must be
distinguished from the visible signs left by totemic creators'
figures at particular places (1982:164). People may greet a particular
named tree:
by announcing who has come and his relation to persons represented.
The junggayi [or ceremonial managers], however, often say that they
have come hungry, that they require a plentiful catch, hunt, or
harvest; they flatter warriwiyan [ancestral beings] by saying that
they are barraj-yirrag (unique mother's property), that is without
equal in plenty and potential. The junggayi often stroke or pat
trees representing people and may rub them with mud as part of the
procedure of asking for plenty and to ensure fertility of the
locality. Thus, the warriwiyan serve as a direct link to the
potential of localities at which they stand (1982:155)
If a named tree dies or is burnt, the named person, if living, is
responsible for paying his ceremonial manager, generally a sister's
son or mother's brother. It seems that it is probably some time
before the person's name is transferred to another tree (1982:156).
Merlan speaks of this practice as personalising rather than
individualizing, the landscape, commenting that:
It seems needlessly literal to ask whether trees and other objects
"are" people: both people and the objects are linked with locality
by the ... [totemic creator figure] that placed them in the social
order. The objects are treated like people in certain ways. Easily
perceptible natural objects are used by the Mangarrayi as a system
of expression conventionally correlated with a certain content, the
interconnected cultural ideas described above. This conventional
correlation of expression with content establishes a cultural order
in which it is meaningful for a member of the society to talk about
links between trees (or other objects) and persons, even to the
extent of saying that a certain tree (or any number of trees, and
other objects) "is" a certain person. Nothing belongs to the sign
system that is not culturally considered to represent an
expression/content correlation of the requisite kind. Thus there
are many available trees that do not (at least at any given moment)
belong to the system (1982:162).
Michael Jackson gives an extended account of damage to a tree that
was identified with the spirit of a number of Warlpiri men, but despite
his own phenomenological orientation, he did not understand the Warlpiri
to believe the tree was sentient, but rather related to the spirit of
the deceased men through the sharing of the life force between them that
had originated from the original founding ancestor (1995:137-155). (4)
Like Munn and Merlan, Jackson uses the language of symbolism,
representation and metaphor and not of sentience. These divergent views
call for a review of the Warlpiri evidence.
WARLPIRI WORLD VIEW
Although there is no doubt that now, and almost certainly in
classical times too, there was a diversity of beliefs, there is no
question that most, if not all Warlpiri believe that they are liable to
encounter non-corporeal persons in their interaction with the world. The
question is whether they believe that the flora, fauna and material
objects in their environment can be communicated with.
There are a number of spirits and forces in the Warlpiri world.
Pilirrpa/kuntu is the soul or life force; yiwirnngi is the conception
spirit or life force that is originally located in the environment;
kurruwalpa are invisible beings thought by Warlpiri to be like small
children, that live in big trees such as in bloodwoods, ghost-gums and
river-gums, who can kill people but who can also enter into the stomachs
of women and then turn into children. Manparrpa are ghosts of a dead
person. Jarnpa/Jilpirda is a man with special powers to make himself
invisible and equated with the Kurdaitcha. Kuuku is a term for bogeymen
and used mainly to scare children. Kinki is a monster or devil that is
often cannibalistic. All of these terms for spirits involve human-like
forms with the exception of the pilirrpa and life force (yiwirnngi). The
nature of this life force emerges from an entry in the Warlpiri
dictionary related to increasing species. (5)
Ngarrmirni means 'to perform a ceremony to increase goannas,
snakes, edible seeds or anything else', an operation that can be
carried out in one of two ways. The dictionary provides a text from a
Warlpiri person in relation to a common public way this can take place:
People who are owners for the country concerned, who are closely
related to it through their fathers and mothers they work on the
Dreaming which put down some food such as yams to make it
plentiful. They rub and rub and grind off coarse sand from those
small stones from the Dreaming, which they then scatter in the
direction of yam country, they throw it all over so that the yams
will grow up everywhere, so that they will develop under the
ground. That is how they increase them. Later when good heavy rain
falls, the burnt off ground will turn into a yam garden.
That is, life force (yanjarra, kuruwari or kurrunpa) (6) is seen as
a particle that comes from grinding two stones together, at least one of
which in this case is a metamorphosis of a yam ancestor, to produce a
dust that is then scattered to the four winds. (7) On the several
occasions I have seen this done the action was carried out in silence or
during conversational small talk. Sometimes such an increase action can
simply involve brushing a particular ancestral transformation with
ironwood leaves. As the above statement makes clear, nothing happens
until the right conditions come along, indicating that the particles
containing life-force have to be activated in some appropriate way. No
reference is made to exhortations, supplications, requests or the
singing of particular verses or chanting of particular formulae that
would suggest that a sentient yam or seed person was being addressed.
(8)
The second way in which such increase can be encouraged is through
the performance of a male only ritual that involves one or more men
decorated with down and dancing to the accompaniment of others singing.
At the climax of what is always a brief ritual act, although after an
extended period of preparation, celebrating an ancestor's actions
at a locality, down from the shoulder of the decorated man is brushed
off and said to infuse the surrounding environment with life-force. The
songs recount the actions of the ancestor at the time of the original
founding drama. There is no hint of worship or supplication in this
instance either.
On some occasions, and in some localities, older Warlpiri people
can be seen apparently addressing thin air. This is generally when
bringing an outsider to an area for the first time. On the several
occasions I have been present in such situations, when visiting places
that nobody had visited for many years, or approaching a dangerous
sacred site, the person speaking indicated that they were addressing
ancestral spirits and/or those of deceased people closely associated
with the locality as site owners (see also Rose 1996:71). On other
occasions, where there is deep water, the same practice is directed
towards rainbow serpents (warnayarra) conceived of as invisible spirit
beings that reside at the bottom of such bodies of water.
WARLPIRI SYMBOLIC ACTION AND METAPHOR
I will focus on two aspects of Warlpiri symbolic action and
metaphors as they evidence Warlpiri ontological, and cosmological
understandings. (9) A key point I wish to make is quite straightforward.
Symbolic activities manifest in ritual actions are usually surprisingly
transparent, although this is not always evident initially, and there
may be no interest or even, perhaps, awareness of this among
participants in the ritual, and certainly no ready acknowledgement by
them. By contrast statements about ancestral beings or those made in
songs, which are understood as the words of the ancestral beings, and
which can be taken by us as evidence of thinking about them, cannot
always be taken literally and are not nearly so transparent.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
As an example of symbolic action I will briefly look at a Warlpiri
ritual episode which makes manifest the connections between what takes
place and Warlpiri cosmology. It involves what Munn calls spacetime, by
which she means the nexus of relations produced out of the interaction
between participants in rites and some of the related spaces they create
and occupy (1996:449). These spacetime episodes illustrate, and are
evidence for, aspects of Warlpiri ideas about cosmic order that Munn has
explored in relation to iconography (1973).
The full sequence of Warlpiri boys' maturity rites takes place
over several days and involves two occasions when people sing from dusk
to dawn: in the first of these, the men, women, children and boys to be
circumcised--there are generally two--gather together on the fringe of
the main camp at dusk. The men sit facing east and behind them at a
distance of ten metres or so sit the women facing in the same direction.
Immediately behind the women is a windbreak and behind the windbreak is
a small fire, besides which crouch the boys to be circumcised, each
watched over by a sister's husband.
The men sing of the travels of a group of women from west to east
to a place where they held an ancestral circumcision ceremony. With each
verse women, including the mothers and sisters of the boys to be
circumcised, get up and datace forward in a hopping style to close to
the back row of seated men before returning to sit down. Each verse
lasts a minute or so and is followed by 2-3 minutes of good-humoured
conversation.
From time to time, the actual mothers of the two boys leave the
women and move round behind the windbreak and dance around the two
crouching boys for a few minutes before returning to join the other
women. At dawn the boys are taken off to a secluded place and everybody
else returns to the camp to sleep.
Throughout much of the night, the two boys covered in white down
have been required to remain in a sphinx-like crouching position, but
with their heads lowered. The small fire helps keep them warm, while
their two guardians ensure they remain completely still, allowing them
to stand up and stretch only occasionally. As dawn breaks the white down
on the crown of the boys' heads is removed and replaced with red.
Later, once the sun is up, the boys are covered in red ochre from head
to toe.
In her examination of Warlpiri iconography, Munn pays particular
attention to the circle, which she says can represent not only an
enclosure but can also represent a three-dimensional container. If the
circle is to indicate something enclosed, the nature of the enclosure
needs some other element within it to indicate the kind of enclosure. So
a circle with another inside it may represent a convex surface with
something on top of something else, or a concave surface like a nest. A
covering container is usually expressed with concentric rings or a
spiral. Thus, sons crouch all night, within circles traced in the sand
by their mothers' feet, suggesting foetuses. The colours also
suggest the boys are being represented as foetus-like. The down covering
their body is white, the colour associated with spirits, both conception
spirits and those of the deceased. As dawn breaks in the east, the sun
shines on the crown of their emerging heads, now red, as they begin
their journey into the world.
What is striking about this vignette is how iconic the spacetime
and performative aspects are on examination: they are rich, complex but
deeply grounded in everyday experience. However, that may not be so
evident during the performance, when so much else is going on, when
events are fleeting, seen by few, and always accompanied by singing. In
my experience, the participants are preoccupied with their dance
movements, song verses, immediate sensual experiences and bodily states,
and the great majority do not respond to a distanced overview or readily
adopt another's subject position. The embeddedness of these actions
in wider Warlpiri understandings is a lot more evident on reflection
than in participation. (10)
ANCESTRAL BEINGS
Henry Jakamarra talking about two ancestral kangaroo beings that
are his dreaming, and are associated with the introduction of male
initiation, tells the story of their journey across the southern Tanami
desert. At one point one of them drowns. The other one sees something:
'What is this sitting here?'
It was a type of little mouse, a little rat, somewhat related to a
mouse but slightly different, called wulyu-wulyu. He approached it and
picked it up. He looked at it closely.
'Ah, it has two ears, two ears and it has a tail, it has two
feet, and it has a tail.' He looked at it carefully. In particular
he looked at its tail.
'What will I do with this creature? I'll take it with me,
I'll take it along.' He carried it to Mulyu, he carried it to
Mulyu. There he remade it, he remade it completely. He made it into a
kangaroo. Now that he was a kangaroo, he started to hop about. Where did
he go? He went away.'....(Napaljarri and Cataldi 1994:47)
The transformation of this small animal, recognisably like the
surviving kangaroo, with its two ears, two feet and a tail closely
inspected, is clearly a metaphorical account of making a boy into a man.
This is suggested by the association of this story with initiation, a
little animal is made into a big animal like the protagonist, and later
on the two together sing the initiation songs to make others into young
men (see 1994:51).
It is a very common feature of song cycles, which are seen to be an
ancestral inheritance of songs originally sung by the ancestral beings
during their foundation travels, that many of the verses use the first
person singular. Thus as the ancestral party of women sung about in the
initiation ceremony crosses the track of the above two kangaroos much
further south, the men sing:
Yilimintirrirna karri
Two legs--1SG subj. stand
Wawirrirna parnkaparnka
Red kangaroo-1SGsubj. run-redup.
I, red kangaroo, stand up on my two legs and run off (11)
No one would suggest that the singers think they are literally
ancestral beings, any more than Bororo literally think they are red
parrots (Smith 1972). Indeed, during the course of this, and many other
song cycles, the singers adopt several different subject positions, the
complexity of which affirms the highly intellectual nature of Warlpiri
ontological thinking. Clearly the use of this kind of language is
expressive and may evoke complex feelings and identifications in some
individuals in particular because the songs link place, ancestral
actions, and often with known individuals, they may create the sense of
the landscape being almost alive, yet the ancestral beings and the
stories about them are spoken of in resolutely human terms.
CONCLUSION
This brings us to the heart of the issues: do these beliefs and
practices constitute animism? While it is potentially an interesting
analytical approach to conceptualise things as possible persons, as the
new animists do, because it may be helpful in seeing what would not have
been seen otherwise, the new animists' accounts of the
relationships between people and things lose subtlety, and introduce
misleading speculation. It is significant that their ideas are almost
always invoked in the context of a ritual or in reference to one. Even
if people speak to trees, it is the social relations between people that
are the bedrock of ritual, not the ritually engendered relationships in
the special circumstances of particular ritual enactments. One cannot
leap from these highly specific circumstances of particular ritual
enactments to the everyday ontological significance of trees acquiring
personhood.
So what may be an analytically useful strategy should not lead to
presumptions about people's ontology.
Nobody today is writing about a primitive mentality but some people
are asserting things about Aboriginal people's perception of the
world that are unsupported by ethnographic evidence. We know that humans
go in for believing, and often find each other's beliefs strange if
not unbelievable, but the evidence that normatively Warlpiri, or other
desert Aboriginal people believe or believed that the world is full of
persons, only some of whom are humans or that plants and trees have
sensibilities, is lacking.
It is worth recalling what Graham Harvey (2005:xvii) says that
people holding animist views mean by persons:
... those with whom other persons interact with varying degrees of
reciprocity. Persons may be spoken with. Objects, by contrast, are
usually spoken about. Persons are volitional, relational, cultural
and social beings. They demonstrate intentionality and agency with
varying degrees of autonomy and freedom. That some persons look
like objects is of little more value to an understanding of animism
than the notion that some acts, characteristics, qualia and so on
may appear human-like to some observers.
Writing of the Warlpiri view of the desert landscape as sentient
easily leads to a confusion between their perception of the landscape as
being occupied by the spirits of human ancestors and other human-like
spirit beings, and the understanding that they hold animistic beliefs
about plants, animals and inanimate objects. It fails to reflect the
complexity of their highly intellectual and richly metaphorical
ontology, replacing it with an overly literal 'relational'
ontology.
As Durkheim long ago perceived Aboriginal practice does not make
the totemic animal sacred, but only objects like sacred boards and other
things that stand for the totemic ancestors, which are at the centre of
religious life. Their conception of life force has a lot more in common
with the idea of sperm than it does with a world filled with
sperm-persons. Deep ecology and other anti-modernist views seem
influential here and until we have some substantive evidential base for
Warlpiri, or other Aboriginal, animist views, what we know points in the
direction of a variety of ancestor oriented religion. (12) As to the new
animism, where it is not the old animism, it seems to be either the
product of a substantial, if well meaning, empathetic muddle in the
Australian context, or confusing literary licence.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This is a modified version of the Joel Kahn Annual Anthropology
Lecture given at La Trobe University in May 2009. An early version was
presented at the ANU Joint Staff seminar. I would like to thank Long
Paddy Jakamarra White, in particular, for his interest and persistence
in helping me gain some understanding of the richness and complexity of
Warlpiri religious life. I would also like to thank Georgia Curran, who
worked with myself, Mary Laughren and Stephen Wild on recording,
transcribing and translating Warlpiri song cycles with assistance from
many Warlpiri people, but especially Jeannie Nungarrayi Egan and Thomas
Jangala Rice on an Australian Research Council Linkage grant LP0560567
in which our partners were the Central Land Council and the Janganpa
Association. I have also benefited from discussions with John Carty,
Chris Gregory, Howard Morphy, Mary Laughren, and Ben Smith. I would like
to thank the people at the lecture and the seminar for their comments. I
am especially indebted to Don Gardner and Michael Houseman for close and
challenging readings, from very different perspectives, and also to John
Morton and David Nash for helpful comments. The reports of three engaged
reviewers of this paper have strengthened it.
REFERENCES
BIDDLE, J. 2007. Breasts, Bodies, Canvas: Central Desert Art as
Experience. Sydney: UNSW Press.
BIRD-DAVID, N. 1999. Animism revisited: personhood, environment and
relational epistemology. Current Anthropology 40 (Supplement--Special
Issue: Culture. A second chance?): s67-s91.
BRADLEY, J. (1996) Li- Anthawirriyarra, People of the Sea: Yanyuwa
Relations with their Maritime Environment. PhD. Thesis. Northern
Territory University.
CLAMMER, J., S. POIRIER, and E. SCHWIMMER, (ed). 2004. Figured
Worlds: Ontological Obstacles in Intercultural Relations. Toronto:
Toronto University Press.
CRUIKSHANK, J. 2005. Do Glaciers Listen? Local Knowledge, Colonial
Encounters, and Social Imagination. Toronto: University of British
Columbia Press. (Reviewed in JRAI 13(1): 230-1).
DESCOLA, P. 1996. Constructing natures: symbolic ecology and social
practice. In Nature and Society: Anthropological Perspectives (eds) P.
Descola and G. Palsson. London: Routledge. Pp. 82-102.
1992. Societies of Nature and the Nature of Society. In
Conceptualizing Society (ed) A, Kuper. London: Routledge. Pp. 107-126.
HARVEY, G. 2005. Animism: Respecting the Living World. London:
Hurst.
JACKSON, M. 1995. At Home in the World. Durham: Duke University
Press.
LATOUR, B. 2005. Reassembling the Social: an Introduction to
Actor-Network-Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
MERLAN, F. 1982. A Mangarrayi representational system: environment
and cultural symbolization in northern Australia. American Ethnologist 9(1): 145-166.
MORPHY, H. 1994. The interpretation of ritual: reflections from
film on anthropological method. Man 29(1): 11746.
1988. The resurrection of the hydra: twenty-five years on research
of Aboriginal religion. In Social Anthropology and Australian Aboriginal
Studies: a Contemporary Overview (ed) R. Berndt and R. Tonkinson.
Canberra: Aboriginal Studies Press. Pp. 239-266.
MORRIS, B. 1987. Anthropological Studies of Religion: an
Introductory Text. Cambridge: CLIP.
MOUNTFORD, C. 1968. Winbaraku and the Myth of Jarapiri. Adelaide:
Rigby
MUNN, N.1996. Excluded spaces: the figure in the Australian
Aboriginal landscape. Critical Inquiry 22:446-465.
1973. Cosmic order in Walbiri iconography. In Primitive Art and
Society, (ed) A. Forge. London: Oxford University Press. Pp. 193-220.
1970. The transformation of subjects into objects in Walbiriand
Pitjantjatjara myth. In Australian Aboriginal Anthropology (ed) R.
Berndt. Perth: University of Western Australia Press. Pp. 141-163.
MURDOCK, G. 1968 Discussion: are hunter-gatherers a cultural type?
In Man the Hunter (eds) R. Lee and I. DeVore. Chicago: Chicago
University Press. Pp.335-339.
NAPALJARRI, P and L. CATALDI, 1994. Yimikirli: Warlpiri Dreamings
and Histories. Collected and translated by Peggy Rockman Napaljarri and
Lee Cataldi. San Francisco: HarperCollins.
POIRIER, S. 2005. A World of Relationships: Itineraries, Dreams and
Events in the Australian Western Desert. Toronto: Toronto University
Press.
POVINELLI, E. 1995. Do rocks listen? The cultural politics of
apprehending Australian Aboriginal labor. American Anthropologist 97(3);
505-519.
1993. Labor's Lot. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
ROSE, D. 1998. Totemism, regions, and co-management in Aboriginal
Australia. On the web at:
<http://dlc.dlib.indiana.edu/archive/00000145/00/rose.pdf> 25th
April 2007.
1996. Nourishing Terrains: Australian Aboriginal Views of Landscape
and Wilderness. Canberra: Australian Heritage Commission.
SMITH, J. 1972. I am a parrot (red). History of Religions
11(4):391-413.
STANNER, W.E.H. 1966. On Aboriginal Religion. Sydney: Oceania
Monograph.
STOCKING, G. 1987. Victorian Anthropology. New York: The Free
Press.
STREHLOW, T.G.H. 1947. Aranda traditions. Melbourne: Melbourne
University Press.
TANNER, A. 1979. Bringing Home Animals: Religious Ideology and Mode
of Production of the Mistassini Cree Hunters. London: Hurst. Social and
Economic Studies No 23.
TYLOR, E. 1929. Primitive Culture, 2 vols. Fifth edition. London:
John Murray.
VIVEIROS DE CASTRO, E. 1999. Comment. On Bird-David's paper,
Animism revisited: personhood, environment and relational epistemology.
Current Anthropology 40 (Supplement--Special Issue: Culture. A second
chance?): s79-s80.
1998. Cosmological deixis and Amerindian perspectivism. JRAI
4(3):469-488.
Nicolas Peterson
Australian National University
NOTES
(1.) However, it is clear in many cases that not only were these
ancestral beings humans first and foremost, even if endowed with shape
changing powers (eg. see Rose 1996: 29; Strehlow 1947:7-9), but as
Phillipe Descola has emphasised, the 'pure form' of totemism
in Australia, in which a distinction is made between species as a class
concept, from individual members of a species, is rare and distinct from
animic systems, as he calls them, although in his view animic systems
are most common in hunting societies (1996:94). The absence of large and
dangerous animals in Australia may relate to this difference.
(2.) The relatively recent discussion of animism in the South
American context around the concept of perspectivism is quite different
from the issues discussed in this paper. By perspectivism is meant,
viewing the world as inhabited by different sorts of subjects or
persons, human and non-human, which apprehend reality from distinct
points of view (see Viveiros De Castro 1998:469). This is not the old
animism but a new animism, which can be defined as an ontology that
postulates the social character of relations between humans and
non-humans (1998:473).
(3.) The issue here is whether animism is an ontology concerned
with being and conceptions of reality, rather than an epistemology
concerned with knowledge and knowing as Eduardo Viveiros de Castro
points out in his comment on Bird-David's (1999:s79; see also
1998).
(4.) Interestingly at the important soakage of Yilkardi a dead tree
was identified with a deceased traditional owneron my last visit
although to my knowledge it was not identified with him thirty years
earlier when he had been alive.
(5.) All of the definitions in this paragraph are taken from the
Warlpiri dictionary.
(6.) For some evidence of this view see Mountford 1968:73.
(7.) Writing of the Arrernte/Aranda, southeastern neighbours of the
Warlpiri, Strehlow (1947:17-18) calls these particles life-cells or
life-atoms.
(8.) On one occasion recently I witnessed a woman rubbing a stone
to increase witchetty grubs at a site for which her husband was the
senior owner (kirda), but which, because of infirmity, he was unable to
walk to.
(9.) This is a complex area and my limited knowledge of Warlpiri
certainly means that I am not in a position to provide a comprehensive
account of metaphor in Warlpiri language and life. Further there is the
question of how metaphors relate to individual perceptions of the world:
they can, of course, have nothing to do with them. Yet it would be a
radical step to say the core metaphors and symbols shed no light on
these issues.
(10.) Howard Morphy has addressed this issue of coherence in
respect of Yolngu understandings of their rituals. He comments that
participants are concentrating on reproducing the correct form, rather
than reading meaning into the actions (1994:137) and that the coherence
of ritual events is more apparent retrospectively for both
anthropologists and senior participants (1994:140). He also suggests
that the meaning of ritual episodes 'must always have varied on an
individual basis according to experience, personality, knowledge and so
on. In times of rapid social change, however, each generation's
experience is often radically different from the previous
generation's' (1994:138).
(11.) I thank Georgia Curran for this example derived from her
fieldwork during the ARC Linkage project on Warlpiri Songlines.
(12.) Cree animism (eg. see Tanner 1979) has been drawn on by
JohnBradley in an ethnographically unconvincing way in his PhD (1996)
thesis by suggesting a parallel between Cree hunters and their
relationships with caribou, and Yanyula and dugong.