Intraindividual variation among pregnant adolescents: a pilot study and conceptual discussion.
Blinn-Pike, Lynn M. ; Stenberg, Lori ; Thompson, Cecelia 等
Background
After many years of working with, researching, and reading survey
research about pregnant adolescents, the conclusion was reached that
samples of pregnant adolescents provide self-report data of questionable
psychometric value. First, they display very labile moods. Second, they
may experience cognitive-developmental changes due to the experience of
being pregnant (Blinn, 1988). Third, they tend to provide inaccurate
information about areas such as family or parental socioeconomic status,
and tend toward social desirability when it comes to self-report data on
sensitive information such as substance use, sexual behavior, and
delinquent behavior. An adolescent who is one month pregnant is quite
different in her thoughts, attitudes, and expectations from one who is
further along in her pregnancy, even by a few weeks. A pregnant
adolescent's responses at one time may be very different one hour,
day, week or month later. To report which trimester of pregnancy an
adolescent is in does not recognize changes that occur on a more
frequent basis. The purpose of the present pilot study was to illustrate
the lability of pregnant adolescents' moods which may impact on
both their decision-making processes and the psychometric properties of
the data they provide.
Researchers in the area of adolescent pregnancy have several unique
problems, and researchers who want to do short-term longitudinal work
and address changes over the course of the pregnancy have an even more
difficult task. Gaining access to samples of pregnant adolescents can be
difficult. Samples of pregnant adolescents are generally heterogeneous
in terms of gestational progress, life situation, and age of the
adolescents. Recognizing these sampling issues as unavoidable in this
area of research, it is imperative that researchers compensate by
reporting more detail about the gestational variation in the samples, at
least the months of the subjects' pregnancies. They also need to
more critically address the issues of the validity and reliability of
their data.
To confirm the conclusion that there was a lack of specificity in the
gestational and psychometric data provided on survey research with
pregnant adolescents, an informal content analysis was conducted using
30 research articles published in refereed journals from 1977 to the
present dealing with the measurement of psychosocial variables in
pregnant adolescents. All of the studies were selected because they used
survey methodologies. Two methodological patterns were revealed. First,
for understandable and practical reasons, all of the research involved
convenience samples with adolescents in various stages of pregnancy.
However, little specific information was provided about gestational
progress other than by trimesters. Common phrases used to describe the
pregnancy status of the adolescent samples were: "All of them were
past the first trimester." "All were in the second trimester or beyond." "All were in the end of their first trimester or
in the second trimester when they entered the study." "All
were at least four weeks from delivery." None of the authors
reported the mean and standard deviation of the pregnancy status of the
sample or recognized that there may be variations due to finer
distinctions in pregnancy status.
Second, none of the researchers reported the test-retest or
split-half reliabilities of the instruments used based on the pregnant
samples being studied. None addressed validity issues by including
measures of social desirability. Those authors who included information
on the reliability of the survey instruments they used reported
reliability coefficients previously determined with nonpregnant samples.
The instruments used were all well known for their application with
nonpregnant samples and included the Piers-Harris Self-Concept
Questionnaire (Piers & Harris, 1983). Rosenberg Self-Esteem
Questionnaire (Rosenberg, 1965), Offer Self-Image Questionnaire for
Adolescents (Offer, Ostrov, & Howard, 1982), State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970), Beck Depression
Inventory (Beck et al., 1979), and the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale
(Fitts, 1965).
The findings led to the present attempt to illustrate variations in
pregnant adolescents' moods. The intent was not to provide
generalizable findings about the moods of pregnant adolescents, but to
provide rich, qualitative data on a small sample of pregnant adolescents
in order to document the need to explore this area in greater depth in
future research.
Nesselroade (1991, p. 6) makes a distinction in developmental
research between studying intraindividual variation and intraindividual
change. Intraindividual variation is defined as "short-term and
often reversible changes in emotions," such as moods or states in
the state-trait distinction. Intraindividual change is defined as
long-term and irreversible changes often associated with development,
learning, and progressive organic changes. This is related to the trait
element of the state-trait distinction. Intraindividual variability
among pregnant adolescents was the specific focus of the present effort.
Intraindividual variability may be overlooked as a threat to the
reliability of much self-report survey data and has been virtually
ignored in research with pregnant adolescents. The reliability of survey
research with pregnant adolescents needs to be reexamined in light of
the increasing evidence that self-report data from this population are
influenced by intraindividual variations.
Specifically, the foci of this study were the affective tone,
contextuality, and lability of pregnant adolescents' moods over
time. Mood was used interchangeably with intraindividual variation and
was defined as a quickly changing, intensely felt, and wildly
fluctuating emotional experience (Bence, 1992). Lability and affective
tone have been the main variables addressed in the few research studies
that have been done on adolescent moods (Bence, 1990, 1992). The present
work was unique in that it added the dimension of contextuality, which
was defined as relating to three topics: their feelings about their
mothers, babies' fathers, and selves.
Traditionally, mood states have been viewed as easily influenced by
situational factors, whereas feelings about the self and others have
been considered more stable and enduring. This study questioned whether
pregnant adolescents' mood states may, at least temporarily,
influence how they feel about themselves and those around them. It also
questioned whether the influence of emotional highs or lows are
generalizable across their feelings toward the people around them. For
example, if they are in a very negative mood, does this feeling carry
over to their feelings toward themselves, their mothers, and their
babies' fathers?
LITERATURE REVIEW
It has come to be accepted as part of the popular culture that both
pregnant women and nonpregnant adolescents experience mood swings,
including extreme positive and negative emotional states. The labile
feelings of these two groups have been attributed to both psychological
and hormonal causes. Yet there has been no research on the variations in
moods among pregnant adolescents, the focus of this investigation. In
one of the few studies examining intraindividual change among pregnant
adolescents, Blinn (1988) examined how pregnant adolescents' views
of their future changed over the course of their pregnancies. She
reported that their expectations changed from the second to the third
trimester of pregnancy. Blinn asked that closer attention be paid to a
possible cognitive-developmental theory of adolescent decision making
during pregnancy.
Due to the fact that there is no research on the mood states of
pregnant adolescents, the literature was reviewed in two separate areas:
mood variations during adolescence, and during pregnancy. No research
has been done, and it is beyond the scope of this paper, to determine if
mood swings during adolescence compound "normal" mood swings
during pregnancy.
Intraindividual Variations During Adolescence
Although the "storm and stress" hypothesis of adolescence
is no longer strongly supported, adolescence may still be a time of
heightened emotional state (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987; Larson,
Csikszentmihalyi, & Graef, 1980). Adolescents appear to experience
more intense feelings than do adults (Bradburn 1969, Campbell 1981;
Diener, Sandvik, & Larsen, 1985). Larson, Csikszentmihalyi, and
Graef (1980) used electronic pagers to alert adolescents and adults to
record their emotional feelings at random times during the day. They
found that adolescents reported more occasions of both negative and
positive feelings.
According to Brooks-Gunn and Reiter (1990, p. 43), "Perhaps the
most commonly held belief about puberty is that it heralds increased
emotionality in the form of negative moods and swings in moods. While
not as intense or extreme as popular accounts would have us believe, on
average, increases in negative emotions do seem to occur between late
childhood and late adolescence. Whether mood swings increase as well is
a fascinating and virtually unexplored issue."
Research on adolescent moodiness should not be ignored because the
storm and stress view of adolescence has been under fire. Buchanan
(1991) stressed the importance of studying adolescent moods by
hypothesizing that the challenge to the storm and stress stereotype of
adolescence has been based on research on such behaviors as conflict
with parents and deviance, rather than on research examining moods. It
is possible that moods become more unstable at puberty, but that they do
not bring on crises in the adolescents who are experiencing them.
Intraindividual Variations During Pregnancy
It is important to understand mood variability during pregnancy
because there appear to be strong relationships between pregnancy mood
states and (a) intrapartum processes such as labor and delivery
complications (Perez, 1983), (b) intrapartum analgesia requirements
(Perez, 1983), and (c) physical and psychological postpartum adjustment
(Smith, Cubis, Brinsmead, & Levin, 1990). Likewise, the effects of
depressed maternal mood have been shown to lessen the ability of mothers
to elicit positive responses from their infants, and of infants to
contingently respond to their mothers (Zekoski, O'Hara, &
Willis, 1987).
Smith et al. (1990) investigated the relationships among mood
changes, obstetric experience, and hormonal changes in 97 primiparous women. Smith found that mood disturbances rose significantly during
pregnancy and peaked at birth, as did levels of plasma cortisol,
beta-endorphin, and corticotrophin-releasing hormones. He also found
that the women who had the highest mood disturbance scores at 28 weeks
gestation received significantly more pain relief during delivery.
Ballinger, Kay, Naylor, and Smith (1982) found that women who had
fewer negative moods during pregnancy also reported more positive mood
changes two to four days after delivery. They further stated that women
showing a positive mood change after delivery were distinct in both mood
and biochemistry during pregnancy, and this may help identify these
women prior to delivery. Buchanan (1991) traced women's emotional
states at three and eight months of pregnancy, and again at three and
eight months postpartum. High emotional lability was found to be
positively associated with postpartum depression.
Finally, Salovey and Birnbaum (1989) reported that the mood status of
ill persons in general may negatively influence their perceptions of
levels of control over their health status and ability to carry out
health-seeking behaviors. This may result in failure to follow
prescribed regimens and to retard recovery. Although the Salovey and
Birnbaum research did not involve pregnant women, it may be that the
negative moods experienced by some pregnant women influence health and
lifestyle behaviors which in turn affect the health and well-being of
their babies.
Given the critical nature of the decisions made by pregnant
adolescents, and the implications drawn from their self-report data, it
is important to not only chart their mood changes, but to explore the
context of these moods. In this study, in order to explore the
context-specific nature of mood states, fluctuations in pregnant
adolescents' feelings about their mothers, babies' fathers,
and selves were compared. The pregnant adolescents' feelings about
their mothers and babies' fathers were considered important because
they may be the people who are closely involved in the situation and who
have a substantial impact on decisions concerning pregnancy outcome,
marriage, education, and health-related behaviors.
In addition, pregnant adolescents' mothers and babies'
fathers were selected because of the conflicting findings concerning
their relationships with these two individuals. Early researchers
reported that an adolescent pregnancy worsens the mother-daughter
relationship (Crumidy & Jacobziner, 1966; Friedman, 1966). Others
have reported that it strengthens the relationship (Smith, 1975; Young,
Berkman, & Rehr, 1975). A number of research studies have shown that
the involvement of the babies' fathers during pregnancy improves
adolescent mothers' sense of competency, self-image, and attachment
to their babies (Cervera, 1991; Panzarine, 1983). On the other hand,
Giblin, Poland, and Sams (1987) reported that tangible and emotional
support from adolescent fathers was not related to the pleasure the
pregnancy provided for the adolescents. Pleasure with pregnancy was
positively associated with assistance from their mothers and
satisfaction with their living arrangements.
Little is known about the interaction between pregnant girls'
feelings toward their mothers and their babies' fathers. It is not
known if a negative relationship with one of them makes the pregnant
adolescent feel particularly vulnerable and overly dependent on the
other. Likewise it is not known if the affective tone of one
relationship generalizes to the other relationship.
Guiding Questions
The purpose here was to begin to illustrate and understand
intraindividual variations in pregnant adolescents. The following
questions guided this effort: What mood patterns (affective tone and
lability) are evident within the context of pregnant adolescents'
feelings about themselves, their mothers, and their babies' fathers
over time? How are their mood patterns concerning themselves, their
mothers, and their babies' fathers related? What tentative
hypotheses can be generated about interindividual variability during
adolescent pregnancy from examining multiple cases of intraindividual
variability?
METHOD
Participants
The participants consisted of 14 pregnant adolescents who volunteered
to keep weekly written diaries concerning their feelings about
themselves, their mothers, and their babies' fathers for six weeks.
They were all attending regular classes in their public high schools and
were asked to volunteer for the project by their classroom teachers. The
diaries were completed on a weekly schedule because there is evidence
that daily fluctuations in mood exhibit a seven-day cycle based on
social, psychological, and biological processes (see Larsen &
Kasimatis, 1990). All of the adolescents lived in Idaho and were each
paid ten dollars for their involvement.
All of the females were white with a mean age of 16 years (SD =
1.18). The girls' levels in school were: 9th grade (2), 10th grade
(3), 11th grade (5), and 12th grade (4). Nine of them were receiving at
least one form of public financial assistance such as food stamps or
AFDC. The mean number of months pregnant was 5.1 (SD = 1.28). Their
pregnancy status was: three months pregnant (2), four months (2), five
months (3), six months (6), and seven months (1).
Two were married, the others had never been married. Eight lived with
at least one of their parents, two lived with their husbands, one lived
with a sister, and three lived in foster or residential homes. Most of
them were lower or lower middle class as evidenced by the places of
employment and the educational levels of their parents. One father had
completed elementary school, six had completed high school, two had
undergraduate degrees, and two had other forms of vocational or
postsecondary training. Three females provided no information about
their fathers' level of education. Three mothers had completed
elementary school, seven had completed high school or the GED, one had
an undergraduate degree, and two had other forms of postsecondary
education. One girl did not provide information about her mother's
educational level.
Instrumentation
Bence (1990) stated that little is known about the developmental
aspects of mood variability among nonclinical adolescents because of
problems with the measurement of mood variability. Mood variation is
most often measured by calculating the standard deviation of mood
self-reports. The adolescents are often asked to rate their emotional
moods on a form of semantic differential or to select from a series of
adjective pairs. Measurement includes calculating the mean and standard
deviation of each adolescent's series of mood reports (see Larson
& Lampman-Petraitis, 1989 for a more detailed discussion).
Another technique for measuring rate of mood change includes spectral
analysis or Fast Fournier transformation of each adolescent's
time-ordered composite mood scores. This procedure uses sine and cosine waves of different frequencies to describe any cyclical phenomena that
may be occurring (see Larsen, 1987; Larsen & Kasimatis, 1990).
Although these procedures clearly trace the patterns of
adolescents' moods, due to the present interests in the qualitative
nature and contextuality of moods, a creative methodology was employed.
All 14 females were asked to write about their feelings toward their
mothers, babies' fathers, and selves on a weekly basis for six
weeks. Their teachers collected the diaries each week and mailed them to
the researchers. The adolescents were each given a form with the same
type of fill-in-the-blank question for each topic (mother, baby's
father, or self): "During the last week I thought |my mother, my
baby's father, or I~ was . . . because. . . ." In addition,
during the first week of participation, their teachers assisted them in
completing demographic information sheets.
Data Analysis
Four individuals read each diary and provided 18 ratings per diary.
They were asked to read each entry and rate their overall impression
without further training. They rated each of the three topics for each
of the six weeks on a five-part scale with one (very negative) to five
(very positive) reflecting the affective tone of each diary entry. Three
of the raters were faculty members in departments of Home Economics or
Child and Family Studies at universities in Idaho, Tennessee, and
Arkansas. One rater was a graduate student. Each rater worked
independently, and there was no training or discussion of the content of
the diaries prior to rating. Data analysis involved the calculation of
interrater reliabilities, descriptive statistics, and correlations.
RESULTS
The reliabilities for each topic (mother, baby's father, self)
were calculated for the four raters for each of the six weeks. The
Spearman Brown reliability coefficient for mother was .95; for
baby's father, .94; and for self, .90. The reliabilities were
considered high enough to justify reporting the average of the four
ratings as indicators of affective tone for each of the three topics in
the remainder of the results.
The means, standard deviations, and ranges of the raters' scores
were calculated for each subject, topic, and week. These are shown in
Table 1. The grand means for affective tone were 3.26 (SD = .83), 2.71
(SD = .81), and 2.38 (SD = .53) for mother, baby's father, and
self, TABULAR DATA OMITTED respectively. The means of the ranges of
ratings were 1.82 (SD = .94), 1.96 (SD = .92), and 1.89 (SD = .85) for
mother, baby's father, and self, respectively.
The ratings of the affective tone of the three topics (averaged
across raters and across weeks) were correlated to determine if they
were similar in their orderings. The correlations were -.02
(mothers/selves), .01 (babies' fathers/selves), and -.18
(babies' fathers/mothers). None of the Pearson correlations were
significant at the .05 level or high enough to discuss in terms of
practical significance. This was interpreted as indicating a lack of
relationship among the topics based on tonal ratings.
In order to have objective criteria with which to compare
adolescents' feelings, a scatter plot was drawn with each
adolescent's average tone on the horizontal axis and range on the
vertical axis. The distribution was divided into nine sectors based on
the observed clustering of data points. The neutral values for tone were
between 2.5 and 3.5. The moderate values for range were between 1.5 and
2.5. Values below the neutral level were labeled as negative, and those
above the neutral level were labeled as positive for tone. Values below
and above the moderate levels for range were labeled low and high,
respectively.
Table 2 shows how the participants were labeled based on their
location in the nine sectors of the scatter plot. They felt more
positive about their mothers than they did about themselves. Likewise,
their feelings toward their mothers and babies' fathers were more
variable than were their feelings toward themselves. Both the positive
feelings toward their mothers and the neutral feelings toward their
babies' fathers revealed high lability. Their self feelings were
negative and moderately stable.
The following verbatim quotations from the participants' diaries
are included to illustrate the findings reported above. Selected
examples are given for each of the three topics.
Mothers
Participant 4 illustrated one of the two general patterns shown for
feelings toward their mothers (positive tone/high range or neutral
tone/moderate range). She was in the positive/high category because she
felt positive about her mother (tone = 3.62), and yet was extremely
labile in her feelings (range = 3.00). Participant 4 was 17 years old,
was seven months pregnant, primiparous, single, resided in a residential
facility for pregnant adolescents, and had parents with a high school
education. The lability of her feelings can be seen in her descriptions
of her mother as a "bitch," "very helpful,"
"unfair," "best friend," and "greatest mother
in the world." The following are quotations from her diary which
addressed the question: "During the last week I thought my mother
was . . . because . . ."
Table 2
Distribution of Participants by Tone and Range of Feelings
Participants By Numbers
Tone/Range Mothers Babies' Fathers Selves
Negative/Low 5,8 2 1,3,7,11
Neutral/Low -- 6 10
Positive/Low 10,11 -- --
Negative/Moderate -- 1,3,5 2,4,5,6,9,12,14
Neutral/Moderate 6,9,12,14 4,11 8
Positive/Moderate 3 14 --
Negative/High 1 10 --
Neutral/High -- 7,8,9,13 13
Positive/High 2,4,7,13 12 --
Note: Numbers represent the identification of participants
A bitch because she was not agreeing with me about marriage. She
wouldn't let me get married and we fought about it. |week 1~
Very helpful because she backed me up on what I wanted and helped me
feel wanted. She was there when I needed her and she stayed around even
when I was unpleasant to be with. |week 2~
Sometimes she was unfair and sometimes she wasn't. We got along
good for a while and then it went down the dumps. She bought me baby
stuff and told me she wanted to meet my boyfriend. |week 3~
Like a best friend. She listened to me and understood what I was
trying to say. |week 4~
The greatest mother in the world. I thought I went into labor friday
and she took me to the doctor and stayed with me the whole time. When we
were at her house she watched and made sure I was comfortable and
feeling well. |week 5~
Table 2 shows that Participant 9 was included in the most frequent
categories for each of the three topics (mother = neutral/moderate,
father = neutral/high, self = negative/moderate.) Her feelings about her
mother are described first. Her feelings about her baby's father
and self are described under separate headings. She was neutral in her
feelings about her mother (3.10), and these feelings were moderately
variable (1.75). Participant 9 was 16 years old, married, experiencing
her second pregnancy, and six months pregnant. Her parents were better
educated than the other parents in the study. Her father had a
bachelor's degree and her mother had a master's degree.
Compared to Participant 4, Participant 9 was not as labile and intense
in her feelings about her mother. She described her as
"helpful," "lonely," and "nosey."
"During the last week I thought my mother was . . . because . .
."
Helpful because we just got a new apartment and she came over and
helped us move in. |week 2~
Lonely because I went to my grandma's for a family get-together
and she kept hanging on me and telling me how much she missed me and
that we should visit more. |week 4~
Nosey because she keeps asking is everything all right, are you okay
and how is everything. I think she's waiting for me to come running
back home and to say I was wrong. |week 5~
Babies' Fathers
Participant 8 was in the most frequent category of feelings toward
the babies' fathers. She felt neutral about her baby's father
(2.58) and was extremely labile (3.00). She was 17 years old, lived with
her father and stepmother, was single, pregnant seven months,
experiencing her second pregnancy, and had parents with a high school
education. The high lability of her feelings toward her baby's
father are reflected in her description of him as "very
supportive," "mature," "very insensitive,"
"understanding," and "incredibly irresponsible." The
following quotations show her feelings. "During the last week I
thought my baby's father was . . . because . . ."
I haven't been able to see him as much as I like so I have been
feeling neglected. But I know he needs to work. He's very
supportive, I understand and I'm grateful to have him. He's a
little irresponsible at times but I love him a lot. |week 1~
We have both matured a lot. Nine months ago he didn't have a job
and was living with friends. Now he's holding two jobs and has his
own place. Of course, he doesn't have a lot of time for us to see
each other and sometimes I feel a little neglected and fear we are going
to drift apart. And since we have decided to release the baby for
adoption, I am scared that after the baby's gone there will be
nothing to hold us together. I really hope that doesn't happen.
|week 2~
He has been very insensitive lately. We've made plans and he
always wants to be with his brother and friends and play. I'm not
really fond of his friend so I don't like to be around with him
when he is around. |week 3~
At the first of the week we broke it off. Sometimes I feel like
I'm giving up so much of my life because of this. I wanted to do so
much this year like drill team, prom, debate, and a lot of social
activities I'm missing. His life hasn't changed at all. Then
he seems like I have it so easy. Anyway we got back together Wednesday.
We were only broke up for a couple of days, but it was really
depressing. |week 4~
He's been a lot more understanding and caring so our
relationship is doing a lot better now. I hope it stays this way. I just
hope nothing happens to him with the law. If he went back to jail it
would definitely make my delivery harder if he wasn't with me.
|week 5~
He's incredibly irresponsible. We'll make plans and he
doesn't show up so I have to hunt him down and that always makes me
real mad and we end up in a fight. Tuesday we almost missed lamaze class
because of that. |week 6~
Participant 9 also was neutral (3.16) about her baby's father,
who was also her husband, and highly variable (3.00). She used words
such as "supportive," "mean," "helpful,"
"excited," "lazy," and "irritating" to
describe him. "During the last week I thought my baby's father
was . . . because . . ."
Supportive. He usually is but during the last week he's been
helping around the house. Also when I get angry and yell he doesn't
get upset, he just ignores it and goes on. |week 1~
Mean because he takes away all of the food I like to eat and gives me
plain food. |week 2~
Helpful. He helped with the work around the house so I wouldn't
be so tired all the time. Also he was concerned with my health and the
baby's health. |week 3~
Excited. He can't wait for the baby to be born so he can take it
places and show it to people. I really think he will be a good father to
the baby. |week 4~
Lazy because he didn't want to help get the housework done and
usually he helps a lot. But this week our house has looked like a mess.
|week 5~
Irritating because it seemed like everything he did made me want to
scream and get upset and walk out. Hopefully it will pass after I have
the baby. |week 6~
Selves
Participant 7 represented subjects who felt consistently negative
about themselves. The tone of her self-description was 1.47 and her
range was .58. She was 17 years old, single, lived with foster parents,
three months pregnant, primiparous, and had parents with less than a
high school education. She showed a fairly high level of stability in
describing herself as "tired," "lazy,"
"bitchy," "rude," and "under a lot of
stress." "During the last week I thought I was . . . because .
. ."
Being pushed to the limits because my job wanted me to work everyday
and I need to get my life organized but it is impossible. |week 1~
Always being tired and lazy because I felt like crap all week. I was
sick all day. |week 2~
I was being a bitch to everyone because I was sick all of the time.
|week 3~
I was being a bitch to everyone. I was in a bad mood all week and was
rude to everyone at work and at home. |week 4~
Too tired because I've been thinking about my real dad who is in
the hospital. |week 5~
Being under a lot of stress because my real dad was in the hospital
and he treated me like crap when I spent $78 to go to Utah to see him.
|week 6~
Participant 9 was negative in her feelings about herself (2.16) and
moderately labile (2.50). She had a moderately labile score because,
although there were some variations, her self-descriptions included
"cranky," "lazy," and "grumpy."
"During the last week I thought I was . . . because . . ."
Cranky because I haven't gotten enough sleep and any little
thing can make me upset or irritated. I realize the way I am acting but
I can't help the way I act. |week 1~
Lazy because moving made me tired so I was sitting around a lot which
made me feel like a bum. |week 2~
Grumpy because I was tired from my vitamins not working so I guess I
haven't been the easiest person to get along with. |week 3~
Growing up. I feel that I will be able to handle the responsibility
of having a baby and I feel I will be a good mother and so do my family
members and they are all excited too. |week 5~
CONCEPTUAL DISCUSSION
Although there is an established tradition of survey research with
pregnant adolescents, there is a serious limitation in surveying them at
one point in time. It is very difficult to obtain homogeneous samples in
terms of age, gestational progress, and life characteristics. The
heterogeneity of samples, coupled with pregnant adolescents' labile
emotional states, and tendencies toward social desirability, make
generalizability questionable. The topic of mood variability among
pregnant adolescents deserves considerable attention for two related
reasons, one practical and the other methodological. First, pregnant
adolescents are forced to make critical decisions related to pregnancy
outcome, marriage, employment, and family relationships at times when
they also are experiencing emotional mood swings. Second, self-report
data from pregnant adolescents that are used as the basis for public
policy decisions, theory development, and service delivery models may be
unreliable and of questionable validity. There is a need for much more
research and theory on adolescent mood states during pregnancy that will
help explain how the timing, intensity, and context of such moods
interact with adolescent decision making.
The main purpose of this pilot study was to illustrate intrapregnancy
variations in pregnant adolescents, an area without previous research or
a theoretical foundation. The results are preliminary and not
necessarily generalizable to a larger population of pregnant
adolescents. The sample represented primarily low to moderate income
white adolescents in the second trimester of pregnancy who were
generally not living in intact two-parent families. Research will be
needed with larger samples to determine if the patterns revealed here
hold true for pregnant adolescents of other races and socioeconomic
levels, as well as in different stages of pregnancy.
Causality cannot be determined from these findings if (1) events in
their lives or hormonal changes influenced their moods; (2) there was an
interaction between environmental and physiological factors, or (3)
without prospective data if these moods were due to the state of being
pregnant or more stable personality traits.
Nesselroade (1991), in discussing the measurement of change, makes a
strong case for the measurement of three samples: persons, variables,
and occasions. In the present study, the desire to sample six occasions
and gather qualitatively rich data from each adolescent necessitated
using a relatively small number of participants. Nesselroade (1991)
stated that developmental research requires more rather than fewer
occasions, other things being equal. He further stated that "it may
be as important or more important to increase the number of occasions as
the number of subjects when design constrains permit". To
capitalize on the strengths associated with multiple occasions without
influencing the biases associated with a narrow selection of subjects,
he suggested investigating intraindividual variation and change patterns
on multiple individuals. Nesselroade believes that the characteristics
of intraindividual variability at the individual level, such as means,
variances, and periodicities, can be the basis for studying both
intraindividual change and stable interindividual differences.
"Combining intraindividual variability and intraindividual change .
. . leads to a representation of the developing individual that accents
the variable and occasion sampling concerns". His recommendation
was followed here in terms of investigating intraindividual variation in
14 different individuals. Intraindividual change was not within the
scope of this study, but it is hoped that it will be addressed by future
researchers.
In terms of lability, it is clear that these adolescents were very
aware of their mood swings in relation to their mothers and babies'
fathers. Several of them used the word "mood" in their
diaries. Participant 13 described her moods in detail, as can be seen in
the following selected excerpts from her diary. "During the last
week I thought I was . . . because . . ."
Very emotional and lonely. I felt as if I was having second thoughts
but after a while I would return to being happy, but like a pattern I
would again turn to being depressed. |week 1~
Moody because everything that went wrong at all I cried over and when
things would go good I'd be excited and five minutes later I'd
be down again. |week 5~
Still really moody and a little depressed because my doctor changed
my due date again by two weeks! I don't know how far along I am!
|week 6~
In terms of contextuality and emotional tone, the results illustrated
that different patterns of feelings were held for different individuals
in the adolescents' lives. These patterns were positive and labile
feelings toward their mothers, moderate and labile feelings toward their
babies' fathers, and negative and stable feelings toward
themselves. Their feelings on each of the topics were not interrelated;
they were independent and context specific. Only two subjects (1 and 5)
had similar ratings across all three topics, and these were all
negative. On the other hand, six subjects (4, 7, 10, 11, 12, 14) had
feelings that were labeled as positive (mothers), neutral (babies'
fathers), and negative (selves) for the three topics.
Although in their self-descriptions several of the adolescents felt
that they treated everyone around them negatively, they did not
necessarily express negative feelings toward their mothers or
babies' fathers. For example, Participant 7 was extremely negative
in her self-ratings and yet she felt very positive about her mother and
neutral about her partner. Of the four females who felt negatively about
themselves, only one (Participant 1) felt negatively about her mother
and one (Participant 3) felt negatively about her partner.
Future Research
A large number of questions need to be addressed in future research:
Do mood swings during adolescence compound "normal" mood
swings during pregnancy? How do mood swings of pregnant adolescents
compare with those of pregnant adults? Will mood swings have worse
consequences for the pregnancy during adolescence because of differences
in levels of social support from others or because of differences in
cognitive development? What is the relationship between mood variability
and depressed mood state, and the potential consequences of each?
There is room for much creativity in terms of methodologies in
exploring these questions. According to Watson and Tellegren (1985),
positive and negative affectivity may be orthogonal; a person can be
simultaneously high on both. Future researchers could explore two
separate ratings of positive and negative emotional tone for the same
individual at a given time. Another variation may be to ask raters to
give global ratings of mood changes across time for each individual.
In addition, the participants themselves could be asked to complete
instruments measuring dispositional optimism (see Scheier & Carver,
1985), or meta-mood (see Mayer & Gascheke, 1988). The Mayer and
Gaschke Meta-Mood Experience Scale measures personal perceptions of
one's overall mood experiences. For example, one question asks if
the subject agrees that his or her mood is in agreement with the way the
world is. A kind of multimethod comparison could be made between scores
on these instruments and the assessments of affective tone provided by
the raters of the diaries. If the same methodology is followed as was
done here, other topics could be added to or substituted for mothers,
babies' fathers, and selves, such as feelings toward their future,
school, and babies. Finally, the individuals referred to in the diaries
could be asked to complete their own diaries, similar in design to those
completed by the pregnant adolescents, describing their feelings about
the girls over time.
Proposed Hypotheses
Tentative hypotheses resulting from this study that need to be tested
with larger samples include the following: First, pregnant
adolescents' feelings about and relationship with their
babies' fathers do not influence their feelings about their
mothers, or vice versa. Second, pregnant adolescents feel more
negatively about themselves than they do about their babies'
fathers or their mothers. Third, their negative self-images are more
stable than their feelings about their mothers or babies' fathers,
thereby making self-image data more reliable than data on their feelings
about the other two people.
Implications
If pregnant adolescents are generally as moody as those studied here,
there are major implications for the reliability of self-report data so
often gathered with this population. It is not sufficient, when studying
pregnant adolescents, to report the reliability coefficients of the
instruments used when these original values were based on nonpregnant
and/or nonadolescent samples. New reliability coefficients need to be
calculated and reported for the sample of pregnant adolescents currently
of interest. If homogeneous samples are not available, at least the mean
number of months pregnant and standard deviation need to be reported.
Number of months pregnant also might be considered as an independent
variable. Service-related and public policy decisions are often made
based on one-time, self-report survey data from pregnant adolescents.
These data must be viewed with a skeptical eye given the fact that the
adolescents might have been experiencing emotional mood swings at the
time of administration, thereby bringing into question the test-retest
reliability of their scores.
The practical implications for this research are related to helping
pregnant adolescents make effective decisions. Practitioners need to be
aware of (a) the lability and specific patterns of the moods of each of
the pregnant adolescents with whom they work, and (b) the
context-specific nature of these moods. Critical decisions made during
periods of elevated or depressed mood states may have negative and
irrevocable implications in the adolescents' future. Pregnant
adolescents need to be helped to chart their own personal mood patterns
and to understand how these moods can affect their perceptions of the
people and events in their lives, affecting their decision making.
REFERENCES
Ballinger, C. B., Kay, D., Naylor, G., & Smith, A. (1982). Some
biochemical findings during pregnancy and after delivery in relation to
mood change. Psychological Medicine, 12, 549-556.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. P., & Emery, G. (1979).
Cognitive theory for depression. New York: Guilford.
Bence, P. J. (1990, March). Mood variability: Detecting cyclicities
in the experience of mood during adolescence. Paper presented at the
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence, Atlanta,
GA.
Bence, P. J. (1992, March). Patterns of the experience of mood. Paper
presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on
Adolescence, Washington, D.C.
Blinn, L. M. (1988). Stability and change in pregnant
adolescents' perceptions of their future marriage and family life.
Family Perspective, 22, 255-268.
Bradburn, N. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being.
Chicago: Aldine.
Brooks, Gunn, J., & Reiter, E. O. (1990). The role of pubertal processes. In S. S. Feldman, & G. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold:
The developing adolescent (pp. 16-54). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Buchanan, C. M. (1991). Pubertal states in early adolescent girls:
Relations of moods, energy, and restlessness. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 11, 185-200.
Buchanan, T. (1991, April). Predicting change in postpartum
depression: An individual growth curve analysis. Paper presented at the
Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Seattle, WA.
Campbell, A. (1981). The sense of well-being in America: Recent
patterns and trends. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cervera, N. (1991). Unwed teenage pregnancy: Family relationships
with the father of the baby. Families in society: The Journal of
Contemporary Human Services, 72,(1), 29-36.
Crumidy, P. M., & Jacobziner, H. (1966). A study of young
unmarried mothers who kept their babies. American Journal of Public
Health, 56, 1242-1251.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and
reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 174, 526-536.
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Larsen, R. (1985). Age and sex effects
for emotional intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542-546.
Fitts, W. H. (1965). Tennessee Self-Concept Scale. Nashville:
Counselor Recordings and Tests.
Friedman, H. L. (1966). The mother-daughter relationship: Its
potential in treatment of young, unwed mothers. Social Casework, 47,
502-506.
Giblin, P., Poland, M. L., & Sams, B. A. (1987). Effects of
social supports on attitudes and health behaviors of pregnant
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8, 273-279.
Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of mood variability: A spectral
analytic approach to daily mood assessments. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 52, 1195-1204.
Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1990). Individual differences in
entrainment of mood to weekly calendar. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 58(1), 164-171.
Larson, R., & Lampman-Petraitis, C. (1989). Daily emotional
status associated with the onset of adolescence. Child Development, 60,
1250-1260.
Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1980). Mood
variability and the psychosocial adjustment of adolescents. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 9, 469-490.
Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. (1988). The experience and
meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
55, 102-111.
Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). Interindividual differences in
intraindividual change. In L. A. Collins, & J. L. Horn (Eds.), Best
methods for the analysis of change (pp. 92-106). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Offer, D., Ostrov, E., & Howard, K. I. (1982). The Offer
Self-Image Questionnaire for Adolescents: A Manual. Chicago: Michael
Reese Hospital.
Panzarine, S. (1983). Adolescent mothers: Perceived stressors, social
support, and coping during puerperium. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. University of Utah, Salt Lake City.
Perez, R. (1983). Effects of stress, social support and coping style
on adjustment to pregnancy among Hispanic women. Hispanic Journal of
Behavioral Sciences, 5, 141-161.
Piers, E. V., & Harris, D. B. (1983). The Piers-Harris
Children's Self-Concept Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological
Services.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Salovey, P., & Birnbaum, D. (1989). Influence of mood on
health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 539-551.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and
health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.
Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.
Sharp, V. (1980). Adolescence. In J. Bemorad (Ed.), Child development
in normality and psychopathology (pp. 174-218. New York: Brunner/Mazel).
Smith, E. W. (1975). The role of the grandmother in adolescent
pregnancy and parenting. Journal of School Health, 45, 278-283.
Smith, R., Cubis, J., Brinsmead, M., & Lewin, T. (1990). Mood
changes, obstetric experience and alterations in plasma cortisol,
beta-endorphin and corticotrophin-releasing hormone during pregnancy and
the puerperium. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, (1), 53-69.
Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1970). Manual for
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Watson, D., & Tellegren, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure
of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235.
Young, A., Berkman, B., & Rehr, H. (1975). Parental influence on
pregnant adolescents. Social Work, 20, 387-391.
Zekoski, E. M., O'Hara, M. W., & Wills, K. E. (1987). The
effects of maternal mood on mother-infant interaction. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 361-378.
Lori Stenberg, Assistant Professor, School of Home Economics,
University of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho 83843.
Cecelia Thompson, Associate Professor, College of Education,
University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, Arkansas 72701.