Enuresis, Firesetting, and Cruelty to Animals: Does the Ego Triad Show Predictive Validity?
Slavkin, Michael Lawrence
ABSTRACT
The hypothesis tested in this study was that the presence of
enuresis and cruelty to animals in juvenile firesetters would be
significantly related to recidivistic firesetting. This hypothesis was
related to Yarnell's belief in an ego triad among juvenile
firesetters, which linked the occurrence of enuresis, cruelty to animals
and others, and firesetting. No relationship was found between groups
for firesetting recidivism and enuresis. However, juveniles who were
identified as being cruel to animals were more likely than those who
were not cruel to animals to engage in recidivistic firesetting
behaviors.
Each year, fires set by juveniles account for a large portion of
fire-related public property damage and deaths. Fires set by children
and adolescents are more likely than any other household disaster to
result in death (National Fire Protection Association, 1999). In 1998,
it was estimated that fires set by children and juveniles resulted in
6,215 American deaths, another 30,800 injuries, and billions of dollars
in property damage (National Fire Protection Association, 1999). Despite
the costs and impact of juvenile firesetting, it remains a
little-studied area. The limited research that does exist is dominated
by a psychodynamic perspective.
Examinations of the motivating forces behind juvenile firesetting
have often relied on a psychoanalytic orientation (Kaufman, Heims, &
Reiser, 1961; Lester, 1975; Rothstein, 1963; Yarnell, 1940). Those
writings are largely based upon Freud's (1932) assertion that
firesetting in youth is a regressive retreat to "primitive
man's" desire to gain power and control over nature. Freud
(1930) states: "In man's struggles to gain power over the
tyranny of nature, his acquisition of power over fire was the most
important. It is as if primitive man had had the impulse when he came in
contact with fire, to gratify an infantile pleasure in respect of it and
put it out with a stream of urine....Putting out fire by urinating...
therefore represents a sexual act with a man, an enjoyment of masculine
potency in homosexual rivalry. Whoever was the first to deny himself
this pleasure and spare the fire was able to take it with him and break
it into his own service. By curbing the fire of his own sexual passion
he was able to ta ke fire as a force of nature.... It is remarkable how
regular analytic findings testify to the close connection between the
ideas of ambition, fire, and urethral eroticism" (p. 50).
Freud's perception of the youthful firesetter has guided the
firesetting literature, specifically that juvenile firesetting is an
ego-oriented conflict.
In her seminal work on juvenile firesetters, Yarnell (1940)
examined 60 young psychiatric patients at Bellevue Hospital in New York.
Yarnell (1940) asserted that firesetting in juveniles is the result of
(1) castration fears, (2) enuresis, and (3) the influence of the mother
figure in the life of the child. Yarnell speculated that youths who set
fires do so in order to gain power over adults. She emphasized the
neglectful nature of the mother-son relationship. Yarnell also examined
the juvenile firesetters' relationships with fathers as they
related to professional issues, such as for children whose fathers were
fire-fighting professionals. She supported Freud's views that
juvenile firesetters have difficulty with enuresis and cruelty to
animals and to others.
Yarnell proposed an ego triad among juvenile firesetters that
linked the occurrence of enuresis, cruelty to animals and others, and
firesetting in youth. The comorbidity of these behaviors and their
predictive power in identifying adult criminal behavior have been
verified in a number of studies (Lester, 1975; Prentky & Carter,
1984; Robbins & Robbins, 1967; Rothstein, 1963; Wax & Haddox,
1974).
It is not surprising that these three behaviors were identified
simultaneously in juveniles who set fires; the studies that validated
the triad used institutionalized samples. Moreover, these reports were
based on case study reviews and data extrapolated from projective instruments (Kaufman et al., 1961; Lester, 1975; Macht & Mack, 1968;
Quinsey, Chaplin, & Upfold, 1989; Rothstein, 1963).
Juvenile firesetters have been reported to be more likely than
other groups of juveniles to display cruelty to children or animals, as
well as to have difficulties with enuresis (Quinsey et al., 1989;
Sakheim & Osborn, 1999; Sakheim, Osborn, & Abrams, 1991;
Saunders & Awad, 1991), although the predictiveness of these factors
is limited (Blumenberg, 1981; Heath, Gayton, & Hardesty, 1976;
Showers & Pickrell, 1987). Justice, Justice, and Kraft (1974)
questioned whether the ego triad is an adequate predictor of violent
behavior in adulthood. They asserted that the ego triad largely has been
found to occur simultaneously with factors that may be better predictors
of violent adult behaviors.
In order to evaluate the predictive nature of the ego triad, the
present study investigated whether the presence of enuresis and cruelty
to animals in juvenile firesetters is related to recidivistic
firesetting.
METHOD
Data Collection
Data collection took place during a scheduled three-hour interview
between the fire professional, the juvenile firesetter, and the
firesetter's parent/guardian following referral to the MCAIN
(Marion County Arson Investigation Network) Fire Stop Program. The
interview protocol followed the recommended format designed by the
Federal Emergency Management Agency. Parents and youths were told that
they were to engage in a brief discussion regarding firesetting
behaviors. The youths and their parents were also required to complete
written questionnaires as part of the program.
Each record in the MCAIN database includes demographic information,
the nature of the firesetter's behaviors, the firesetting incident,
and whether the individual received any counseling following the fire.
Four primary pieces of information are included in each record: (a)
narrative information from the fire-site interview, (b) the Family Fire
Risk Interview Form (Fineman, 1997a), (c) the Juvenile Fire Risk
Interview Form (Fineman, 1997b), and (d) the Parent Fire Risk
Questionnaire (Fineman, 1997c).
Demographic information was obtained from the Family Fire Risk
Interview Form (Fineman, 1997a), which was completed by the parent'
guardian at the time of Fire Stop Program interview. Information
included child's age, race, gender, level of education, school
enrollment, number of siblings, parent's level of education, and
parent's present job.
Participants
Records from 78 firesetters aged 3 to 6 years, 240 firesetters aged
7 to 10 years, 157 firesetters aged 11 to 14 years, and 413 firesetters
aged 15 to 18 years who were referred to MCAIN for firesetting behaviors
were analyzed. Many firesetters over the age of 12 years are referred
for incarceration rather than psychoeducation, which may have limited
the sampling of older juveniles. For the purposes of the study, the
entire MCAIN database was used (firesetters in the MCAIN database range
in age from 3 to 19 years).
Independent Variables
The presence of enuresis was identified using information taken
from the Parent Fire Risk Questionnaire (Fineman, 1997c). Juveniles were
identified as either having problems with enuresis or not having such
problems. The presence of cruelty to animals was also identified using
information taken from the Parent Fire Risk Questionnaire (Fineman,
1997c). Juveniles were identified as either having problems with cruel
behavior toward animals or not. The presence of recidivistic firesetting
behaviors was identified using information taken from the Juvenile Fire
Risk Interview Form (Fineman, 1997b). Juveniles were identified as
either having problems with recidivism or not.
RESULTS
No relationship was found between firesetting recidivism and the
presence of enuresis, [chi square](1) = .16, p = .82. However, juveniles
who were identified as being cruel to animals were more likely than
those who were not cruel to animals to engage in recidivistic
firesetting, [chi square](1) = 25.88, p = .001. When the ego triad was
assessed using an analysis of variance (the presence of enuresis and
cruelty to animals as relating to recidivistic behaviors), the two-way
interaction was not statistically significant, F = 1.69, df= 1, p = .23.
Interestingly, the level of enuresis in the firesetters was
significantly higher than in a normative sample of juveniles, t = 6.41,
p = .02 (Health Examination Survey Cycle III, 1978).
DISCUSSION
The hypothesis tested in this study was that the presence of
enuresis and cruelty to animals in juvenile firesetters would be
significantly related to recidivistic firesetting. This hypothesis
emerged from Yarnell's belief that there is an ego triad among
juvenile firesetters, which links the occurrence of enuresis, cruelty to
animals and others, and firesetting in youth.
No differences were found in firesetting recidivism based on the
presence of enuresis. However, juveniles who were identified as being
cruel to animals were more likely than those who were not cruel to
animals to engage in recidivistic firesetting behaviors. Further, the
incidence of enuresis was elevated in the present sample when compared
with a normative sample.
Although cruelty to animals seems to hold potential as a predictor
of recidivistic firesetting, it is most likely an externalizing behavior
that correlates highly with delinquency. Justice, Justice, and Kraft
(1974) have asserted that the ego triad largely occurs simultaneously
with factors that may be better predictors of violent adult behaviors.
While enuresis may not be an indicator of recidivistic firesetting,
it would appear to be an effective marker of the problematic behaviors
that are comorbid with firesetting in juveniles. In addition, the
significant difference in enuresis between a normative population and
the present sample is noteworthy.
Implications
It is interesting that fires set by juveniles account for a large
portion of fire-related public property damage and deaths, but these
crimes have received little attention both in the literature and in the
press. Because of the costs and impact of juvenile firesetting,
developmentalists and mental health professionals need to more
thoroughly examine this behavior. Though efforts to understand and
reduce firesetting are increasing, further research is needed,
particularly from developmental perspectives that include individual as
well as environmental factors, rather than from a psychodynamic/ego
triad perspective.
An examination of the literature on firesetters shows that a
variety of characteristics can define specific types of firesetters.
Further, firesetting appears to differ as a result of both maturational
and environmental factors. Future investigations should include an
examination of the firesetter's history, such as prior learning
experiences with fire, cognitive and behavioral development, and family
influences and stressors.
REFERENCES
Blumenberg, N. H. (1981). Arson update: A review of the literature
on firesetting. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the
Law, 9, 255-265.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta
(Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 6). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Fineman, K. R. (1997a). Family Fire Risk Interview Form.
Washington, DC: Federal Emergency Management Agency.
Fineman, K R. (1997b). Juvenile Fire Risk Interview Form.
Washington, DC: Federal Emergency Management Agency.
Fineman, K. R. (1997c). Parent Fire Risk Questionnaire. Washington,
DC: Federal Emergency Management Agency.
Freud, S. (1930). Civilization and its discontents. New York: W. W.
Norton and Company.
Freud, S. (1932). Original papers: The acquisition of power over
fire. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, October, 51-98.
Health Examination Survey Cycle III. (1978). Information taken from
Center for Disease Control Data Base, Atlanta, GA.
Heath, G. A., Gayton, W. F., & Hardesty, V. A. (1976). Early
childhood firesetting. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 21,
229-237.
Justice, B., Justice, R., & Kraft, I. A. (1974). Early warning
signs of violence: Is a triad enough? American Journal of Psychiatry,
131, 457-459.
Kaufman, I., Heims, L. W., & Reiser, D. E. (1961). A
re-evaluation of the psychodynamics of firesetting. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 31, 123-136.
Kolko, D. J. (1985). Juvenile firesetting: A review and
methodological critique. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 345-376.
Lester, D. (1975). Firesetting. Corrective and Social Psychiatry and Journal of Behavior Technology, Methods, and Therapy, 21, 22-26.
Macht, L. B., & Mack, J. E. (1968). The firesetter syndrome.
Psychiatry, 31, 277-288.
National Fire Protection Association. (1999). Statistics on the
national fire problem [On-line]. Available: http://www.fema.gov/nfpa/.
Prentky, R. A., & Carter, D. L. (1984). The predictive value of
the triad for sex offenders. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 2,
341-354.
Quinsey, V. L., Chaplin, T. C., & Upfold, D. (1989). Arsonists
and sexual arousal to fire setting: Correlation unsupported. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 20, 203-209.
Robbins, E., & Robbins, L. (1967). Arson: With special
reference to pyromania. New York State Journal of Medicine, 67, 795-798.
Rothstein, R. (1963). Explorations of ego structures of firesetting
children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 246-253.
Sakheim, G. A., & Osborn, E. (1999). Severe versus nonsevere
firesetters revisisted. Child Welfare, 78, 411-434.
Sakheim, G. A., Osborn, E., & Abrams, D. (1991). Toward a
clearer differentiation of high-risk from low-risk firesetters. Child
Welfare, 70, 489-503.
Saunders, E. B., & Awad, G. A. (1991). Adolescent female
firesetters [Special issue: Child and adolescent psychiatry]. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 401-404.
Showers, J., & Pickrell, E. (1987). Child firesetters: A study
of three populations. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 38, 495-501.
Wax, D. E., & Haddox, V. G. (1974). Enuresis, fire setting, and
animal cruelty: A useful danger signal in predicting vulnerability of
adolescent males to assaultive behavior. Child Psychiatry and Human
Development, 4, 151-156.
Yarnell, H. (1940). Firesetting in children. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 10, 272-287.