Massage therapy improves mood and behavior of students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Khilnani, Sonya ; Field, Tiffany ; Hernandez-Reif, Maria 等
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most recent
diagnostic label for children and adolescents who present with
attention, impulse control, and overactivity problems. Children and
adolescents with ADHD are commonly referred to family physicians,
pediatricians, pediatric neurologists, and child psychiatrists and
psychologists. The attention-deficit category has become increasingly
popular among clinicians, and while the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-1V-TR; American Psychiatric Association,
2000) suggests that only 3% to 7% of all school-age children and
adolescents are affected by the disorder, at least 10% of behavior
problems seen in general pediatric settings are due to ADHD, and up to
50% in some child and adolescent psychiatric samples. In clinical
samples, ADHD is diagnosed in nine males for every female, however the
rate of ADHD among girls is rapidly increasing (Robison, Skaer,
Sclar,& Galin, 2002). ADHD girls who are clinic-referred are as
impaired as their male counterparts in inattention, internalizing
behavior, and peer aggression.
ADHD not only has a financial impact, but also is associated with
family stress, school disruption, and risk for criminality and substance
abuse. School-age youths with ADHD and co-occurring psychopathology have
inferior academic performance and poorer social, emotional, and adaptive
functioning than their peers (Wilens, Biederman, Brown, Tanguay,
Monuteaux, Blake, & Spencer, 2002).
ADHD's etiology is unknown, although multiple pathways have
been suggested for this syndrome. Among those are heritability, which is
high for ADHD. According to DSM-IV-TR, ADHD is more common in the
primary biological relatives of children and adolescents with ADHD than
in the general population. Neurophysiological theories have also been
investigated. Porges (1984, 1998), for example, examined the physiologic
correlates of attention. The inability to attend appropriately has been
associated with a variety of diagnoses, including hyperactivity and
learning disorders. Porges (1998) has suggested that these behavioral
pathologies have a common physiological substrate. The inability of the
hyperactive or learning-disabled child/adolescent to mediate and inhibit
spontaneous activity is thought to be paralleled by a deficient
inhibitory system manifested in the parasympathetic control of the
heart. That is, individuals who exhibit an attention disorder may
demonstrate relatively lower vagal tone, indicated by heart rate
variability during periods when sustained attention is required.
Children with ADHD may vary considerably in their symptoms across
situations. However, they are often described as having chronic
difficulties in regard to inattention, impulsivity, and
overactivity--the "holy trinity" of ADHD. Barkley (1990) notes
that ADHD children/ adolescents commonly display these characteristics
early, to a degree that is inappropriate for their age, and across a
variety of situations. According to DSM-IV-TR criteria, symptoms must be
"maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level." The
symptoms must also be present across two or more settings.
Comorbidity
Early-onset ADHD frequently co-occurs with other disorders, and
symptom overlap raises diagnostic problems. Common mimics of attention
disorders are anxiety disorders and mood disorders, and these must be
carefully ruled out.
ADHD is often comorbid with other disorders such as learning
disabilities (LD), internalizing disorders, and externalizing disorders.
Children with both LD and ADHD have more sociobehavioral problems than
those with LD alone, and the former group comprises between 25% and 31%
of students in LD classrooms (Forness, Kavale, & San Miguel-Bauman,
1998). About one-fourth of children/adolescents who are anxiety
disordered have a comorbid diagnosis of ADHD (Perrin & Last, 1995).
Between 10% and 20% of children with ADHD have mood disorders, 20%
have conduct disorders (CD), and up to 40% may have oppositional defiant
disorder (ODD) (Goldman, Gonel, Bezman, & Slanetz, 1998). In
addition to the high level of comorbidity with anxiety disorders, ADHD
often coexists with depressive disorders (Bussing, Zima, Belin, &
Forness, 1998). This overlap is more common in ADHD children/
adolescents who also experience learning problems. For those with a
combination of early-onset ADHD and CD or ODD, there is a more
persistent and stable pattern of antisocial behavior. More severe
degrees of psychopathology and psychosocial risk occur in youths with
both ADHD and an externalizing disorder. ADHD-CD comorbidity has been
found to be associated with nonalcohol substance use disorder, drinking
levels, and CD severity (Molina, Bukstein & Lynch, 2002). The risk
of developing substance use disorders in those with ADHD increases
during adolescence (Goldman et al., 1998). Lower attention/
attention-functioning scores in adolescents have been found to predict
substance use and dependence symptoms eight years later (Tapert,
Baratta, Abrantes, & Brown, 2002). Even as college students, those
with ADHD symptoms experience more driving anger and display more
hostility/aggression and risky behavior on the road. They are prone to
be involved in more crash-related outcomes and tend to display their
anger in socially unacceptable ways (Richards, Deffenbacher,& Ros,
2002).
Medication Therapy
Psychostimulant medication is the most widespread treatment for
ADHD. More children/adolescents receive medication to manage ADHD than
any other childhood disorder. Recent reports suggest that prescriptions
for psychotropic drugs are increasing among children with ADHD (Guevara,
Lozano, Wickizer, Mell, & Gephart, 2002). Lately, more girls are
being prescribed stimulants as a result of the broadening
conceptualization of ADHD (Goldman et al., 1998)--the initial focus on
hyperactivity has shifted to attentional problems and impulsivity. The
mean number of office-based visits documenting a diagnosis of ADHD among
girls tripled in the 1990s, whereas the number for boys increased about
twofold (Robison, Skaer, Sclar,& Galin, 2002).
Currently, the disorder consists of the following three subtypes:
predominantly inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, and combined.
According to the DSM-IV, most children/adolescents have the combined
type. The most common form of stimulant medication is methylphenidate,
otherwise known as Ritalin. Other medications such as Adderall,
Dexedrine, and Cylert are also used. Numerous studies (double-blind,
placebo-controlled) have concluded that stimulants are more effective in
ameliorating ADHD's core behavioral symptoms of hyperactivity,
impulsivity, and inattentiveness than placebos, nonpharmacological
therapies, or no treatment (Spencer, Biederman, Coffey, Geller,
Crawford, Bearman, Tarazi, & Faraone, 2002). Improvement in
dysfunctional social behavior and internalizing symptoms has also been
reported (Schachar, Jadad, Gauld, Boyle, Booker, Snider, Kim, &
Cunningham, 2002).
There is little empirical evidence that stimulant use contributes
to longer-term improvements in academic functioning. Moreover, this mode
of treatment continues to be controversial with this population because
of its behavior-modifying properties and associated side effects. The
following adverse side effects increase linearly with dosage:
nervousness, headache, insomnia, and tachycardia (Klein-Schwartz, 2002).
Further, as the therapeutic use of stimulants increases, the risk of
abuse, overdose, and medication errors may also increase. According to
Goldman et al. (1998), stimulants as a class of drugs have "marked
abuse potential, and their misuse can have severe medical and social
consequences." Clinical manifestations of overdose include
agitation, hallucinations, psychosis, lethargy, seizures, tachycardia,
dysrhythmia, hypertension, and hyperthermia (Klein-Schwartz, 2002).
Moreover, in a study of 223 ADHD children aged three years and younger,
over half received psychotropic medication in an idiosyncratic manner
and almost half did not have opportunities for optimal monitoring
(Rappley, Eneli, Mullan, Alvarez, Wang, Luo, & Gardiner, 2002).
Nonpharmacological Therapy
Although pharmacotherapy remains the current treatment of choice
for ADHD children, benefits from nondrug treatments have been noted. For
example, Wilmshurst (2002) reported good results for youths from a
home-based preservation program in terms of reductions in clinical
symptoms of ADHD, as well as general anxiety and depression, at one-year
follow-up. However, behavioral therapy has generally not proven
effective except when combined with pharmacotherapy (Barkley, 1990).
ADHD researchers emphasize the importance of employing a multimodal
treatment approach with ADHD children. Multimodal therapy involves
integrating pharmacotherapy with a number of environmental, educational,
psychotherapeutic and school-based approaches to meet the child's
particular needs. A combined intervention of medication management and
behavioral treatment has been found to be more successful than either
approach alone in reducing core ADHD symptoms in children from more
educated families (Rieppi et al., 2002). In their literature review of
the long-term treatment of ADHD, Schachar et al. (2002) noted that
combination therapy adds to the effects of medication.
Massage Therapy
A nonmedication intervention that has only recently been explored
with ADHD children is massage therapy. In a recent study, ADHD
adolescents who received ten massage treatments over the course of two
weeks rated themselves as happier than those who participated in
relaxation therapy; observers rated the massage therapy group as less
fidgety, and teachers reported more on-task behavior, when compared to
the relaxation therapy group (Field, Quintino, Hernandez-Reif, &
Koslovsky, 1998). Teachers also noticed a significant decrease in
hyperactivity for the massage therapy group but not for the relaxation
therapy group.
Similar changes have been noted for child and adolescent
psychiatric inpatients diagnosed with adjustment and depressive
disorders; they showed significantly less depression and anxiety
following massage therapy (Field, Morrow, Valdeon, Larson, Kuhn, &
Schanberg, 1992). The massage group's salivary and urinary cortisol levels decreased, indicating a decrease in stress. Staff nurses also
rated the inpatients who received massage therapy as being less anxious
and more cooperative when compared to a control group, which viewed
relaxing videotapes. In addition, nightwakings decreased and time spent
in quiet sleep increased for the massage group.
Touch therapies have also been successful in reducing off-task
behavior in autistic children. In one study, massage therapy with
autistic children led to a decrease in both off-task behavior and
attention to irrelevant sounds and to an increase in social relatedness
during classroom observations (Field, Lasko, Mundy, Henteleff, Talpins,
& Dowling, 1996). Another study involving autistic children, many of
whom had sensory processing deficits, suggested that physiological
stimulation from deep touch pressure (a hug machine) reduced hyperactive
and self-stimulatory behaviors (Imamura, Wiess, & Parham, 1990).
Furthermore, massage therapy has been shown to significantly reduce
anxiety as well as systolic and diastolic blood pressure (Cady &
Jones, 1997; Shulman & Jones, 1996). In addition, Field, Ironson,
Scafidi, Nawrocki, Goncalves, Burman, Pickens, Fox, Schanberg, and Kuhn
(1996) have reported an increase in alertness and EEG wave changes
conforming to increased alertness (decreased alpha and beta and
increased delta) and better performance on math tasks (half the time
required with half the errors) following massage therapy. These studies
suggest that touch therapy may attenuate stress and enhance
attentiveness in children and adolescents with ADHD. Improved
psychological functioning would be expected to result in less acting-out
behaviors in the classroom.
The underlying mechanism by which massage therapy decreases
hyperactivity and increases attentiveness is not clear, although
physiological and biochemical data from the Field et al. studies suggest
some possibilities, including that brain waves are altered in the
direction of heightened alertness (see Field et al., 1996). In addition,
increased vagal tone (and thus increased parasympathetic activity) has
been noted during massage therapy, and this increase is often associated
with enhanced attentiveness and a more relaxed state (Porges, 1991).
Massage therapy may enhance vagal control of the heart by improving a
deficient physiological inhibitory system. This, in turn, might help
hyperactive or learning disordered children to mediate and inhibit
spontaneous activity and thereby increase their level of attentiveness.
Those with each subtype of ADHD are expected to benefit from massage
therapy given that the attentional deficit is considered the primary
symptom underlying hyperactivity-impulsivity (Dykman, Ackerman,
Clements, & Peters, 1971). The restlessness is assumed to be
secondary and reflective of diffuse patterns of sensory discharge in the
brain (activating motor areas).
In the present investigation, massage therapy was selected as an
additional treatment for those receiving ongoing intervention, because
prior studies found that it exceeded the effects of relaxation therapy
and other stress management treatments used in various clinical samples,
including youths with ADHD (Field et al., 1992, 1998). Previous massage
studies also reported increases in vagal tone during massage therapy
(Field, 1995). It would follow that massage therapy might improve
attention in those with ADHD by promoting vagal control of heart rate.
The present study explored the effects of massage therapy on
behavioral, emotional, and physiological functioning in ADHD
children/adolescents who were receiving special education. The calming
effects of massage therapy were expected to ameliorate common behavioral
symptoms of ADHD, including restlessness, inattention, and impulsivity.
Emotional correlates, such as depressed mood, were also expected to
decrease. Specifically, the following hypotheses were tested: (1)
students in the massage therapy group would rate themselves as happier
and feeling better postmassage on both assessment days when compared to
those in the wait-list control group; (2) students who received massage
therapy would show a significant decrease in salivary cortisol (a
measure of stress) postmassage on both assessment days when compared to
those in the wait-list control group; (3) the massage therapy group
would show a significant reduction in behavioral problems in the
classroom, specifically hyperactivity and inattention (based on teacher
ratings), when compared to the wait-list control group; and (4) the
massage therapy group would show a significant decrease in depressed
mood when compared to the wait-list control group.
METHOD
Participants
Parental consent forms were distributed to all students attending a
learning center for children and adolescents with academic and
behavioral problems. The sample consisted of the first 30 students who
returned signed consent forms and who met the following criteria. Each
participant had a current DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD. Students with other
complex medical issues, such as cerebral palsy or a known organic brain
dysfunction, were excluded from the study. In addition, all participants
had initial T scores in the subclinical to clinical range (i.e., 60 or
above) on the hyperactivity and/or inattention subscales of the Conners
Teacher Rating Scale to confirm that they were currently displaying
symptoms associated with ADHD. Students whose T scores fell below 60
were excluded from the study to ensure a truly clinical sample.
Fifty-seven percent of the entire sample scored in the subclinical to
clinical range (i.e., 85th percentile or above) on both hyperactivity
and inattention. At intake, group means for depression and anxiety did
not fall within the clinical range. Therefore, comorbidity was not a
salient issue in the present study.
The participants were between the ages of 7 and 18 years (M = 13
years). Eighty percent were male and 20% were female. They came from
middle socioeconomic backgrounds (M = 2.5 on the Hollingshead Two Factor
Index), and the ethnic distribution was 77% Caucasian, 13% Hispanic, and
10% African-American.
Procedure
Massage therapy. The students were randomly assigned to a massage
therapy group or wait-list control group. The massages were held in a
large, quiet room located in the school building. Each student in the
massage therapy group received two 20-minute massages per week for a
total of nine treatment sessions. Massages were conducted on portable
massage tables, and all participants remained fully clothed during each
massage. Students were told that the massages might help them relax. The
massage entailed moderate-pressure stroking for four-minute periods in
each of five regions: head/neck, arms, torso, legs, and back. Massage
while in the supine position lasted ten minutes and included lateral
stroking of the forehead, gentle rocking (torso and legs), and
continuous stretching of the Achilles tendon. Massage while in the prone
position also lasted ton minutes and included lateral lumbar stretches,
neck squeezes, and kneading of the back.
The massages were given at the same time of day (mid-afternoon)
over the course of a month. Treatments were performed by licensed
massage therapists and coordinated by the first author. The gender of
the massage therapists was counterbalanced. Last day assessments were
conducted after the eighth (penultimate) massage session instead of the
ninth in order to minimize potential termination effects such as feeling
disappointed that the massages were over.
Wait-list control group. Participants in this group were informed
that they would have an opportunity to experience the massage procedure
on a voluntary basis during the following month and that the reason all
students were not massaged at the same time was due to the limited
number of available massage therapists. Like the massage therapy group,
the wait-list control group completed the identical short-term and
longer-term measures during the same time frame. However, the wait-list
control group did not receive any massages for the first month of the
study. For the assessment sessions, they were asked simply to relax for
the 20-minute period.
Short-Term Measures
Pre-post measures were administered at the first session and
read-ministered four weeks later, at the eighth (penultimate) session,
for the massage group. These measures were also administered at the
first and second assessment sessions for the control group. The intent
was to assess the immediate effects of the treatment.
Stress. Salivary samples were obtained (participants placed a
dental swab dipped in sugar-free lemonade crystals along their gumline
for 30 seconds) immediately before and 20 minutes after the sessions (on
the assessment days) to be assayed for cortisol (a hormone indicative of
stress level). They were collected 20 minutes postsession because the
cortisol response has a 20-minute lag time. The second sample of saliva
reflected the participants' stress level during the massage
therapy/control session.
Mood state. Information about mood state was collected through the
developmentally appropriate use of pictorial self-reports. A faces scale
was used to provide an estimate of the participants' experiences.
This pictorial measure was a modification of the self-report method
designed to assess pediatric patients' anxiety and pain during
invasive medical procedures, such as bone marrow aspirations (LeBaron
& Zeltzer, 1984). LeBaron and Zeltzer used faces showing increasing
degrees of distress and found a strong correlation (r = .63, p <
.001) between patient ratings and observer ratings of anxiety. In the
present study, drawings of four faces ranging from sad (scored as one)
to happy (scored as four) were presented to the participants before and
after the first and last assessment sessions (for both massage and
control groups). The participants were then asked which face best
described the way they felt at that moment. This scale's utility in
previous massage studies supported its use as a self-report measure in
this investigation.
A modification of the Children's Pain/Fear Thermometer Rating
Scale (a drawing of a thermometer with a vertical scale) was used as a
second measure of mood state. Participants were asked to select the
point on the scale, ranging from zero (i.e., not feeling good at all) to
ten (i.e., best I have ever felt), that best described the way they
felt. The original scale was used in a study of pain in pediatric cancer
patients undergoing bone marrow aspirations (Jay, Ozolins, Elliot, &
Caldwell, 1983). In that study, pain ratings were significantly
correlated (r = .67, p < .02) with observed behavior in youths aged 8
years and older. In the current study, this scale was administered
before and after the first and last assessment sessions (for both
massage and control groups).
Longer-Term Measure
Teachers (who were blind to group assignment) were asked to
complete the Conners Teacher Rating Scale, which provided an index of
changes in classroom behavior across the course of the study. This
measure was used at the first session and four weeks later for both
groups.
Classroom behavior. The Conners Teacher Rating Scale (Conners,
1969) is one of the most widely used behavior rating scales for
assessing externalizing symptoms in children. The following six factors
from this 39-item scale were examined: hyperactivity, conduct problems,
emotional-indulgent, anxious-passive, asocial, and daydream/attention
problems. Test-retest reliability coefficients over a one-month interval
ranged from .70 to .90 across factors (Conners, 1973). Inter-teacher
agreement of .92 was reported for the entire scale (Trites, Blouin,
& Laprade, 1982).
RESULTS
A t test was performed on age, and chi-square tests were performed
on gender and ethnicity. No significant differences were noted between
the massage group and wait-list control group on these background
variables.
Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted with
the massage therapy and wait-list control groups as the grouping
variable and session (pre, post) and day (first, last) as the repeated
measures. Significant interaction effects were followed by
alpha-corrected post hoc comparison t tests.
Pre-Post Treatment Measures (Immediate Effects)
Faces. The ANOVA on the faces scale revealed a significant group by
session interaction effect, F(1, 27) = 5.46, p < .05. Post hoc t
tests revealed that the massage therapy group reported feeling happier
after the first and last day sessions (see Table 1).
Thermometer. The ANOVA on the thermometer scale revealed a
significant group by session interaction effect, F(1, 27) = 4.70, p <
.05. Post hoc t tests indicated that the massage therapy group rated
themselves as feeling bettor after the first and last day sessions (see
Table 1).
Cortisol. No significant effects were obtained for salivary
cortisol in either group (see Table 1).
First-Last Day Measures (Longer-Term Effects)
Conners Teacher Rating Scale. There were significant group by day
interaction effects, showing reductions in hyperactivity, F(1, 28) =
7.92, p < .01, anxiety, F(1, 28) = 14.70, p < .01, and
daydreaming/ inattention, F(1, 28) = 4.42, p < .05, only for the
massage group. Both groups improved on the emotional-indulgent factor,
F(1, 28) = 14.39, p = .001 (see Table 1).
DISCUSSION
Massage therapy appeared to benefit the children and adolescents
with ADHD. Students who received massage therapy twice per week over the
course of a month rated themselves as happier and feeling relatively
better after the treatment sessions. The present findings are consistent
with a recent massage therapy study in which ADHD adolescents showed
improvements in fidgetiness, hyperactivity, on-task behavior, and
subjective feelings of happiness (Field et el., 1998) and another study
that reported less anxiety after massage therapy (Shulman & Jones,
1996).
The enhanced mood states found in the present study may have
contributed to the improved classroom behavior. Teacher ratings of ADHD
students who participated in the massage therapy suggest that the
therapy reduced the problems most associated with ADHD, namely
hyperactivity and daydreaming/inattention, over the course of the
treatment period. The convergence of self-report measures and teacher
ratings highlights both the immediate and longer-term effectiveness of
massage therapy and supports the use of this treatment with this
population.
As predicted, ADHD students who received massage therapy showed
improvements in short-term (immediate) mood state and longer-term (over
the course of a month) classroom behavior. While the findings look
promising, they cannot be definitively applied to all ADHD students. The
participants in the present study attended a small, private learning
center and came from middle to upper socioeconomic status families. As
previously noted, they also were receiving school-based interventions.
Whether massage therapy would benefit students who had other
socioeconomic backgrounds and were in regular classrooms at larger,
public schools remains to be studied.
Future studies are needed to ascertain how massage therapy impacts
academic achievement in students with ADHD. Incorporating additional
measures such as vagal tone and EEG might also help explain the
relationship between massage therapy and on-task behavior in ADHD
children and adolescents. Several studies suggest that massage therapy
enhances cognitive performance (Hart, Field, Hernandez-Reif, &
Lundy, 1998), including improved math computations following EEG changes
to a pattern of heightened alertness (Field, Ironson, Scafidi, Nawrocki,
Goncalves, Burman, Pickens, Fox, Schanberg, & Kuhn, 1996). In the
Hart et el. (1998) study, massaged children improved their performance
on the Block Design and Animal Pegs subtests of the Wechsler Preschool
and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R). In the present
study, the increased attentiveness observed in the classroom (using the
Conners scale) could be related to enhanced vagal activity that occurs
during massage therapy (Field, 1990). Increased vagal control of the
heart may enhance the ability of the hyperactive child to mediate
spontaneous activity (Porges, 1998).
Anxious-passive behaviors measured by the Conners scale also
decreased for the massage therapy group in the present study. This is
consistent with another massage therapy study in which child and
adolescent psychiatric inpatients displayed fewer internalizing
behaviors following massage (Field et al., 1992). However, unlike
previous studies by Field and colleagues which noted decreased stress
hormones, no decrease in cortisol levels was found here. This may have
been due to the time of sampling. In the present study, saliva samples
were obtained during the mid-afternoon, a time when cortisol levels are
at their lowest and thus difficult to reduce further. Moreover,
decreases in salivary cortisol are not necessarily related to reduced
activity level (Field et al., 1992). Specific relationships between
physiological responses and massage therapy may be more difficult to
establish in children due to variability of responses--shaped
differentially by environmental events. There is also some evidence that
the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis may be underactive in
neuropsychiatrically disordered children. In one study, salivary
cortisol was significantly lower in a comorbid ADHD/ODD group compared
with healthy controls (Kariyawasam, Zaw, & Handley, 2002); this
finding suggests underarousal in behaviorally disturbed children.
Nevertheless, other, longer-term measures of stress, such as urinary
cortisol and norepinephrine levels, might have revealed significant
changes in the present study.
In this investigation, initial group means for depression and
anxiety did not fall within the clinical range. Therefore, comorbidity
was not a salient issue. In future studies, it would be useful to
ascertain whether massage therapy is effective in ameliorating not only
short-term mood state in ADHD students, as the present study
demonstrated, but also chronic mood disorders.
While teachers and school staff in general supported the use of
massage therapy for students, concerns were also raised about how these
youths would respond to extensive touch, since those with ADHD usually
do not remain still for prolonged periods of time. For example, a few of
the children were initially fidgety at the beginning of the massage,
whereas the adolescents seemed more comfortable with the procedure.
However, the younger participants became less fidgety with time.
Although massage therapy appears to improve short-term mood state
and decrease problem behaviors in the classroom in students with ADHD,
its efficacy needs to be compared with other treatments such as tai chi
therapy, which has also been effective with ADHD children
(Hernandez-Reif, Field, & Thimas, 1999). In addition, assessing
whether the massage therapy effects generalized to other settings would
have been an important outcome to explore (e.g., using the Conners
Parent Rating Scale to document the effects of massage therapy in the
home). Nonetheless, the findings from both teacher ratings and
self-reports suggest that massage therapy could be an important tool in
the multimodal management of ADHD.
Table 1
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Massage Versus Control Group on
Pre-Post Session and Session and First-Last Day Measures
Massage Group
First Day
Variables Pre Post
Mood and Stress
Faces 3.1[(0.8)sub.a] 3.5[(0.6).sub.b] (1)
Thermometer 6.3[(2.4).sub.a] 7.8[(2.1).sub.b] (3)
Cortisol 1.3[(0.6).sub.a] 1.3[(0.3).sub.a]
Last Day
Variables Pre Post
Mood and Stress
Faces 3.3[(0.8).sub.a] 3.7[(0.5).sub.b] (2)
Thermometer 6.9[(2.8).sub.a] 8.7[(1.6).sub.b] (2)
Cortisol 1.5[(0.3).sub.a] 1.4[(0.6).sub.a]
Behavior (T scores) First Day Last Day
Hyperactivity 62.9[(13.7).sub.a] 54.2[(10.4).sub.b] (4)
Conduct Problems 58.7[(16.4).sub.a] 54.1[(13.3).sub.a]
Emotional-Indulgent 63.9[(19.5).sub.a] 55.3[(14.4).sub.b] (1)
Anxious-Passive 48.9[(10.5).sub.a] 44.3[(7.0).sub.b] (3)
Asocial 47.8[(8.6).sub.a] 46.7[(6.9).sub.a]
Daydream/Attention 61.2[(10.2).sub.a] 52.7[(7.0).sub.b] (4)
Problems
Control Group
First Day
Variables Pre Post
Mood and Stress
Faces 3.3[(0.6.sub.a] 3.4[(0.5).sub.a]
Thermometer 7.5[(1.8).sub.a] 7.8[(2.0).sub.a]
Cortisol 1.4[(0.7).sub.a] 1.3[(0.9).sub.a]
Last Day
Variables Pre Post
Mood and Stress
Faces 3.7[(0.5).sub.a] 3.7[(0.5).sub.a]
Thermometer 8.4[(1.9).sub.a] 8.6[(1.8).sub.a]
Cortisol 1.6[(1.1).sub.a] 1.5[(0.8).sub.a]
Behavior (T scores) First Day Last Day
Hyperactivity 63.0[(8.9).sub.a] 60.5[(10.0).sub.a]
Conduct Problems 57.4[(11.5).sub.a] 57.0[(12.1).sub.a]
Emotional-Indulgent 65.9[(9.1).sub.a] 61.5[(9.9).sub.b] (2)
Anxious-Passive 53.5[(10.9).sub.a] 56.0[(9.9).sub.a]
Asocial 54.5[(11.1).sub.a] 55.3[(12.6).sub.a]
Daydream/Attention 58.2[(58.2).sub.a] 56.4[(10.4).sub.a]
Problems
Note. Different letter subscripts indicate significant differences
within groups using post hoc comparison t tests. Superscripts
indicate level of significance for adjacent means: (1) p = .05,
(2) p = .01, (3) p = .005, (4) p = .001.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision).
Washington, DC: Author.
Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A
handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
Press.
Bussing, R., Zima, B. T., Belin, T. R., & Forness, S. R.
(1998). Children who qualify for LD and SED programs: Do they differ in
level of ADHD symptoms and comorbid psychiatric conditions? Behavioral
Disorders, 23, 85-97.
Cady, S. H., & Jones, G. E. (1997). Massage therapy as a
workplace intervention for reduction of stress. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 84, 157-158.
Conners, C. K. (1969). A teacher rating scale for use in drug
studies with children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 884-889.
Conners, C. K. (1973). Rating scales for use in drug studies with
children. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 14, 24-42.
Dykman, R. A., Ackerman, P. T., Clements, S. D., & Peters, J.
E. (1971). Specific learning disabilities: An attentional deficit
syndrome. In H. R. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities.
New York: Grune and Stratton.
Field, T. (1990, April). Newborn behavior, vagal tone and
catecholamine activity in cocaine exposed infants. Symposium presented
at the International Society of Infant Studies, Montreal, Canada.
Field, T. (1995). Touch in early development. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Field, T., Ironson, G., Scafidi, F., Nawrocki, T., Goncalves, A.,
Burman, I., Pickens, J., Fox, N., Schanberg, S., & Kuhn, C. (1996).
Massage therapy reduces anxiety and enhances EEG pattern of alertness
and math computations. International Journal of Neuroscience, 86,
197-205.
Field, T., Lasko, D., Mundy, P., Henteleff, T., Talpins, S., &
Dowling, M. (1996). Autistic children's attentiveness and
responsivity improved after touch therapy. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 27, 329-334.
Field, T., Morrow, C., Valdeon, C., Larson, S., Kuhn, C., &
Schanberg, S. (1992). Massage reduces anxiety in child and adolescent
psychiatric patients. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 37-48.
Field, T. M., Quintino, O., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Koslovsky, G.
(1998). Adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder benefit from massage therapy. Adolescence, 33, 103-108.
Forness, S. R., Kavale, K.A., & San Miguel-Bauman, S. (1998).
The psychiatric comorbidity hypothesis revisited. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 21, 203-206.
Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J., & Slanetz, P. J.
(1998). Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Medical
Association. 279, 1100-1107.
Guevara, J., Lozano, P., Wickizer, T., Mell, L., & Gophart, H.
(2002). Psychotropic medication use in a population of children who have
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 109, 733-739.
Hart, S., Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Lundy, B. (1998).
Preschoolers' cognitive performance improves following massage.
Early Childhood Development and Care, 143, 59-64.
Hernandez-Reif, M., Field, T., & Thimas, E. (1999). ADHD
benefits from tai chi. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 5,
120-123.
Imamura, K. N., Wiess, T., & Parham, D. (1990). The effects of
hug machine usage on behavioral organization of children with autism and
autistic-like characteristics. Sensory Integra Quarterly, 27, 1-5.
Jay, S. M., Ozolins, M., Elliott, C. H., & Caldwell, S. (1983).
Assessment of children's distress during painful medical
procedures. Health Psychology, 52, 729-738.
Kariyawasam, Z. H., Zaw, F., & Handley, S. L. (2002). Reduced
salivary cortisol in children with comorbid attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder.
Neuroendocrinology Letters, 23, 45-48.
Klein-Schwartz, W. (2002). Abuse and toxicity of methylphenidate.
Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 14, 219-223.
LeBaron, S., & Zeltzer, L. (1984). Assessment of acute pain and
anxiety in children and adolescents by self-reports, observer reports,
and a behavior checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
52, 729-738.
Molina, B. S., Bukstein, O. G., & Lynch, K.G. (2002).
Attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder and conduct disorder
symptomatology in adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Psychology of
Addictive Behavior, 16, 161-164.
Perrin, S., & Last, C. (1995). Dealing with comorbidity. In A.
R. Eisen, C. A. Kearney, & C. E. Schaefer (Eds.), Clinical handbook
of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 412-435). New
Jersey: Jason Aronson.
Porges, S. W. (1984). Physiologic correlates of attention: A core
process underlying learning disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North
America, 31, 371-385.
Porges, S. W. (1991). Vagal tone: A mediator of affect. In J. A.
Garber & K.A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of affect regulation and
dysregulation (pp. 111-128). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Porges, S. W. (1998). Love: An emergent property of the mammalian
autonomic nervous system. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 837-861.
Rappley, M. D., Eneli, I. U., Mullan, P. B., Alvarez, F. J., Wang,
J., Luo, Z., & Gardiner, J. C. (2002). Patterns of psychotropic
medication use in very young children with attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics, 23, 23-30.
Richards, T., Deffenbacher, J., & Ros, L. (2002). Driving anger
and other driving-related behaviors in high and low ADHD symptom college
students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 6, 25-38.
Rieppi, R., Greenhill, L. L., Ford, R. E., Chuang, S., Wu, M.,
Davies, M., Abikoff, H. B., Arnold, L. E., Conners, C. K., Elliot, G.R.,
Hechtman, L., Hinshaw, S. P., Hoza, B., Jensen, P. S., Kraemer, H. C.,
March, J. S., Newcorn, J. H., Pelham, W. E., Severe, J. B., Swanson, J.
M., Vitiello, B., Wells, K. C., & Wigal, T. (2002). Socioeconomic
status as a moderator of ADHD treatment outcomes. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 269-277.
Robison, L. M., Skaer, T. L., Sclar, D. A., & Galin, R. S.
(2002). Is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder increasing Among
girls in the US? Trends in diagnosis and the prescribing of stimulants.
CNS Drugs, 16, 129-137.
Schachar, R., Jadad, A. R., Gauld, M., Boyle, M., Booker, L.,
Snider, A., & Cunningham, C. (2002). Attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder: Critical appraisal of extended treatment studies. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 337-348.
Shulman, K. R., & Jones, G. E. (1996). The effectiveness of
massage therapy intervention on reducing Anxiety in the workplace.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 32, 160-173.
Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Coffey, B., Geller, D., Crawford, M.,
Bearman, S. K., Tarazi, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2002). A double-blind
comparison of desipramine and placebo in children and adolescents with
chronic tic disorder and comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 649-656.
Tapert, S. F., Baratta, M. V., Abrantes, A. M, & Brown, S. A.
(2002). Attention dysfunction predicts substance involvement in
community youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 680-686.
Trites, R. L., Blouin, A. G., & Laprade, K. (1982). Factor
analysis of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale based on a large normative
sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 615-662.
Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., Brown, S., Tanguay, S., Monuteaux, M.
C., Blake, C., & Spencer, T. J. (2002). Psychiatric comorbidity and
functioning in clinically referred preschool children and school-age
youths with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 262-268.
Wilmshurst, L. A. (2002). Treatment programs for youth with
emotional and behavioral disorders: An outcome study of two alternate
approaches. Mental Health Services Research, 4, 85-96.
The authors would like to thank the school staff, students,
parents, and massage therapists who participated in this project. This
research partially satisfied the requirements for a doctoral degree for
Sonya Khilnani and was supported by funds from Johnson & Johnson
awarded to the Touch Research Institutes.
Sonya Khilnani, Touch Research Institutes, University of Miami School of Medicine.
Tiffany Field, Touch Research Institutes, University of Miami
School of Medicine.
Saul Schanberg, Duke University School of Medicine.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Maria Hernandez-Reif, Touch
Research Institutes, University of Miami School of Medicine, P.O. Box
016820 (D-820), Miami, Florida 33101. E-mail:
[email protected]