Vocal behavior in the dyadic interactions of preadolescent and early adolescent friends and acquaintances.
Feldstein, Stanley ; Field, Tiffany
An increasing number of studies are concerned with the extent to
which the vocal behavior of pairs of individuals in conversational
interactions with each other shows mutual vocal coordination or
entrainment. Most of these studies have involved the interactions of
adults and young adults (e.g., Crown, 1991; Feldstein & Welkowitz,
1987; Field et al., 1992) and, more recently, the interactions of
infants with mothers and with strangers (e.g., Beebe, Alson, Jaffe,
Feldstein, & Crown, 1988; Cohn & Tronick, 1988; Jaffe, Beebe,
Feldstein, Crown, & Jasnow, 2001; Jasnow & Feldstein, 1986). The
study reported here examined the interactions of preadolescent dyads.
The study had three major aims. The first was to determine whether, and
how much, entraimnent, or what we call coordinated interpersonal timing,
occurs in the dialogues of preadolescent pairs. Coordinated
interpersonal timing, or CIT, refers to changes in the temporal patterns
of one person in a conversation as a function of changes in those of the
other pers on. The second aim was to provide descriptive statistics of
the states that comprise the vocal patterning of preadolescent
interactions. The last, but not at all the least aim, was to compare, in
terms of the state durations and frequencies and the coefficients of
CIT, the interactions of friends and acquaintances in mixed- and
same-gender dyads. Thus, the study is primarily an analysis of the
temporal structure of preadolescent dialogues. This information was
expected to extend our knowledge of what may be the basis for subsequent
differences in the social interactions of friends and acquaintances.
This expectation is based upon a "dyadic systems" position
which holds that the two-person group is a basic psychological unit in
which personality is originally formed (Sullivan, 1947) and in which the
behavior of one of the individuals is determined by the behavior of both
individuals (Jaffe, Beebe, Feldstein, Crown, & Jasnow, 2001). It has
been shown (Jaffe et al., 2001) that the degree to which the temporal rh
ythms of mothers and their infants are coordinated not only initiates,
for four-month-old infants, the formation of an adult dialogue structure
prelinguistically, but predicts the quality of the mother-infant
relationship that will have developed by age 12 months.
The few investigations that have examined the chronography of
children's interactions were concerned with whether the
conversational time patterns of the children exhibited CIT, as well as
the effects of age, gender, and ethnicity on the conversational time
patterns of the children. One study (Welkowitz, Bond, & Feldstein,
1984a) of Hawaiian children found that the vocal time patterns are
stable indices of children's conversational behavior, and that the
patterns seem to vary as a function of the gender and ethnicity of the
conversational pairs. Another study (Welkowitz, Bond, & Feldstein,
1984b) of Japanese-American children and adults in mixed- and
same-gender pairs found gender effects for the adults but not for the
children. Two earlier studies (Garvey & BenDebba, 1974; Welkowitz,
Cariffe, & Feldstein, 1976) seemed to indicate that the development
of CIT is positively related to age. However, none of these studies
involved preadolescents, and the techniques for assessing CIT were
relatively crude. In the present study, CIT was estimated by the use of
time-series regression analyses.
METHOD
Participants
The 30 female and 26 male pre- and early adolescents who
participated in the study were recruited from two sixth-grade classes at
the West Laboratory Elementary School for a study by Field et al.
(1992). The average age of the participants was 11.5 years, and the
friends knew each other for an average of 4.2 years. The acquaintances
knew each other for approximately five months. The selection of friends
and acquaintances was made on the basis of a sociogram that presented
cartoon faces with messages in balloon-like clouds coming from the
cartoon faces such as "I know ______ the best" and "I
know ______ the least." The choices of the students were validated
by their teachers, who were asked to rank order, for each student, two
of the student's closest friends (Field et al., 1992). For the
present study, it is important simply to note that the boys and girls were assembled into same- and mixed-sex pairs, and that each pair
participated in a 10-minute, face-to-face interaction across a small
table, about any top ic or topics they desired, and that the interaction
was audiotaped such that each voice was on a separate channel.
Coding Vocal Interactions
The coding of the vocal behavior of a dialogue is accomplished by
means of the direct input of the two audio signals, one for each person,
into a specialized computer system known as the Automated Vocal
Transaction Analyzer (AVTA; Jaffe & Feldstein, 1970). AVTA first
performs an analogue-to-digital conversion. Specifically, the two
channels of incoming audio signals are sampled synchronously every 250
msec to determine whether the signal in each channel is on or off,
without regard to the frequency or intonational characteristics of the
sounds. The sole requirement is that the sounds be within the range of
human hearing. These observed time series are stored digitally in the
computer in the form of a sequence of four number codes that index the
four observable dyadic states: when one signal is on and the other is
off, the code is "1"; if vice versa, the code is
"2"; if both signals are on, the code is "3"; or if
both are off, the code number is "0." In the present study,
the AVTA software transforms these dec imal numbers into a set of
dialogic vocal states (defined next) and averages their durations for a
fixed time unit. Although the observable states 1 and 2 distinguish
between the speakers, the coactive states 3 and 0 do not. To have them
do so requires a superordinate characteristic of dialogue that allows
for unequivocal identification of the speaker. The speaking turn is the
characteristic that provides such identification. It begins the instant
either participant vocalizes alone, and it is held by that participant
until the other vocalizes alone, at which point the other participant
takes the turn. By virtue of the turn, the AVTA system is able to
compute five vocal states (Feldstein & Welkowitz, 1987; Jaffe &
Feldstein, 1970) that are subsumed by the turn: vocalizations (V),
pauses (P), switching pauses (SP), and interruptive and noninterruptive
simultaneous speech (ISS and NSS, respectively). Figure 1 diagrams an
interactional sequence. The numbered line at the bottom represents
250-msec units. The arrow s that point down denote the end of speaker
1's turns; those that turn up denote the end of speaker 2's
turns.
A vocalization is a continuous utterance containing no silence as
long as 250 msec (silences less than 250 msec are attributable to stop
consonants in speech and are bridged by our A/D converter). Joint
silence is classified in terms of its outcome. A pause is a joint
silence greater than 250 msec bounded by the vocalizations of the
speaker who holds the turn. A switching pause is a joint silence greater
than 250 msec initiated by the speaker who holds the turn, but
terminated by the other speaker, who thereby gains the turn. In
relatively unconstrained dialogues, it is assigned, for theoretical and
empirical reasons, to the speaker whose turn it terminates.
Similarly, simultaneous (joint) speech is that uttered by the
person who does not hold the turn during a vocalization of the person
who does and it, too, is classified in terms of its outcome.
Noninterruptive simultaneous speech (NSS) is a segment of speech that
begins and ends while the person who holds the turn vocalizes
continuously. Interruptive simultaneous speech is part of a speech
segment that begins while the person who holds the turn is vocalizing
but continues when he or she stops. Only that part of the segment
uttered coactively is considered interruptive simultaneous speech. The
trailing portion, inasmuch as it is then a unilateral utterance, marks
the beginning of the turn for the person who initiated the interruption
and is, therefore, considered his or her vocalization.
The AVTA system computes, for turns and each of the vocal states,
the average duration for the entire interaction session but also the
average duration for every five seconds of the interaction. The latter
computation is for the purpose of subjecting the data to time-series
analyses.
RESULTS
Frequencies and Durations
The descriptive statistics of the states for the interactions
studied are presented in Tables 1 and 2. Separate factorial ANOVAs were
used to compare speaking-turn frequencies and durations, the vocal-state
frequencies and durations of the pairs of friends and acquaintances, and
their gender and dyad gender (whether same-gender or cross-gender). The
results provide no evidence (with regard to frequencies and durations)
that gender or dyad gender (same or mixed), as a main effect,
differentiated the two groups for any of the vocal states. Frequency of
turns, however, yielded au F ratio that, although only bordering on
significance, F(1, 108) = 3.53, p = .06, [euro] = .15, tended to suggest
that the girls used more turns than did the boys.
The participants' use of interruptive and noninterruptive
simultaneous speech yielded significant differences. The boys engaged in
more frequent interruptive and noninterruptive simultaneous speech (ISS
and NSS, respectively) when they were with acquaintances than when they
were with friends whereas the girls engaged in more interruptive and
noninterruptive simultaneous speech when they were with friends than
when they were with acquaintances, F(1), 108) = 17.05, p = .00, [euro] =
.36, and F(1, 108) = 14.31, p = .00, [euro] = .33, respectively. Figure
2 graphs the influence of gender by dyad type on ISS and Figure 3 graphs
the same type of interaction for NSS.
Although not formally significant, there was some tendency of
friends to generally use longer durations of ISS in their interactions
than did acquaintances, F(1, 108) = 3.77, p = .06, [euro] = .14, but the
analysis also yielded three significant two-way interactions of gender
by dyad type, F(1, 108) = 22.22, p = .00, [euro] = .40, gender by dyad
gender, F(1, 108) 6.60, p = .01, [euro] = .22, and dyad type by dyad
gender, F(1, 108) = 5.88, p .02, [euro] = .20. The boys used longer
durations of ISS than did girls when they were with acquaintances,
whereas the girls used longer ISS durations than did boys when they were
with friends (Figure 4). However, the girls used somewhat shorter
segments of ISS than did the boys when they were in mixed (cross-gender)
dyads, but longer segments of ISS than the boys when they were in
same-gender dyads (Figure 5). Finally, friends used longer segments of
ISS in same-gender dyads than in cross-gender dyads, but acquaintances
did the opposite (Figure 6).
Coordinated Interpersonal Timing
Time-series regression. In order to assess CIT, the turn durations
and the average state durations of each pair were subjected to bivariate time-series regression analyses that examined the coordination of each
partner's vocal behavior with the prior vocal behavior of the other
partner. The ARIMA modeling procedures of SPSS were used to
"pre-whiten" the data, and the ACF subprogram of SPSS Trends
was used to allow for visual and statistical tests of the best-fitting
model. After the model was selected, the AREG subprogram computed the
equations. The analyses yield squared semi-partial regression
coefficients that are the proportions of variance of one partner's
vocal behavior accounted for by the other partner's vocal behavior
(Table 3). The square roots of these coefficients index the strength of
the coordination between the partners.
After completing the time-series regression analyses, a special
meta-analytic procedure was used to determine, treating each dyad as a
separate experiment, whether the groups of friends and acquaintances
each exhibited, in general, significant degrees of coordination with the
prior temporal behaviors of their partners. The meta-analytic approach
suggested by R. Rosenthal (personal communication, September, 1988) was
employed and involved three simple steps for a particular group of CIT
values: (a) transform the probability values associated with the
coefficients to normal standard deviate scores (z scores); (b) square
the z scores to yield chi-squares, each with a degree of freedom; and
(c) sum the chi-squares for each group to provide a chi-square with
degrees of freedom equal to the number of chi-squares in the sum. The
summed chi-square value indicates whether the coefficients (the square
roots of the CIT values) are significantly different from zero. The
reason for this approach is that the usual meta-ana lysis tests a
directional hypothesis, whereas in this study the coefficients support
the hypothesis that coordinated interpersonal timing has occurred
whether they are positive or negative. The only value that does not
support the hypothesis is zero. A separate meta-analysis was computed
for each of the vocal measures for each of the groups. The results
(Table 4) of the analyses reveal that, in general, there was a
considerable degree of temporal coordination across groups and states.
An exception was that neither friends nor acquaintances coordinated the
durations of their vocalizations with the prior vocalizations of their
partners.
Comparisons of Gender and Groups
Multiple regression analyses were performed to determine whether
CIT was influenced by gender, dyad type, and dyad gender. The dependent
variable in each of the hierarchical equations was comprised of the
squared semi-partial coefficients ([r.sup.2.sub.sp]) indexing the CIT of
one of the vocal states. The independent variables were gender, dyad
type, dyad gender, and the products of the three variables that carry
the interaction effects.
The results yielded an interaction of gender by dyad type,
indicating that although the boys' turn, F(1, 108) = 5.187, p =
.02, [r.sub.sp] = -.21 (Figure 7), and switching-pause, F(1, 108) =
4.611, p = .03, [r.sub.sp] = -.20 (Figure 8), durations showed greater
coordination with the prior turns and switching pauses of their partners
when they were friends than when they were acquaintances, the
girls' turn and switching-pause durations showed greater
coordination with those of their partners when they were acquaintances
than when they were friends. In short, they boys coordinated the timing
of their vocal behavior to a greater extent when they were friends than
when they were acquaintances, and the girls did the opposite.
The results also indicate that the girl's pause durations show
greater coordination with the prior pauses of their partners than do
those of the boys, regardless of whether the pairs were friends or
acquaintances or of same or mixed gender. F(1,108) = 5.78, p = .02,
[r.sub.sp] = .22. Unlike switching pauses, which comprise an
interpersonal variable in the sense of marking the boundary between the
speakers' turns and the fact that their occurrences and durations
are jointly determined, pausing is an intrapersonal behavior. That the
girls coordinated the durations of their pauses to a greater extent than
did the boys may indicate that they were more attentive and involved in
their interactions than were the boys.
Finally, there was an interaction between dyad type and dyad
gender, F(1, 108) = 5.l68,p = .02, [r.sup.sp] = -.21 (Figure 9), such
that friends coordinated the durations of their ISS with those of their
partners to a significantly greater extent in same-gender dyads than in
mixed-gender dyads, whereas acquaintances did the opposite. These
results are similar to those found previously for differences in the
durations of ISS rather than in their coordination, and suggest that
interruptive behavior is the one state that is sensitive to the gender
composition of a dyad, but only when mediated by the degree of
relationship between the partners.
DISCUSSION
Interruptive behavior is important not only because it has been
tagged as a significant predictor of coronary artery disease (Siegman,
Feldstein, Tomasso, Ringel, & Lating, 1987), but also because it is
thought to reflect dominance in interpersonal interactions. Most, if not
all of the relevant literature indicates that men interrupt more than do
women, that women are interrupted more than are men, and that, in
mixed-gender dyads, men interrupt women more than women interrupt men
(Deaux, 1976; Dindia, 1987; Eakins & Eakins, 1978; Henley, 1977;
Zimmerman & West, 1975). Dindia (1987), who published a relatively
brief but useful review of the literature, argued that these conclusions
are based upon both inadequate designs and faulty statistical analyses.
Her own study, which attempted to correct such problems, found that men
did not interrupt more than did women, and women did not get interrupted
more than did men. She also found that mixedgender pairs produced more
interruptions than same-gender pairs and that interruptions were
differentially distributed in mixed- and same-gender pairs, but not as a
function of gender. Even more persuasive is an experiment by Crown and
Cummins (1998) which demonstrated that, despite the fact that the
conversations they heard were chosen because the men and women in them
had equal numbers of objectively measure interruptive behaviors, judges
perceived women in the study to use more interruptive behavior than did
the men.
Our results suggest that the frequency, duration, and even the
interpersonal coordination of interruptive behavior depend upon the
social-relationship context within which it is used. In particular,
whether the pairs of boys or girls in this study engaged in more ISS
depended primarily upon whether they were friends or acquaintances. Even
the interruptive behavior in mixed- or same-gender dyads appeared to
differ as a function of whether the partners were friends or
acquaintances. That there were no differences in interruptive behavior
simply as a function of gender accords with the findings of Dindia
(1987) and Crown and Cummins (1998), and the findings with regard to
dyad gender are not at variance with those of Dindia (1987) or Natale,
Entin, and Jaffe (1979). That there were no simple differences between
the interruptive behaviors of boys and girls argues against the
prevalent notion that interruptive behavior reflects interpersonal
dominance, which is a second-order inference based upon men's
alleged gre ater use of such behavior. It is also in line with the adult
findings of Welkowitz, Bond, and Feldstein (1984b).
It may be more profitable to consider the interruptive behavior of
the pre- and early adolescents in somewhat broader terms. Deaux (1977)
proposed that the strategy of men in interactive contexts tends to be
competitive whereas that of women tends to be affiliative. Although the
constructs of competitiveness and dominance share certain attributes, to
be competitive is not necessarily to be domineering. Might it be that
the boys' more frequent use, and the girls' less frequent use
of ISS and NSS when they were acquaintances reflect such strategies? It
may well be that having to interact with acquaintances elicits
competitive behaviors on the part of boys because of the inherent
uncertainties about each other and the situation. But because the
situation is an interpersonal one, the same uncertainties may elicit
affiliative behavior on the part of girls. On the other hand, it may not
be as necessary for boys to be as competitive, or for girls to be as
affiliative when they are interacting with friends.
The finding that the girls engaged in more coordinated vocal
behavior when they were acquaintances than when they were friends while
the boys did the opposite presents an interesting comparison with the
findings of a number of previous studies. Field and her colleagues
(Field et al., 1992), using spectral analysis, found that there was
greater coherence in the vocal behavior of pairs of acquaintances than
of pairs of friends without regard to gender, which supports the present
findings to some extent. Crown (1991) investigated the coordination of
vocal timing in the conversations of college students who liked each
other, disliked each other, and were unacquainted with each other. She
found that the most coordination occurred in the conversations of the
pairs who were unacquainted and the pairs who disliked each other. But
all of her students were women. The results of the current study raise
the question of whether the temporal coordination of men might be
greater in their conversations with friends than with acquaintances.
The rather complex pattern of results also provides a salutary caution that things are often more complicated than they might otherwise
appear. Specifically, these results point to the importance of
considering the context of social interactions as a prime determinant of
the function of any particular behavior. It is difficult, if not
impossible to interpret, in the sense of main effects, the meaning and
function of a particular behavior. We found that understanding the
meaning of CIT in the conversations of these pre- and early adolescents
required examination of the interactions rather than main effects.
Clearly, CIT plays an important role in structuring these dialogues.
However, one cannot interpret the role of CIT without considering the
gender of each participant, the gender composition of the dyad, and the
nature of the relationship (friend or acquaintance). The necessity of
considering higher-order interactions replicates the findings of Jaffe
and colleagues (2001) regarding the role of CIT in the dev elopment of
adult dialogue. They found that the predictive utility of CIT varied
significantly as a function of infant gender, whether the infant was
interacting with mother or a stranger, the site of the interaction
(laboratory or home), and the dependent variable under consideration
(attachment or cognitive development). The degree of coordination
between infant and adult assumed a very different meaning depending upon
the context examined in any particular set of interactions. While the
scope of the present study is more circumscribed than that of Jaffe et
al., it too replicates the essential fact that the meaning of CIT can
best be understood through the sophisticated analysis of higher-order
interrelations. The subtleties of social interaction are not well
captured solely by main effects.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 7 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 8 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 9 OMITTED]
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations of the Speaking-Turn and Vocal-State
Frequencies that Comprise Vocal Behavior
T V P SP NSS ISS
MALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M 92.11 178.94 86.83 45.39 26.22 21.50
SD 15.68 56.06 54.38 14.92 14.53 11.09
Acquaintances
M 103.45 180.41 76.95 44.14 52.86 36.09
SD 30.20 51.89 36.97 19.53 54.78 22.03
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M 107.75 169.13 61.38 44.63 40.38 37.63
SD 27.37 29.17 34.06 8.37 21.56 23.13
Acquaintances
M 88.75 145.25 56.50 46.13 29.50 24.75
SD 20.84 53.60 42.50 18.16 31.18 19.48
FEMALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M 89.33 174.42 84.70 34.08 74.29 32.46
SD 43.28 77.85 54.30 23.89 77.16 16.99
Acquaintances
M 77.50 165.61 88.11 36.56 25.56 19.78
SD 26.29 55.07 54.49 10.69 21.54 17.56
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M 107.75 193.25 85.50 49.13 39.88 31.88
SD 27.47 39.83 26.84 12.37 23.81 18.73
Acquaintances
M 88.63 188.63 100.00 44.38 31.25 23.63
SD 20.79 56.81 54.87 19.17 32.84 20.85
Note. T = Speaking Turns
V = Vocalizations
P = Pauses
SP = Switching Pauses
ISS = Interruptive Simultaneous Speech
NSS = Noninterruptive Simultaneous Speech.
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations of the Speaking-Turn and Vocal-State
Durations that Comprise Vocal Behavior
T V P SP NSS ISS
MALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M 3.176 1.057 .640 .819 .661 .385
SD 1.274 .472 .218 .329 1.078 .081
Acquaintances
M 3.090 1.264 .653 .693 .470 .468
SD 1.142 .412 .184 .171 .155 .214
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M 2.682 1.117 .682 .673 .404 .377
SD 1.391 .207 .230 .263 .102 .067
Acquaintances
M 2.484 1.007 .706 .774 .469 .456
SD .722 .445 .233 .249 .123 .226
FEMALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M 4.887 1.601 .610 .784 .571 .651
SD 5.585 1.772 .294 .586 .352 .405
Acquaintances
M 4.221 1.185 .607 .749 .353 .336
SD 3.343 .429 .157 .252 .084 .079
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M 3.094 1.174 .630 .721 .404 .413
SD .938 .326 .206 .205 .073 .088
Acquaintances
M 3.740 1.177 .659 .749 .384 .406
SD 1.374 .406 .139 .167 .143 .202
Note. The average durations are in seconds. T = Speaking Turns; V =
Vocalizations; P = Pauses; SP = Switching Pauses; ISS = Interruptive
Simultaneous Speech; NSS = Noninterruptive Simultaneous Speech.
Table 3
Means and Standard Deviations of the Rs that Index the Coordination of
Each Partner's Temporal Behavior with the Prior Temporal Behavior of the
Other Partner
T V P SP NSS ISS
MALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M .216 .117 .150 .150 .142 .188
SD .138 .047 .085 .057 .087 .109
Acquaintances
M .167 .109 .108 .105 .133 .123
SD .142 .059 .066 .048 .082 .068
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M .220 .144 .094 .170 .138 .118
SD .172 .059 .035 .064 .037 .037
Acquaintances
M .264 .113 .092 .128 .140 .158
SD .261 .066 .031 .058 .081 .059
FEMALE
SAME GENDER
Friends
M .146 .108 .150 .122 .173 .142
SD .138 .047 .106 .100 .109 .117
Acquaintances
M .240 .130 .168 .142 .133 .108
SD .145 .054 .129 .071 .087 .047
MIXED GENDER
Friends
M .124 .119 .146 .118 .105 .123
SD .123 .034 .058 .060 .053 .052
Acquaintances
M .296 .139 .160 .126 .126 .140
SD .241 .049 .082 .102 .081 .056
Note. See Table 1 for definitions of the state symbols. The n for the
current coefficients is 56.
Table 4
Summary of Meta-Analyses of the Coefficients of Coordination for Friends
and Acquaintances
V P SP ISS NSS
Friends
Partner A
R .34 .37 .34 .38 .39
[chi square] 31.61 95.11 * 43.73 * 82.83 * 76.85 *
Partner B
R .33 .37 .38 .38 .36
[chi square] 34.88 52.57 96.87 * 79.84 * 82.92 *
Acquaintances
Partner A
R .34 .36 .34 .34 .35
[chi square] 30.28 66.05 * 27.75 29.41 55.47 *
Partner B
R .34 .35 .35 .36 .36
[chi square] 32.43 77.53 * 49.28 * 53.79 * 59.70 *
Note. The chi-squares index the significance of the CIT associated with
the groups of friends and acquaintances. The Rs index the strength of
the coordination.
* P < .05
REFERENCES
Beebe, B., Alson, D., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S., & Crown, C. L.
(1988). Vocal congruence in mother-infant play. Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 245-259.
Cohn, J., & Tronick, E. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face
interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles
in either partner's behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24,
386-392.
Crown, C. L. (1991). Coordinated interpersonal timing of vision and
voice as a function of interpersonal attraction. Journal of Language and
Social Psychology, 10, 29-46.
Crown, C. L., & Cummins, D. A. (1998). Objective vs. perceived
interruptions in the dialogues of unacquainted pairs, friends, and
couples. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 17(3), 372-389.
Deaux, K. (1976). The behavior of women and men. Monterey, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Deaux, K. (1977). Sex differences. In T. Blass (Ed.), Personality
variables in social behavior (pp. 357-377). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Associates.
Dindia, K. (1987). The effects of sex of subject and sex of partner
on interruptions. Human Communication Research, 13, 345-371.
Eakins, B. W., & Eakins, R. G. (1978). Sex differences in human
communication. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Feldstein, S., & Welkowitz, J. (1987). A chronography of
conversation: In defense of an objective approach. In A. W. Siegman
& S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (2nd
ed., pp. 435-499). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Associates.
Field, T., Greenwald, P., Morrow, C., Foster, T., Guthertz, M.,
Healy, B., & Frost, P. (1992). Behavior state matching during
interactions of preadolescent friends versus acquaintances.
Developmental Psychology, 28, 242-250.
Garvey, C., & BenDebba, M. (1974). Effects of age, sex, and
partner on children's dyadic speech. Child Development, 45,
1159-1161.
Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977). Topography of marital conflict: A sequential analysis of verbal and nonverbal
behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 461-577.
Henley, N. (1977). Body politics. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Jaffe, J., & Feldstein, S. (1970). Rhythms of dialogue. New
York: Academic Press.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M.
D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 66(Serial No. 265), 1-131.
Jasnow, M. D., & Feldstein, S. (1986). Adult-like temporal
characteristics of mother-infant vocal interactions. Child Development,
57, 754-761.
Natale, M., Entin, E., & Jaffe, J. (1979). Vocal interruption
in dyadic communication as a function of speech and social anxiety.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 865-878.
Siegman, A. W., Feldstein, S., Tomasso, C. T., Ringel, N., &
Lating, J. (1987). Expressive behavior and the severity of coronary
artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 545-561.
Sullivan, H. S. (1947). Conceptions of modern psychiatry.
Washington, DC: William Alanson White Foundation.
Welkowitz, J., Bond, R. N., & Feldstein, S. (1984a).
Conversational time patterns of Hawaiian children as a function of
ethnicity and gender. Language and Speech, 27, 173-191.
Welkowitz, J., Bond, R. N., & Feldstein, S. (1984b). Gender and
conversational time patterns of Japanese-American adults and children in
same- and mixed-gender dyads. Journal of Language and Social Psychology,
3, 127-138.
Welkowitz, J., Cariffe, G., & Feldstein, S. (1976).
Conversational congruence as a criterion of socialization in children.
Child Development, 47, 269-272.
Zimmerman, D. H., & West, C. (1975). Sex roles, interruptions
and silences in conversation. In B. Thorn & H. Henley (Eds.),
Language and sex: Differences and dominance (pp. 105-129). Rowley, MA:
Newbury House.
The authors are indebted to the Academic Computing Center of the
University of Maryland, Baltimore County, for making their facilities
and time readily available. They are also indebted to Dr. Michael Jasnow
for his invaluable comments. Finally, the authors are grateful for the
participation and cooperation of the teachers, parents, and children at
the West Laboratory Elementary School in Miami, Florida.
Tiffany Field, Touch Research Institutes, University of Miami School of Medicine, Florida.
Reprint requests to Stanley Feldstein, Department of Psychology,
University of Maryland, Baltimore County, 1000 Hilltop Circle,
Baltimore, Maryland 21250. Electronic mail may be sent to
feldstei@)umhc.edu.