IDENTITY STATUS FORMULAE: GENERATING CONTINUOUS MEASURES OF THE IDENTITY STATUSES FROM MEASURES OF EXPLORATION AND COMMITMENT.
Schwartz, Seth J. ; Dunham, Richard M.
ABSTRACT
Mathematical formulae were devised for the purpose of generating
continuous measures of the four identity statuses from measures of
exploration and commitment. The formulae were consistent with the
conceptual definitions of the statuses. They were found to be effective
both in terms of generating continuous measures of the statuses and in
terms of deriving status assignments.
During the last thirty years, identity formation has been a worthy
focus of considerable theoretical and empirical research. In 1993,
Marcia estimated that more than 300 studies had been published on
identity. Most of this research drew on the formulation that Marcia
(1966) derived from the work of Erikson (1950). This formulation is
quite parsimonious and has been shown to possess adequate construct
validity (Waterman, 1988).
Marcia extracted two principal dimensions from Erikson's work,
exploration and commitment. Exploration signifies the search for a more
complete sense of self. Commitment is the act of deciding on a
particular set of goals, values, and beliefs to which one will adhere.
Traditionally, exploration and commitment have each been bifurcated into high and low levels, with the median score on each serving as the
dividing point. Marcia derived four identity statuses by juxtaposing
each level of exploration with each level of commitment. These statuses
are achievement, moratorium, foreclosure, and diffusion.
Achievement represents high levels of both exploration and
commitment. It is the state of having committed to specific identity
elements following a period of exploration. Achieved individuals tend to
be balanced thinkers (Marcia, 1980) and to be willing to explore further
if the situation requires it (Stephen, Fraser, & Marcia, 1992).
Moratorium is the state of active exploration without much
commitment. In developmental terms, it usually precedes the achieved
status (Waterman, 1988). Individuals in moratorium tend to be anxious
and depressed (Kidwell, Dunham, Bacho, Pastorino, & Portes, 1995),
and they have been shown to display more creative thought than do those
in the other statuses (Berman, Schwartz, Kurtines, & Berman, 2000).
Foreclosure represents a commitment formed without adequate prior
exploration. This commitment often stems from the expectations of
parents or other significant figures in the individual's life.
Foreclosure may be associated with smug self-satisfaction (Marcia, 1980)
and authoritarianism (Marcia, 1967).
Diffusion is the relative absence of exploration and commitment.
Diffused individuals are often apathetic and disinterested (Marcia,
1980). They may also be especially prone to academic difficulties
(Berzonsky, 1985).
Formulae are provided here for calculating continuous measures of
the four basic identity statuses from continuous measures of exploration
and commitment. Tests of the effectiveness of the derived identity
measures were accomplished by: (a) comparing two relationships, namely
that between those derived continuous measures and an independent
measure with that between existing continuous measures of identity
status and the same independent measure; and (b) comparing
categorization data based on the use of the formulae with data using
more usual methods of categorization, on a single instrument.
Classification of Instruments that Assess Identity Status
There are two general forms of identity measures, called here
direct (designed to measure identity status directly) and derived
(identity status is derived from measures of exploration and
commitment). In addition, there are two types of identity status
scaling, categorical and continuous. Table 1 presents several
instruments classified by mode of assessment and type of scaling. There
have been no derived continuous identity measures.
Categorical measures of identity status are commonly used. They
lend themselves to statistical analyses that are less powerful than
those that are possible with continuous measures. The validity of
categorical measures derived from continuous measures, such as measures
of exploration and commitment, may be diminished due to the inherent
uncertainty and ambiguity of setting thresholds and interpreting values
near those thresholds. Instruments that assign status directly, through
the subjective judgment of a human rater or coder, may also face the
reliability problems that characterize subjective judgments of complex
conditions.
In addition to allowing statistical analyses with potentially
greater power, continuous measures of identity status can be correlated
with variables from other constructs. Further, scores may indicate not
only identity status, but also how much of each status is manifested in
a given individual. Thus, continuous measures may facilitate the
detection of low-magnitude differences in identity status (Adams,
Bennion, & Huh, 1989; Bennion & Adams, 1986).
The measurement of identity status has most commonly involved the
structured interview. Structured interviews provide for the direct
categorical assignment of an overall identity status as well as status
within specific content areas (such as politics and occupation), which
are called domains (e.g., Grotevant & Cooper, 1981; Marcia &
Archer, 1993). As incidental steps in their use, structured interviews
may yield continuous measures of overall or domain-specific exploration
or commitment (Jackson, Dunham, & Kidwell, 1990), such as when
coders are instructed to assign each participant an exploration score
and a commitment score (both usually between 1 and 4) for each domain,
as a method of deriving status categorizations. The continuous measures
have, at times, also been used to assign identity status by domain.
Thus, derived categorical functioning may be superimposed on an
instrument designed for direct categorical measurement.
The Formulae
In light of the distinctions among identity status instruments, the
need for derived continuous measures is clear. Such measures would build
upon available continuous measures of exploration and commitment, but
would respect past theory, methodology, and findings with regard to
identity statuses. Therefore, the measures would have to be
demonstrably: (a) based on theoretically grounded definitions of the
statuses, and (b) plausible in terms of the outcomes of their usage (the
outcomes would need to be reasonable, in comparison with outcomes from
preexisting approaches, when conceived as continuous measures and also
when reduced to categorical status assignments).
What follows is a presentation of four formulae, one for each of
the basic identity statuses, together with a brief discussion of the
theoretical justification for the formulae. Two sets of calculations,
from two samples of identity data, are then presented to assess the
plausibility of the formulae in comparison with established approaches.
The scores generated by the formulae are compared with those obtained
from a direct continuous measure in terms of their relationships to a
third identity instrument, the Identity Style Inventory (Berzonsky,
1997), which has been shown to be related to both derived categorical
and direct continuous identity measures (Berzonsky, 1989; Schwartz,
1996).
Marcia presented exploration and commitment in a four-cell diagram
to account for the four identity statuses. Table 2 shows a similar
formulation, but the terms "manifest" and "less
manifest" have been used. This reflects the fact that questions
concerning how much exploration or commitment is necessary to cross the
threshold between identity statuses have not been systematically
addressed in the empirical literature. Each derived categorical
instrument has its own rules for status assignment. Implicitly, the
direct categorical instruments do also, in that they identify
exploration and commitment levels by domain, and then invite a coder or
interviewer to judge or assign the status (Jackson, Dunham, &
Kidwell, 1990). Presumably, exploration and commitment, or any given
status, may exist to some degree or intensity within the individual and
will be reflected in relevant ideation, perception, and behavior (e.g.,
cognition, stances or styles, self-ascriptions, information-seeking and
decision-making behavior, a nd imitation or conformity). When the
strength of exploration or commitment is low, the individual is taken to
fall into a less mature status. Low strength may be judged in relative
terms. It may fall below some threshold, perhaps a median, in the
distribution of exploration or commitment scores for a given sample or
population.
Much the same reasoning applies to the assignment of statuses.
Although individuals may differ in status from domain to domain, overall
assignment proceeds on the assumption that the behavior of an individual
is best reflected in a single status. The behaviors characteristic of
the other statuses are presumed to be less observable or less manifest.
When they are less manifest, it may be because they do not exist in the
repertory of the individual, because they cannot compete successfully
for expression with others that are stronger, or because of some other
suppressor process. Since developmental expectations would almost always
lead to the belief that the component behaviors of all four statuses are
established in the repertories for the ages in question--and because
they are frequently observed to be present in analyses by domain--it may
be assumed that less-manifest statuses have been suppressed or
inhibited. They have lost out to other, momentarily stronger tendencies,
or been influenced by the inhibitor y social prescriptions of
authoritative models.
The classes of instruments referred to in Table 1 either directly
assess the signs of the presence of a status or assess the signs of the
presence of exploration and commitment. Even the direct continuous
measures were created, and are used, with the assumption that
exploration and commitment are underlying orthogonal processes. As shown
in Table 3, the information in Table 2 may be recast to clarify the
underlying logical processes affecting definitions of the four identity
statuses.
In order to construct a set of equations to express those
conditions and relationships, three representations must be contrived.
The first is the representation of exploration and commitment in
quantitative terms. That, of course, is provided by the scores of any
derived continuous instrument. The second is the representation of
"and" as reflecting the relationships between exploration and
commitment for the four statuses. Third, the terms "manifest"
and "less manifest" must be represented.
Syntax provides "and" as the expression of the logical
operator "conjunction." That logical operator is expressed in
mathematical terms as addition or multiplication. In the present
research, the operator "and" was treated as reflecting a
process of multiplication rather than merely one of addition. The choice
of a multiplicative process rests on the assumption that, whereas
exploration and commitment may be independent and mathematically
orthogonal processes, they are interactive, and with regard to the
identity achievement status, they may be synergistic (e.g., Jessee,
Gruenfeld, & Waterman, 1995). Thus, because identity achievement
represents exploration and commitment, the identity achievement score
(A) may be construed as the product of the exploration (E) and
commitment (C) scores: A = E X C.
The third representation is of the terms "manifest" and
"less manifest." In the rationale for the formula for
achievement, the manifestations of exploration and commitment are
represented as whole, continuously distributed numbers. "Less
manifest" is, of course, some degree of negation of
"manifest." Following Freud (1905/1950) and others, negation
is the logical maneuver underlying suppression and repression of an
idea. In the mathematized psychology of Herbart (1866/1961), repression
is represented mathematically as inversion. As such, "less
exploration" or "less commitment" becomes the inverse, or
reciprocal, of exploration or commitment, that is 1/E or 1/C.
Putting the previous representations together yields the additional
necessary formulae. Moratorium symbolizes exploration in the relative
absence of commitment, with the moratorium score (M) being the product
of the exploration score and the inverse of the commitment score: M =
E/C. Similarly, foreclosure embodies the concept of commitment without
exploration, with the foreclosure score (F) equaling the commitment
score multiplied by the inverse of the exploration score: F = C/E.
Finally, identity diffusion represents the relative absence of
exploration and commitment; therefore, the diffusion score (D) would be
calculated as the product of the inverses of the exploration and
commitment scores: D = (1/E) x (1/C).
Having sketched the logic and theoretical base of the formulae, a
test of the comparability of the continuous status measures produced by
the formulae with other well-established procedures was conducted.
Berzonsky (1989) has provided findings on the relationship of a direct
continuous measure and his Identity Style Inventory, a measure that is
theoretically and empirically related to ego identity. The present
research investigated the relationship of a derived categorical measure,
which has been afforded derived continuous properties by the formulae,
and the same independent measure. The task was to determine whether
continuous measures based on exploration and commitment scores from a
derived categorical measure relate in theoretically and methodologically
sensible ways to the independent measure, in light of previous findings
with a direct continuous measure.
In addition, status assignments using the formulae should be
comparable to status assignments made with the same sample and measure
(the Ego Identity Process Questionnaire) using the conventional median
split technique offered by Balistreri, Busch-Rossnagel, and Geisinger
(1995). The effectiveness of the formulae was tested in this way.
Two studies were conducted in order to test the effectiveness of
the formulae in producing continuous identity status scores. Study 1
tested the formulae in the two ways outlined above. Study 2 replicated
Study 1 with a larger sample.
STUDY 1: METHOD
Participants
The participants for Study 1 were 113 Florida State University undergraduates (13 males, 100 females), between 18 and 22 years of age,
who were enrolled in introductory psychology courses. Participation in
the study partially fulfilled the research requirement for those
courses.
Measures
Three instruments were used: the Extended Objective Measure of Ego
Identity Status (EOM-EIS; Bennion & Adams, 1986), the Ego Identity
Process Questionnaire (EIPQ; Balistreri, Busch-Rossnagel, &
Geisinger, 1995), and the Identity Style Inventory (Berzonsky, 1997).
The EIPQ is a derived categorical measure; the EOM-EIS is a direct
continuous measure; and the Identity Style Inventory is a prominent
independent measure within the identity literature. The relationship of
the EIPQ and Identity Style Inventory was estimated on the basis of data
gathered for this report. The relationship of the EOM-EIS and Identity
Style Inventory was estimated from a study by Berzonsky (1989).
Ego identity. The EIPQ is an objective instrument that generates
scores for exploration and commitment, sums those scores across eight
domains, and uses median splits to categorize individuals into identity
statuses. It does not, however, generate continuous scores for the four
statuses.
The EIPQ contains 32 items, 16 assessing exploration and 16
targeting commitment. The EIPQ assesses ego identity in four ideological
domains (occupational choice, political preference, religious
affiliation, and personal values) and in four interpersonal domains
(friendships, dating, sex roles, and family). A six-point Likert-type
scale, ranging from "strongly disagree" to "strongly
agree," is used to rate each item. There are two exploration and
two commitment items per domain.
Cronbach's alpha for the EIPQ was .76 for exploration and .75
for commitment. Test-retest reliability coefficients were .90 for
exploration and .76 for commitment.
Following recommendations by Balistreri et al. (1995), median
splits were conducted on the exploration and commitment scores. Scores
falling at or above the median were classified as "high," and
the remainder as "low." Identity status categories were
assigned on the basis of those median splits. High scores on both
exploration and commitment placed the respondent in the identity
achievement category. One who had a high score on exploration and a low
score on commitment was judged to be in moratorium, whereas someone with
the reverse pattern was considered to be foreclosed. Low scores on both
scales placed the individual in the identity diffusion category.
For the formulae, a status assignment technique was created based
on the classification method for the Identity Style Inventory and the
EOM-EIS. All four scores for each participant were converted to standard
scores, and the participant was assigned to the status that had the
highest standard score. However, following recommendations by Bennion
and Adams (1986), participants whose four status scores were all within
one standard deviation of their respective means were assigned to a
fifth status, low profile moratorium.
Identity style. Building on Kelly's (1955) personal construct
theory, Berzonsky (1989) designed an objective measure that also uses
continuous scores to obtain a categorical placement. Berzonsky has not
purported to measure identity status; rather, he has claimed to measure
identity style. The Identity Style Inventory proceeds according to the
assumption that all individuals form and maintain their identities
through the use of any combination of three distinct processing styles.
The identity style paradigm differs from, and builds on, identity status
theory in that it is intended to represent a dynamic problem--solving
process. One may show a predilection for an extended period of time,
although one may not remain within a single identity status for very
long (see Waterman & Waterman, 1971). Therefore, identity style is
taken to represent the general process through which an individual makes
identity-related decisions. Berzonsky correlated identity status scores
from the EOM-EIS with the Identity Style Inve ntory scales and found
compatibility between the identity status and identity style constructs.
The first and most cognitively sophisticated of the three identity
styles is the informational approach. When informational individuals are
presented with a problem, they tend to seek advice and/or facts from
various sources (e.g., friends, books). They often avoid committing to
any specific solution until they have investigated multiple courses of
action. Once decided, those individuals remain open to new ideas and
facts and are willing to change course.
The normative identity style is characterized by adherence to
familial, social, societal, or other group standards. When faced with a
predicament, normative individuals are apt to seek counsel from people
who serve as authority figures. The result is closed-mindedness toward
non-authoritative input and a rigid decision-making style. Not
surprisingly, normative individuals tend to be highly committed
(Berzonsky & Neimeyer, 1994).
The third identity style, diffuse / avoidant, symbolizes the
inability or unwillingness to make decisions at all. Diffuse/avoidant
individuals tend to procrastinate and avoid thinking about prospective
or imminent situations (Berzonsky, 1992). Consequently, their levels of
commitment are low (Berzonsky & Neimeyer, 1994).
The 40-item Identity Style Inventory (Berzonsky, 1997) measures the
use of the three identity styles, as well as level of ideological and
interpersonal commitment (the version of the Identity Style Inventory
used in the two studies reported here is Version 3; Berzonsky used the
original 20-item version in his 1989 study). Responses to each item are
made on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from "not at all like
me" to "very much like me." Eleven items assess the
informational style, 9 target the normative style, 10 are aimed at the
diffuse/avoidant style, and 10 measure commitment (not analyzed here).
Of the three styles, the one assigned the highest standard score becomes
the participant's classification.
Internal reliability coefficients (Cronbach's alpha) for the
Identity Style Inventory, as reported by Berzonsky (1997), were:
informational style, .70; normative style, .64; and diffuse/avoidant
style, .76. Test-retest reliabilities over a two-week interval were:
informational style, .87; normative style, .87; and diffuse/avoidant
style, .83.
Procedure
Approval for the current study was obtained from the Florida State
University Human Subjects Committee. Each participant signed a consent
form before completing either of the measures. Participants were
identifiable only by a code number and were therefore anonymous.
The EIPQ was presented first, followed by the Identity Style
Inventory. Both instruments were presented in their paper-and-pencil
forms.
STUDY 1: RESULTS
Exploration and commitment scores from the EIPQ were transformed
into identity status scores using the formulae introduced in this
article. The continuous status scores were then correlated with the
Identity Style Inventory variables, and the correlation coefficients
were compared with those from the study by Berzonsky (1989). (See Table
4.)
In order to further compare the transformation formulae with the
existing continuous measures of identity status, the correlation
coefficients from both methods were converted to percentages of
explained variance. Explained variance obtained using the EOM-EIS was
subtracted from that obtained using the EIPQ. The results of those
calculations are displayed in Figure 1.
In 8 of the 12 comparisons between the correlation coefficients,
the differences were not statistically significant. The formulae
produced relatively strong and theoretically predictable relationships
between the Identity Style Inventory informational style measure and the
derived identity status scales. With respect to the diffuse/avoidant
style scale, the derived continuous scores and the existing continuous
scores performed in an equivalent manner, with the exception that the
existing diffusion measure appeared to relate more strongly. The two
sets of identity measures were similar in their relationships to the
Identity Style Inventory normative measure, with the exception that the
existing measure of achievement portrayed a stronger relationship to the
normative style than did the derived continuous measure of achievement.
For the Identity Style Inventory informational style scale, two of
the four differences in correlation coefficients were significant at the
.05 level. For both achievement and diffusion, the continuous scores
derived from the formulae were more strongly correlated with the
informational style than were the existing continuous scores. A third
difference, in which the continuous measure of foreclosure correlated
more strongly with the informational style scale than did the existing
continuous measure of foreclosure, approached statistical significance.
For the Identity Style Inventory normative style scale, one
difference in correlation coefficients was significant at the .05 level.
The existing continuous measure of achievement was more strongly
correlated with the normative style than was the achievement measure
derived from the formulae.
For the Identity Style Inventory diffuse/avoidant style scale,
onedifference in correlation coefficients was significant at the .05
level. The existing continuous measure of diffusion was more strongly
correlated with the diffuse/avoidant style than was the diffusion
measure derived from the formulae.
The formulae have been offered to provide the power of continuous
measures in statistical analysis. In order to assess the possibility
that they also provide adequate categorizations by status, status
assignments were made both by the median split technique and the
standard score technique, and those sets of assignments were compared
with one another. After the 55 participants assigned to low profile
moratorium were eliminated from the analysis, the two techniques
produced the same categorizations in 55 of 58 cases (94.8%). Among the
three discrepancies, two participants classified as foreclosed using the
median splits were classified as achieved using the formulae, and one
participant assigned to moratorium using the median splits was
classified as achieved using the formulae.
STUDY 2: METHOD
Participants
The participants in Study 2 were 325 Florida International
University students (83 males, 219 females, 23 did not report gender),
between 18 and 27 years of age, who were enrolled in psychology courses.
These students received course credit for their participation.
Measures
The EIPQ and Identity Style Inventory were used.
Procedure
Approval for this study was obtained from the Research Review
Committee of Florida International University. Each participant signed a
consent form and was assigned a code number to ensure anonymity. All
participants completed the EIPQ and Identity Style Inventory in a
research laboratory on the university campus.
STUDY 2: RESULTS
As in Study 1, exploration and commitment scores from the EIPQ were
transformed into identity status scores using the formulae. These status
scores were then correlated with the Identity Style Inventory style
measures, and the resulting correlations were compared with those from
Study 1. No correlations were significantly different between Study 1
and Study 2, and all of the differences that were significant between
Study 1 and the Berzonsky (1989) study were also significant between
Study 2 and the Berzonsky study.
As in Study 1, status assignments were made using the formulae and
were compared with those obstained using the median split technique.
After the 142 participants assigned to low profile moratorium using the
Bennion and Adams (1986) criteria were eliminated from the analysis, the
two techniques produced the same classifications for 176 of the
remaining 183 participants (96.2%); [[chi].sup.2](9, 183) = 678.17, p
[less than] .00001. Of the seven participants who were not placed into
the same status using both techniques, three were classified as diffused
using the formulae and as foreclosed using the median splits, two were
classified as diffused using the formulae and assigned to moratorium
using the median splits, and two were assigned to moratorium using the
formulae and classified as achieved using the median splits.
DISCUSSION
The present report has offered simple formulae for deriving
continuous measures of ego identity status from continuous measures of
exploration and commitment. The effectiveness of the formulae was tested
by means of comparing (a) the relationship of continuous measures of
status on the EIPQ to Identity Style Inventory style measures with (b)
the relationship of continuous measures of status on the EOM-EIS with
Identity Style Inventory style measures.
The advantages of generating continuous status scores are
threefold. First, continuous measures of identity status empower the
investigation of correlates to each status. Second, continuous status
scores allow for status assignment by means of standardization
procedures rather than simply through median splits. Finally, and
perhaps most importantly, continuous status scores may offer a more
sensitive measure of the relative strength of each status for a given
individual.
The continuous scores derived using the formulae did not differ
from the direct continuous measures in terms of 8 of the 12
relationships between identity status and identity style. In all 12
cases, the formulae produced theoretically consistent relationships
between identity status and identity style. Among the four differences,
two were found for the informational style, one for the normative style,
and one for the diffuse/avoidant style.
First, the correlation between the informational style and the
achievement status, which was theoretically expected to be high and
positive (Berzonsky, 1990), was significantly greater in the current
study than in the results by Berzonsky (1989). The larger correlation
between achievement and the informational style obtained using the
formulae may, to some extent, be a result of the presence of both
exploration and commitment, each of which has been shown to be
significantly and positively related to the informational style
(Schwartz, 1996), in the numerator of the achievement formula.
Second, the diffusion measure created by the formulae was
negatively correlated with the Identity Style Inventory informational
style scale to a significantly greater extent than was the EOM-EIS
diffusion scale. This effect may be the inverse expression of that which
produced the higher correlation between the EIPQ achievement measure and
the Identity Style Inventory informational style scale. That is, it may
be caused by the presence of both exploration and commitment in the
denominator of the diffusion formula.
Third, Berzonsky (1989) found a substantial correlation between
achievement and the normative style, which was not theoretically
expected, whereas the current study produced a much more modest finding.
Inasmuch as the normative style is negatively correlated with
exploration (Schwartz, 1996), the presence of the exploration term in
the numerator of the achievement formula may have served to lower the
relationship of the resulting achievement scale with the normative
style.
Finally, Berzonsky reported a higher correlation between diffusion
and the diffuse/avoidant style, whereas the relationship obtained with
the formulae was much lower. The unusually high correlation reported by
Berzonsky may be, to some extent, an artifact of overlapping item
content between the EOM-EIS diffusion measure and the Identity Style
Inventory diffuse/avoidant scale. An inspection of the two scales has
revealed some degree of item overlap.
It is presumed that the formulae can be applied to any identity
instrument, direct or derived, that produces continuous measures of
exploration and commitment. Jackson et al. (1990) derived status
assignments from continuous measures of exploration and commitment, on
the Grotevant and Cooper (1981) Ego Identity Interview, by setting
rational thresholds. Their method did not produce continuous measures of
status, however.
The multiplicative formulae put forth produced nearly identical
results as those for similarly constructed additive formulae, which were
calculated but not reported here. The choice of multiplicative formulae
over additive formulae rests on the logic of Freud and Herbart, which
appears to support a multiplication algorithm. However, it is for
further research to introduce alternative formulae and to test their
effectiveness.
The present investigation was not designed to compare the EIPQ and
the EOM-EIS; additional research will be needed to accomplish that. It
was conducted simply to determine whether a scoring method performs in a
way that is intelligible given the standards of the field. On the whole,
it appears that the transformed status scores perform acceptably.
The two analyses provide some initial evidence that the formulae:
(a) provide meaningful continuous measures of identity status, and (b)
may reliably be used for categorizations. It is perhaps obvious that the
level of match in categorizations is mathematically ordained. Central
tendency, either the median or the mean, z = 0, is the dividing line in
each method, and the continuous values of E and C are basic to the
categorization in each method. However, whenever there was a discrepancy
between the status assigned using the formulae and that assigned using
the median splits, the formulae consistently assigned the more mature
status.
The conceptual difference between the formulae and median split
status assignment techniques is only that the formulae take the
additional step of making clear that the statuses may be thought of as
continuously distributed resultants of the simultaneous consideration of
exploration and commitment measures. This additional step is not only
compatible with the common definitions of the concepts in the source
theories and mathematically obvious, but it may also anticipate a better
treatment of amount or level of a status in future research. It may
anticipate a clear recognition that a psychological resultant is not
merely an amalgam of its antecedent components, but has additional
meaning as an emergent phenomenon. The argument here is not that , for
example, achievement is more than the simultaneous occurrence of
exploration and commitment, but that research with the formulae is more
likely to clarify whether it is more.
Although it may prove fruitful to test the formulae introduced here
on other identity measures, a review and evaluation of the range of
measures within the identity formation literature was beyond the scope
of the present study. It will remain for other studies to clarify the
limits of application for the formulae.
It should be noted that the foreclosure and moratorium equations
are the mathematical inverses of one another, as are the diffusion and
achievement equations. Thus, those two pairs of mathematical inverses
yield scores that are reciprocals of each other. As an artifact of the
mathematical inverse procedure, two orthogonal identity status axes have
been created. The two axes are independent of one another; their
measures are not correlated. Yet, the axes created by the formulae may
reflect some sort of psychological reality. One of the axes connects the
diffusion and achievement statuses, whereas the other axis connects the
foreclosure and moratorium statuses. The Cartesian plane comprised of
the two axes represents a 45-degree rotation of the exploration and
commitment axis matrix offered by Marcia. Further, it is possible that
the achievement-diffusion axis represents the bipolar dimension proposed
by Erikson, ranging from identity synthesis to identity confusion (see
Cote, 1984).
The equations introduced here are intended to reflect the
fundamental epistemological processes that enable four combinations of
exploration and commitment to yield four identity statuses. In the
context of a specific methodological contribution, it is not possible to
devote the space that would be required to treat the historical,
philosophical, and psychometric roots of the four equations fully. Their
rationale has merely been outlined.
A number of historical, philosophical, mathematical, and
psychological points remain to be explored or elaborated. Among those
are the debts to the logic of Herbart and Freud and an extended
discussion of the exact psychological conditions to be reflected in the
formulae. One item within that discussion might be whether the iteration of additive effects of simultaneous or immediately sequential
psychological processes yields a multiplicative overall effect. For now,
however, it seems sufficient to say that a base has been laid for
further empirical work to ascertain the conditions under which the
formulae may be useful or to test the comparative value of other
formulae that might be offered.
Richard M. Dunham, Department of Psychology, Florida State
University, Tallahassee, Florida.
REFERENCES
Adams, G. R., Bennian, L. D., & Huh, K. (1989). Objective
Measure of Ego Identity Status: A reference manual. Unpublished
manuscript, Utah State University, Logan.
Balistreri, E., Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., & Geisinger, K. F.
(1995). Development and validation of the Ego Identity Process
Questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 179-192.
Bennion, L. D., & Adams, G. R. (1986). A revision of the
extended version of the Objective Measure of Ego Identity Status: An
identity instrument for use with late adolescents. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 1, 183-198.
Berman, A. M., Schwartz, S. J., Kurtines, W. M., & Berman, S.
L. (2000). The role of style and competence in identity formation.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Berzonsky, M. D. (1985). Diffusion within Marcia's identity
status paradigm:n Does it foreshadow academic problems? Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 14, 527-538.
Berzonsky, M. D. (1989). Identity style: Conceptualization and
measurement. Journal of Adolescent Research, 5, 268-282.
Berzonsky, M.D. (1990). Self-construction over the lifespan: A
process perspective on identity formation. In G. J. Neimeyer & R. A.
Neimeyer (Eds.), Advances in personal construct theory: Volume 1 (pp.
155-186). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Berzonsky, M. D. (1992). Identity style and coping strategies.
Journal of Personality, 60, 771-788.
Berzonsky, M. D. (1997). Psychometric properties of the Identity
Style Inventory, Version 3. Unpublished manuscript, State University of
New York at Cortland.
Berzonsky, M. D., & Neimeyer, G. J. (1994). Ego identity status
and identity processing orientation: The mediating role of commitment.
Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 425-435.
C[hat{o}]tacute{e} J. E. (1984). The identity crisis: A formulation
and empirical test of Erikson's theory of ego identity formation.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York University, Ontario, Canada.
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Freud, 5. (1950). Negation. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and
pathology of thought: Selected sources (pp. 75-92) New York: Columbia
University Press (Original work published 1905).
Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. R. (1981). Assessing adolescent
identity in the areas of occupation, religion, politics, friendships,
dating, and sex roles: Manual for the administration and coding of the
interview. Journal Supplement Abstract Service Catalog of Selected
Documents in Psychology, 11, 52-53 (Ms. No. 2295).
Herbart, J. F. (1961). Psychology as a science, newly founded upon
experience, metaphysics, and mathematics. In T. Shipley (Ed.), Classics
in psychology (pp. 22-50). New York: Philosophical Library (Original
work published 1866).
Jackson, E. P., Dunham, R. M., & Kidwell, J. S. (1990). The
effects of gender and family cohesion and adaptability on identity
status. Journal of Adolescent Research, 5, 161-174.
Jessee, C. L., Gruenfeld, K. E., & Waterman, A. S. (1995,
February). The relationship of identity status to optimal psychological
functioning and decision making. Paper presented at the second biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Identity Formation, Dog Island,
FL.
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New
York: Norton.
Kidwell, J. S., Dunham, R. M., Bacho, R. A., Pastorino, E., &
Portes, P. R. (1995). Adolescent identity exploration: A test of
Erikson's theory of transitional crisis. Adolescence, 30, 785-793.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity
status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 551-558.
Marcia, J. E. (1967). Ego identity status: Relationship to change
in self-esteem, "general maladjustment," and authoritarianism.
Journal of Personality, 35, 118-133.
Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.),
Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 159-187). New York: Wiley.
Marcia, J. E. (1988). Common processes underlying ego identity,
cognitive/moral development, and individuation. In D. K. Lapsley &
F. C. Power (Eds.), Self, ego, and identity: Integrative approaches (pp.
211-225). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Marcia, J. E. (1993). The ego identity status approach to ego
identity. In J. E. Marcia, A. S. Waterman, D. R. Matteson, S. L. Archer,
& J. L. Orlofsky (Eds.), Ego identity: A handbook for psychosocial research (pp. 1-21). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Marcia, J. E., & Archer, S. L. (1993). The Identity Status
Interview: Late Adolescent College Form. In J. E. Marcia, A. S.
Waterman, D. R. Matteson, S. L. Archer, & J. L. Orlofsky (Eds.), Ego
identity: A handbook for psychosocial research (pp. 318-333). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Meuss, W. (1996). Studies on identity development in adolescence:
An overview of research and some new data. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 25, 569-598.
Schwartz, S. J. (1996). Ego identity, personal expressiveness, and
identity style. Unpublished master's thesis, Florida State
University, Tallahassee.
Stephen, J., Fraser, E., & Marcia, J. E. (1992).
Moratorium-achievement (MAMA) cycles in lifespan identity development:
Value orientations and reasoning system correlates. Journal of
Adolescence, 15, 283-300.
Waterman, A. S. (1988). Identity status theory and Erikson's
theory: Commonalities and differences. Developmental Review, 8, 185-208.
Waterman, A. S., & Waterman, C. K. (1971). A longitudinal study of changes in ego identity status during the freshman year at college.
Developmental Psychology, 5, 167-173.
Some Identity Status Instruments by Mode of Status
Assessment and by Type of Status Scaling
Assessment Categorical Scaling Continuous Scaling
Direct Identity Status Interview Extended Objective Measure
(Marcia & Archer, 1993) of Ego Identity Status
Ego Identity Interview (Bennion & Adams, 1986)
(Grotevant & Cooper, 1981)
Derived [*] Ego Identity Process Questionnaire None [**]
(Balistreri, Busch-Rossnagel, &
Geisinger, 1995)
(*.)calculated from measures of exploration and commitment
(**.)see Jackson, Dunham, & Kidwell, 1990.
Identity Statuses, Reflecting Manifest and
Less Manifest Exploration and Commitment
Commitment
MANIFEST LESS MANIFEST
Exploration MANIFEST Achievement Moratorium
LESS MANIFEST Foreclosure Diffusion
Logical Recasting of Interactions of Exploration and Commitment
Status Exploration Logical Relationship Commitment
Achievement manifest AND manifest
Moratorium manifest AND less manifest
Foreclosure less manifest AND manifest
Diffusion less manifest AND less manifest
Comparison between EOM-EIS and EIPQ
Correlations with the Identity Style Inventory
Identity Style
Informational Normative Diffuse/Avoidant
Achievement
EOM-EIS .25 .52 -.40
EIPQ .53 .20 -.36
Difference (z) 2.27 [*] 2.61 [**] 0.28
Moratorium
EOM-EIS .06 -.29 .29
EIPQ .15 -.50 .02
Difference (z) 0.62 1.72 1.93
Foreclosure
EOM-EIS -.02 .47 -.06
EIPQ -.14 .48 -.10
Difference (z) 0.83 0.07 0.28
Diffusion
EOM-EIS -.28 -.18 .62
EIPQ -.54 -.28 .31
Difference (z) 2.13 [*] 0.76 2.82 [**]
Note: The z test determined whether the correlation
coefficients were significantly different from each other.
(*.)p [less than] .05
(**.)p [less than] .01