Attachment and motivational strategies in adolescence: exploring links.
Soares, Isabel ; Lemos, Marina S. ; Almeida, Cristina 等
INTRODUCTION
Within the scope of motivational theory, psychological needs are
fundamental for understanding human development. The needs for
relatedness, for competence, and for autonomy are critical to
understanding development and action, namely in challenging and
threatening situations (Connell, 1990; Connell & Wellborn, 1991;
Skinner & Wellborn, 1994). The idea that individuals play a central
role in shaping their behavior by selecting and interpreting events and
reacting to them, is related to an individual's beliefs about
one's interactions with the environment, are more specifically,
beliefs associated with the three basic needs: emotional security,
perceived competence, and perceived autonomy. Contextual conditions also
influence individuals' behavior. Contents may allow (or impose) the
satisfaction of these three needs depending on, respectively, the terms
of involvement (versus neglect), structure (versus chaos), and autonomy
(versus coercion) (Skinner & Wellborn, 1994). Internal working
models of attachment, perceived control, and autonomy are viewed as
self-system processes associated with these three needs. During
development, these three self-system processes are built on the basis of
individuals' interactions. Through early interactions with a
supportive and sensitive attachment figure and through successful
independent attempts, the child will develop a representation of the
self as worthy of love, competent, and autonomous, as will it represent
the world as responsive, predictable, and allowing freedom. However,
these three basic needs may be challenged by such contextual factors as
unavailability of the attachment figure, or chaotic and coercive situations. This may result in insecure representations of the self and
of the world as well as negative expectations.
While there is extensive agreement about the importance of these
three basic needs for behavioral development, the relations among them
are still mostly unknown. Although some theoretical hypotheses have been
advanced (e.g., Wellborn, 1995), only a few studies (e.g., Maslin-Cole
& Spieker, 1990) have empirically explored the dynamics among these
needs.
A Goal Approach to Motivation and Action
In the context of the present investigation, needs are viewed as
key motivational constructs, which give purpose and meaning to behavior
and whose influence is not only direct, but mainly mediated by goals. In
this sense, we explore the role of behavior finality through analysis of
individuals' motivational goals. Goals are conceived of as a
cognitive elaboration of needs. Whereas the need concept is vague,
generic, and hardly open to empirical examination, goals are specific,
concrete, subjective, and susceptible to being more directly evaluated.
Our perspective emphasizes the role of goals in determining action
(Lemos, 1993).
Generally, needs are related to self-system processes which are
reflected in individuals' beliefs. Beliefs related to the needs for
competence are control beliefs (expectations about the extent to which
one can obtain desired outcomes), and for autonomy are agency beliefs
(expectations about the extent to which one has access to the means that
produce desired outcomes).
According to organismic perspectives (Ryan, Deci, & Grolnick,
1995; Wellborn, 1992), the need for competence and autonomy is universal
and basic to development. The need for competence is evidenced in
behaviors such as exploration, curiosity, mastery, and a general attempt
at dealing with the environment in a competent way (Harter, 1981; Hatter
& Connel, 1984; White, 1959). The need for autonomy or for
self-determination has been studied in the context of the intrinsic
motivation paradigm (deCharms, 1984; Ryan, Deci, & Grolnick, 1995;
Harter, 1981) and refers to the wish of experiencing the self as
responsible, as the author of one's own actions. Self-determination
allows for the organization of personal development according to
one's own talents and capacities.
The cognitive approach to the study of psychological needs is
represented in goal theories of motivation. Goals are viewed as
resulting from the cognitive elaboration of needs (Lemos, 1993, 1996).
The conceptualization and empirical study of goals is recent and still
lacks conceptual clearness. In our study, the term "goal" is
equated to intentions, purposes, and motivational focuses.
Nuttin (1980) was the first author to emphasize the role of
cognitive motivational goals in guiding and regulating action. He
concentrated particularly on the process of goal-setting and planning to
attain goals. Within his perspective, the basic needs are general,
non-focused, and unspecific. The association of basic needs to action
requires a cognitive elaboration that transforms basic motives into more
concrete and specific goals, directed to particular objects or
situations. Goal elaboration is viewed as an active process of search
and construction. Whereas social learning and situational factors may
contribute to the elaboration of these specific goals, the subject must
creatively reorganize the information in order to adapt the goals to the
particular situation.
The process of goal setting is accompanied by the planning of
means-ends structures that lead to goal attainment. Within this process,
the subject anticipates the means and evaluates the results according to
the established criteria (that is, the goal). Besides allowing for goal
change after reflection about the means, this process encourages
exploration of alternative pathways to goal attainment. In sum, it is
suggested that motivated action may be understood in terms of the
process of goal setting and planning toward the satisfaction of the
basic psychological needs. The cognitive processing of needs also
results in the individualization of motivation which is related to the
ability of self-development and of self-regulation. In fact, the
creation of goals and plans is a personal construction, to which
subjects commit themselves by taking the initiative and the
responsibility for goal attainment. Nuttin contrasts the internal
regulation of behavior with "the motivational alienation," in
which subjects, due to pressures, are led to do or even to wish for
things inconsistent with their intentions and perceptions. The inability
to transform one's plans into action or to elaborate basic needs
into specific goals and plans contribute another form of motivational
dysfunction (Nuttin, 1980). Motivated action is related to intentional actions as opposed to automatic behavior, and is based on goal setting
and planning (reflection about goals and the adequate means for their
accomplishment) as well as adaptation to different situations. This type
of intentional action produces an active and personalized involvement,
in terms of engagement and reengagement in order to attain the intended
goals. In the absence of goals and plans, behavior is automatic, without
perceptible self-direction, thus hindering the internal regulation of
behavior. Such motivational functioning is not goal oriented and may
have negative implications for learning and development, as the subject
behaves without any commitment to a personal project. Consequently, the
internal regulation of behavior and the personification of motivation
are negatively affected, which may interfere with the satisfaction of
psychological needs by generating passivity and low goal-striving. In
sum, goal setting guides behavior by focusing attention and effort in
order to attain the specific goal, thereby diverting the subject away
from irrelevant stimuli.
Self-related Beliefs and Motivation
In general, the motivational constructs related to the need for
competence are termed perceptions of competence (or perceptions of
control) referring to judgments about individuals' access to
effective means to attain desired outcomes. Motivational constructs
related to the need for autonomy refer to perceptions (and judgments) of
self-determination. The need for autonomy has been studied in the
context of the intrinsic motivation paradigm (deCharms, 1984; Ryan,
Deci, & Grolnick, 1995) and reflects the importance of experiencing
the self as the agent of one's own actions. The feeling of owning
one's actions promotes self development according to
individuals' talents and capabilities.
With reference to development, children construct beliefs about
their competence and autonomy by interacting within their social
context. Those who come to believe they can attain desired goals
(control beliefs) and that they have personal access to the adequate
means (agency beliefs), will be more likely to face challenging
situations and engage in adequate strategies to achieve valued outcomes.
By contrast, children who come to believe they cannot obtain desired
outcomes or prevent negative consequences will tend to react in a
maladaptive manner, particularly in stressful circumstances.
In educational contexts, these motivational beliefs are of special
importance, powerfully influencing learning through promoting or
hindering students' willingness to engage in school work. According
to Connell and Wellborn's (1991) motivational model of needs, the
ways in which the basic psychological needs for competence and for
self-determination are fulfilled determines engagement in different
activities. When the needs are fulfilled, children feel more competent,
and will be more fully engaged. When one of these needs is not
fulfilled, children tend to become disaffected and unmotivated.
Engagement and disaffection refer to the intensity and quality of the
student's involvement in activities. Engagement includes behavioral
and emotional components (Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Children who
invest in tasks maintain a behavior involvement in learning activities
and show a positive emotional tone. They choose the tasks according to
their abilities, take initiative when given the opportunity, and show
effort and concentration while performing their tasks. They also show
enthusiasm, optimism, and curiosity. Disengaged children, on the
contrary, are passive, do not strive, and give up easily when confronted
by obstacles. They may feel bored, depressed, anxious, or even angry.
Engagement produces not only better learning and performance, but the
progressive capacity for self-influencing one's own development.
Research based on Skinner's model also shows that control and
agency beliefs consistently influence performance on achievement tasks
(Skinner, 1995).
Attachment and Motivational Systems
Attachment relations have been studied within Bowlby's theory
(1969, 1973, 1980). Attachment is a vital process in human ontogeny, not
only because it is related to individual survival but because it
promotes adaptive development during the entire life cycle (Ainsworth,
1989). The establishment of an attachment relation is viewed as a
social-emotional task of infancy that provides the basis for competence
and self-efficacy, and prepares the child for the resolution of
subsequent developmental tasks in the social-emotional and cognitive
domains (Cicchetti, Cummings, Greenberg, & Marvin, 1990; Matas,
Arend, & Sroufe, 1978; Suess, Grossmann, & Sroufe, 1992;
Thompson, 1999).
The way the attachment figure responds to the child's basic
needs in essential for the development of confidence and security in the
self and toward the attachment figure, which will be later generalized
to other figures and relationships (Bowlby, 1988). When the attachment
figure is available, sensitive, and accessible to the child's needs
and thus promotes feelings of security, he/she may be used as a secure
base from which the child can explore the environment.
Bowlby recognizes, however, the possibility of other patterns of
parental behavior with unfavorable or negative implications for child
development, and leading to insecure attachment as evidenced by the
avoidance of the attachment figure due to fear of rejection, or by a
continuous anxiety in facing the expectation of loss of that figure.
Research has shown that the quality of the attachment between the child
and his/her caregiver has a meaningful impact on the development of
social-emotional competence (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985;
Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 1999). Feelings of security
and control developed within the relationship between the child and
his/her attachment figure seem to contribute to emotional regulation, to
the development of other social relationships, and to the way the child
copes with subsequent development tasks (Grossmann & Grossmann,
1991; Koback, 1999; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990; Thompson,
1999). Children who use the attachment figure as a secure base, are able
to explore the environment with confidence and security, thus enhancing
their competence and autonomy (Grossmann, Grossmann, & Zimmerman,
1999; Main, 1983; Sroufe, 19998) and, most likely, allowing for positive
and efficient strategies of motivation. In challenging situations, these
children seek proximity and support from the attachment figure, which
allows them to return to the exploration of the world. Insecure children
do not seem to be able to use the attachment figure as a secure base
(Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978); insecure-avoidant
children tend to ignore the attachment figure and to concentrate on
objects; insecure-ambivalent or resistant children show less competent
exploratory behavior and avoid challenging tasks (Ainsworth, 1970;
Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main, 1983).
Maslin-Cole and Spieker (1990) analyzed the relation between
attachment and motivation, based on two longitudinal studies with
toddlers, one with a non-risk sample and the other with a social-risk
sample. In general, these studies offered empirical evidence for the
influence of attachment security on early motivation. The quality of
attachment shows some influence on the toddler's level of
motivation, in the sense that avoidance and security of attachment are
related to the highest levels of motivation (in contrast to anxious
attachment). Maslin-Cole and Spieker (1990) emphasize that there are no
clear indications that, over time, secure children have an advantage
(compared to the insecure-avoidant group) for development of a longer
attention span, are more resilient in the face of challenge, and have
greater independent motivation. However, the results do not exclude the
idea that secure children may have more pleasure and gratification in
their interaction with objects, and throughout these experiences may
develop feelings of competence and motivation.
The transition to adolescence is a critical period for cognitive
and emotional development, which might allow a deep understanding of the
relations between attachment and motivation. Within the framework of the
attachment theory, and of the motivational goal theory, our study is
aimed at exploring the relation between the quality of attachment and
the quality of the strategies of motivation in a sample of young
adolescents. More specifically, our study explores possible relations
among patterns of thought, behavior, and emotions, related to
representations of attachment and to the motivational functioning in
situations that challenge or threaten psychological needs for emotional
security, competence, and autonomy. Based on the assumption that, in the
face of stressful situations, individuals may try to restore
psychological well-being by engaging in cognitive and emotional
strategizing, or may give up trying to satisfy their needs, we designed
an empirical study which allows us to evaluate and relate the quality of
the strategies of attachment and the quality of the strategies of
motivation. This research encompasses an important methodological
dimension that is reflected in the evaluation of the strategies of
attachment and motivation.
OBJECTIVES AND METHODOLOGY
Objectives
Based on the aforementioned conceptualization, our study aimed: (a)
to assess attachment strategies in young adolescents; (b) to identify
and assess students' motivational strategies in stressful classroom
circumstances; (c) to assess motivational strategies of students with
high and low control and agency beliefs, and (d) to examine the relation
between attachment and motivational strategies.
Participants
Participants were 44 sixth-grade students, 22 boys and 22 girls
aged 11 to 14. They were selected from a larger sample of 409 students
from a school in the north of Portugal. Based on the Portuguese version
(Lemos & Goncalves, 1995) of the CAMI instrument (The Control,
Agency and Means-ends) (Skinner, Chapman, & Baltes, 1988), two
groups of students were defined: Group 1 consisting of 20 low-control
students, and Group 2 consisting of 24 high-control students.
Measures and Procedures
Assessment of attachment strategies. A Portuguese version of the
Separation Anxiety Test (SAT) was used (Almeida, Soares, & Martins,
1996), based on the original version by Hansburg (1972), the versions by
Klagsburn and Bowlby (1976), and by Resnick (1991a,b). The Portuguese
version of the SAT includes a set of twelve pictures, adapted from
Hansburg's (1972) original drawings. The pictures are presented to
all subjects in the same order, as follows:
1. A teenager living permanently with his/her grandparents and
without his/her parents.
2. The teenager is being transferred to a new school class.
3. The teenager and his/her parents are moving to a new
neighborhood.
4. The teenager is leaving his/her mother to go to school.
5. The teenager is leaving his/her parents to go to camp.
6. After an argument with the mother, the father is leaving.
7. The teenager's brother is a sailor leaving on a voyage.
8. The judge is placing the teenager in an institution.
9. The parents go away for two weeks, leaving the teenager with a
sitter.
10. The teenager's mother is being taken to the hospital in an
ambulance.
11. The teenager and the father are standing at the mother's
coffin.
12. The teenager is running away from home.
For each picture, two open-ended questions are asked: "How
does the boy/girl in this picture feel about this situation?" and
"What does the boy/girl in the picture do next?" All
interviews are audiotaped and transcribed verbatim. This method is based
on the assumption that the quality of the attachment strategies may be
inferred by analyzing how the teenager deals with these attachment
experiences and discusses them in terms of a (in)coherent organization
of his/her feelings, thoughts, and behaviors (Main et al., 1985).
The analysis and classification of the Portuguese version of the
SAT is an adaptation of the method proposed by Resnick (1991a,b) which
was developed based on the Method for Analyzing the Adult Attachment
Interview by Main & Goldwyn (1984). Based on nine rating
scales--emotional openness and vulnerability, dismissing/devaluing of
attachment, self-blame, resistance/withholding, preoccupied anger,
displacement of feelings, anxiety (optimism/pessimism), coherence of
transcript and solutions--three attachment categories may be identified:
secure/autonomous, insecure/dismissing of attachment, and
insecure/preoccupied with attachment relationships (for more details see
Resnick, 1991a,b).
Students assigned to the category of "insecure/dismissing of
attachment" generally show difficulties in identifying feelings,
particularly in the most stressful pictures. The teenager in the picture
is seen as unaffected by the separations. When talking about parents or
friends, they reveal a high level of rejection or depreciation of these
relationships. Some subjects are not as explicit and, instead, are very
resistant to discussing situations, giving very short answers deprived
of affective content. They focus on their strengths to deal with the
situation, viewing their parents and/or friends in terms of their
instrumental qualities.
Subjects classified in the "secure/autonomous" category
show emotional openness and freedom to talk about what the teenager in
the picture feels when facing adversity. They recognize the importance
of attachment relationships and see separation or loss of attachment figures as critical situations giving rise to feelings of vulnerability,
sadness, and loneliness. Although there are differences within this
group at the level of emotional expression and emotional understanding,
their interviews show coherence in terms of the ability to integrate
positive and negative aspects of the pictured situations and clear
interest in the re-establishment of the contact with the parents as a
solution to the separation.
Teenagers classified as "preoccupied with attachment
relationships" show a high level of anger or negative affect, when
discussing separation from attachment figures, and are unable to justify
them. They oscillate between positive and negative aspects involved in
the pictured situations and present contradictory solutions, either
revealing high passivity, or destructive solutions. A preliminary study
was carried out aimed at assessing the inter-judge agreement using the
SAT's scales in a sample of 27 subjects, aged 11 to 14 years. The
agreement found was 87.5% (Almeida, 1998). For the present study, the
inter-judge agreement was calculated based on a group of interviews
randomly selected-30% of the interviews from Group I and Group II; an
86% of agreement was found.
Assessment of the Motivational Strategies
Due to lack of empirical methods of evaluation of students'
motivated behavior in stressful classroom situations, a semi-structured
interview was developed, as well as the respective methods of
codification and classification. This measure was named "Interview
about motivational strategies in the classroom context for
preadolescents" (Lemos & A1meida, 1995). Motivational
strategies are defined as patterns of action composed of the interaction
of different forms of activity, including goals, behaviors, and
emotions. The term "pattern" allows for the detection of
multiple configurations of motivational strategies. The quality of the
strategy is defined on the basis of general indicators of efficient
action and with reference to the particular classroom context.
The interview includes ten vignettes describing classroom
situations which challenge or threaten the needs for competence and/or
autonomy. These situations were selected based on the motivational
literature about events that represent obstacles to the fulfillment of
feelings of competence and self-determination, such as failure; high
ambiguity; lack of resources, support, or information; and conformity
pressure (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Ryan, Deci, & Grolnick,
1995; Wellborn, 1995). The configuration of the situations was adapted
to the typical Portuguese classroom context, portraying a student (girl
or boy) facing specific stressors, as follows:
1. The teacher makes inconsistent demands on the students. One day
she says that students should study using their textbook; the next day
she says that the students should use their classroom notes to study.
This student can't figure out the right thing to do.
2. This student is surprised. He/she had a poor grade on a test,
but had expected a much better result.
3. Although this student has the correct answer, the teacher
doesn't accept it, because the student used a different approach to
complete the task.
4. This student is very bored. The lesson is not interesting.
5. This student is not able to achieve passing grades. Everyone
knows he/she is one of the lowest achievers in the class.
6. This student wishes to participate, but the teacher never picks
him/her.
7. This student is working very hard on a project. He/she is very
enthusiastic about doing a good job. However, the teacher interrupts,
and tells the students to store away their materials.
8. The teacher has already explained the topic twice, but this
student still doesn't understand.
9. The teacher is about to go on to a new topic, which is totally
unfamiliar to this student.
10. This student must carry out an assignment, but the teacher
gives no explanation on how to do it.
The students were individually interviewed concerning the emotions
of the student described in each vignette: "What does the student
feel in this situation?"; The character's behavior: "What
will he/she do next?"; His/her goals: "What for?" Without
being intrusive, the interviewer explored how the student sees, reflects
upon, and acts in the situation as well as the emotions expressed. All
the interviews were taped and fully transcribed. Content analysis was
used to identify students' motivational strategies (for a detailed
description, see Lemos, 1996, 1999). A theory-driven rating system was
defined to examine the students' responses. This system of
classification combines five dimensions to judge the motivational
quality of the students' strategies: intentionality, behavior,
goal-behavior coherence, adaptability, and emotions.
Intentionality reports on the initiation and sustainability of
engagement in activities (Ford, 1992; Nuttin, 1980; Ryan et al. 1995).
According to the system developed by Lemos (1993, 1996) this dimension
assesses whether students elaborate means-ends strategies, and
classifies the answers into two categories: goal-oriented behavior (when
the student defines a goal and a plan of action to attain it), and
non-goal oriented behavior, in its absence. The level of the
student's behavioral activity was also considered for the
evaluation of motivational quality of action when facing obstacles
(Bandura, 1977; Skinner & Wellborn, 1994; Weiner, 1986).
Accordingly, students' answers were classified into two categories:
active behavior and passive behavior. The active category represents
behavioral and/or cognitive determination, effort or concentration in
the search for a solution to the problem. The passive category
represents lack of commitment and involvement (behavioral and
cognitive). Notice that "active behavior" includes both
problem-focused and emotion-focused strategies, provided the student
clearly tries to deal with the situation. It also comprises not only
strategies of approach, but of avoidance, as long as the strategy is
justified. Goal-behavior coherence refers to the global adequacy of the
activities (plan) to reach the intended result (goal). Incoherence refers to the answers in which there is inconsistency between the goal
and the behavior. Whenever responses do not fit this criterion they are
classified as coherent. Adaptability assesses whether students'
responses consider the requirements of the particular situation.
Maladaptive strategies are assigned to responses disregarding ethical
and classroom norms. Other responses are classified as adaptive. The
students' answers are classified as "emotionally
overwhelmed" when the subject expresses strong, generalilzed, and
unrestrained negative feelings, and as "emotionally
responsive" when these criteria are not present. The classification
of the motivational strategy is assigned to the interviews as a whole,
rather than to each cartoon strip. Inter-rater agreement was reached
through a random selection of 16 interviews (four boys and four girls
from each sample group), and was 87.5%.
RESULTS
Attachment Strategies
The majority of the students (59.1%) presented a secure strategy,
and most of the insecure ones were classified as dismissing (27.3%). We
also determined which of the nine rating scales significantly
differentiated between the secure and insecure groups. Results presented
in Table 1 show that all the scales contributed toward distinguishing
the atttachment groups, with the exception of the rating scale of
preoccupied anger, which is close to statistically significance (p =
.053).
Motivational Strategies
Based on combinations of the five dimensions of
classification--intentionality, behavior, goal-behavior coherence,
adaptability, and emotions--four motivational strategies were defined.
These include flexible action, rigid action, disorganized behavior, and
passive behavior.
Generally, the flexible and rigid strategies are considered
constructive, both clearly revealing an active intentional goal-oriented
behavior. Through setting goals and planning courses of action to
restore well-being in the face of stressful situations presented in the
cartoon strips, these subjects show goal-valuing. They acknowledge the
emotional impact of the situation, try to solve the problem, and offer
solutions that are adaptive to the requirements of each situation. Their
strategies are coherent and orderly. In general, such characteristics
suggest that these students use an internal form of behavior regulation.
The differences between flexible and rigid strategies lie in the
quality of the behavior dimension. Flexible students reveal
cognitive-behavioral involvement, engaging in a process of reflection
whereby they seek information, evaluate, re-evaluate, interpret the
situation, and consider alternatives. They try to coordinate different
goals and define priorities. Whereas they may abide by external demands,
they also seem to protect their intentions and their feelings of
competence and of self-determination. Apparently, they accept rather
than are restrained by external pressure, as they identify and explain
it. In contrast, students using a rigid strategy strive for the goal in
an active but rigid way, which means that strategies are not
differentiated according to different situations. Their reaction reveals
an over-regulated, introjected involvement, showing active behavioral
engagement but insufficient cognitive elaboration. For example, the
solutions they present are mostly insensitive to the situation's
cues, and they are perseverant and self-punitive. These subjects are
restrained by the situation, with an apparent unsatisfactory fulfillment
of their needs.
Students with a passive strategy behave without a real goal;
apparently their behavior is mechanical and repetitive, without
revealing a full grasp of the situation. These students do not show
involvement or any determination to solve the situation, as if they do
not acknowledge any potential threat. Their speech is poor, short,
simplistic, and reductive of the complexity of the situation. Their
behavior is not supported by means-ends structures. Apparently the
students only elaborate non-instrumental means since they cannot explain
how these means are going to lead to goal achievement. They keep at some
activity, but no proper action is considered that is related to the
problem. They suggest irrelevant solutions such as "to wait,"
"to do his/her best." From an emotional point of view, they
may experience mild feelings, but never take the initiative to minimize
or prevent the problem.
The disorganized strategy is characterized mainly by being
incoherent and/or maladaptive. Subjects may state vague goals or jump
from one goal to another, never completing a course of action. Their
behavior is confused, impulsive, and even aggressive. There are signs of
abrupt and intense negative emotions.
In our study, a passive strategy was found in 36% of the students,
with 23% showing a disorganized strategy; 25% of the students reveal a
flexible strategy, and 15.9% a rigid strategy.
Relations between Motivational Strategies and Control Beliefs
The relationship between motivational strategies and control
beliefs is statistically meaningful (p < 0.05), as shown in Table 2.
The flexible strategy appears in a larger proportion in Group 2 (90.9%)
than in Group 1 (9.1%), and the rigid strategy appears in a larger
proportion in Group 1 (71.4%) than in Group 2 (28.6%). In short, almost
all subjects with a flexible strategy have high control beliefs, and the
majority of the subjects with a disorganized strategy show low control
beliefs.
Relations between Attachment and Motivational Strategies
The relationship between attachment and motivational strategies is
not significant (p = 0.088) as shown in Table 3. However, secure
adolescents show more constructive motivational strategies and a smaller
proportion of disorganized strategies than do insecure adolescents.
DISCUSSION
Attachment Strategies
Based on the Portuguese Version of the Separation Anxiety Test, it
was possible to identify distinct attachment strategies. These are
inferred from the way the students discuss the pictured separations from
the attachment figure, how they organize their feelings and thoughts,
and how they are engaged in the search for solutions to these critical
attachment experiences (Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Resnick, 1991a,b).
Secure and insecure strategies are inferred through the organization of
language, thought, attention, and memory when discussing relevant
attachment experiences (Main et al., 1985). When the students are
confronted with situations that challenge their attachment needs and may
give rise to anxiety and fear of separation from attachment figures, the
attachment system will be activated and expressed through language in a
narrative form. The quality of the attachment strategies may be inferred
by analyzing how the student deals with these attachment experiences and
discusses them in terms of a (in)coherent organization of his/her
feelings, thoughts, and behaviors (Main et al., 1985).
Our students reveal more secure attachment strategies than do
insecure students. These results are very similar to those found in the
preliminary study (Almeida, 1998), as well as in other Portuguese
samples (Soares, 1996). However, in contrast to the findings presented
by Resnick (1991a), our study shows a higher proportion of insecure
strategies in a sample of 31 children between the ages of 11 and 13.
All the rating scales allowed us to differentiate the secure from
the insecure groups except for the scale of "preoccupied
anger." In general, the expressions of anger are limited and
focused only on the separation situations and on the people involved;
for example, "angry at the parents leaving." Cultural and
educational factors as well as their young age, may contribute to these
"soft" emotional expressions. Resnick (1991a) also found
significant differences between secure and insecure groups, based on the
rating scales of the SAT, with exception of the scales of self-blame,
withholding/resistance, and preoccupying anger. However, it should be
noted that the Portuguese version of the SAT includes more situations of
separation and are different from those proposed by Resnick (1991b),
which may have allowed a more powerful inter-group differentiation. In
short, the way each student confronts him/herself and reflects on and
evaluates separations from attachment figures, seems to constitute an
indicator of the quality of his/her internal models about the self and
the attachment figures, and, consequently, of the quality of his/her
attachment organization. The secure youngsters seem to evaluate the
situation of separation as less threatening to their need for attachment
than do those who are insecure. At least they do not show the same
mechanisms found in the dismissing or in the preoccupied groups. They
are able to present a coherent strategy to deal with stress through a
positive relation between behavior and emotion, according to Skinner and
Wellborn's perspective (1994). They can easily recognize that the
situation challenges their need for attachment and they try to find
internal or external resources to face the situation in a positive
manner. The insecure/dismissing subjects try to divert their attention
from their feelings as a strategy, which prevents them from being
emotionally involved in a critical situation. A way to deal with the
emotional stress elicited by separations may be to deny the importance
of the attachment experiences and relationships in their lives. They
show a moderate level of coherency when discussing their feelings and
behaviors at the level of attachment, revealing few contradictions
between what they feel and do, and how they justify the situations. The
preoccupied group shows high levels of anxiety and a
"heightening" of their attachment system which contrasts with
the vagueness of their emotional expression. They are often confused by
the pictures and not sure about the parents' motives or behavior.
They show high ambivalence and vulnerability in face of separation from
attachment figures, but they are not able to present a constructive
solution for the situation.
These three attachment groups show in a clear way how affect
regulation and self-confidence in dealing with separation are related to
the quality of attachment strategies (Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Wright,
Binney, & Smith, 1995).
Motivational Strategies
The interview, designed to evaluate the quality of students'
motivational strategies in stressful classroom situations through a set
of vignettes, stimulated students' relevant statements by eliciting
experiences related to competence and self-determination. This method
allowed for the detection of four distinct strategies: flexible, rigid,
passive, and disorganized. Each of these motivational strategies has
different implications for task engagement and motivational development.
Flexible students are able to face challenges to their feelings of
competence and of autonomy and to deal with them positively. This
capacity requires positive perceptions of one's competence as well
as some degree of personal determination. These subjects reveal an
autonomous type of action (Ryan et al., 1995), by orienting their own
behavior according to personalized goals, and elaborating adequate
means-ends strategies to attain them (Ford, 1992; Nuttin, 1980; Ryan et
al., 1995). In sum, they reveal several essential characteristics for an
active and continuous involvement in learning and, consequently, good
academic performance.
Students with a rigid strategy also show intentional goal striving,
although apparently using controlled rather than autonomous forms of
action. Although these students are able to satisfy their competence
needs, they seem to devalue autonomy needs. Since both needs are
important for engagement in school achievement and for development,
these students might be at a slight disadvantage. Their strategy seems
to reflect an "introjected" regulation (Connell & Ryan,
1984; Ryan et al., 1995), or "motivational-alienation"
(Nuttin, 1980). These concepts describe a motivational functioning
characterized by tension and internal conflict, namely through fighting
for goals that are not fully self-integrated. Although they reveal
behavioral investment, these students rigidly stick to one course of
action, and their persistence is largely based on guilt-avoidance. This
type of student often behaves according to what he/she think is
expected, but seldom experiences learning as an interesting activity.
Under these circumstances, self-esteem will depend on success and, as a
consequence, instances of failure may hinder or even block investment.
Students with a passive strategy seem to successfully commit
themselves to activities which are not part of more global projects.
Such a form of dealing with stress is hardly compatible with
self-regulation and with assuming responsibility for one's
performance and development (Nuttin, 1980). Their behavior suggests that
they act like "pawns" (DeCharms, 1984) or that they have
learned not to strive for goals. Generally these students will probably
show disengagement and little interest in learning. This type of
behavior does not allow them to satisfy the needs for competence and for
self-determination. Wellborn (1995) emphasizes that these students will
tend to disinvest or to give up on the fulfillment of psychological
needs.
A disorganized strategy is dysfunctional and inadequate since the
satisfaction of psychological needs for competence and for
self-determination are seriously compromised and adaptation endangered.
Contrasting with the strong wish and active struggle to appear competent
and autonomous, students with a disorganized strategy show disoriented and inefficient action. Frequently, they use processes that are
debilitating, such as, devaluing the activities, questioning the
teacher's authority, systematically attributing their failures to
external factors, blaming others or even being aggressive. In the
interviews with these students an unrealistic perception of control not
associated with adequate means is often apparent. They become intensely
upset in stressful situations, but they cannot find a way out. Their
strong negative feelings seem to block the capacity for organizing a
strategy to solve the problem. In terms of personal development, these
youngsters may be at a great disadvantage because their strategies tend
to perpetuate school difficulties. Within this scenario, conditions are
created that are likely to lead them to lose interest or to give up
trying to feel competent and self-determined.
Concerning the implications of these strategies for learning and
development, it would be interesting to empirically evaluate, in future
investigations, the consequences of adopting a flexible, rigid,
disorganized, or passive strategy. Whereas flexible and disorganized
strategies predictably lead to school success and failure, respectively,
the same is not so clear as to rigid and passive strategies, which have
more unpredictable repercussions. In general, the typical strategies of
these two groups of students do not fill their autonomy needs
satisfactorily, and may even hamper their development. Students with
passive strategies develop few reflective skills and act without much
intention. However, they are able to keep "on task" and
probably attain some important goals, even if these may be ill-defined
and impersonal. The instability of active and personal investment in the
elaboration of goals and plans exhibited by these students, has also
been associated with deficits in responsibility for one's actions
and for internal behavioral regulation (Lemos, 1993; Nuttin, 1980). It
is also possible that for some of them, the situations were not really
stressful. In that case, their behavior might reflect automatic over
learned behavior rather than passivity. Future research to clarify this
issue should assess students' assessments of stressful
circumstances. Students with a rigid strategy may have a better chance
to achieve academically. Their capacity for goal-setting and planning
for competence will certainly allow them an oriented action toward
seeking success and avoiding failure. Problems may arise, however, in
situations which demand initiative and autonomy. Under these
circumstances, performance may be affected by emotions, such as fear,
tension, pressure, and anxiety. Patrick, Skinner, and Connell (1993)
have emphasized that autonomy may be the key to understanding students
who are behaviorally involved in the task, but become anxious or upset.
Both students with passive and rigid strategies might be risking
personal development and social/emotional adjustment, especially because
the school and teachers tend not to be aware of their difficulties.
Relations Between Motivational Strategies and Control Beliefs
As expected, there was a clear association between level of
perceived control and control and motivational strategies. Students with
a flexible strategy have high perceptions of control; that is, they are
confident they will attain the desired results and feel they have access
to the means required for goal achievement. In situations that challenge
or threaten their needs for competence and self-determination, their
high control beliefs seem to help them deal with the situation in a
positive manner. These beliefs seem to support involvement in strategies
that reveal interest, optimism, negotiation, and commitment, as well as
self-initiated and self-directed action.
In contrast, subjects with a disorganized strategy show low control
beliefs, which may, in part, explain the type of strategy they adopt. It
is suggested that low perceived control in achieving highly desired
goals may negatively affect the capacity to mobilize and organize
resources.
Most of the students with rigid strategies also showed low control
beliefs. The lack of flexibility might reflect a defensive strategy
toward threats associated with low perceived control. However, this type
of strategy seems more functional than that of disorganized students.
For rigid students, low perceived control apparently leads to the
repeated use of readily available strategies. This may partly protect
their feelings of competence by enhancing the possibility of success, or
at least the avoidance of failure.
In general, students with high control beliefs demonstrate good
strategies. However, some students with high perceived control were
classified as having a passive strategy. In these cases passivity does
not derive from low outcome expectation or low self-efficacy, but seems
more directly related to the issue of autonomy. In the face of coercive
situations, these subjects seem to give up self-determination and
initiative. Their actions seem constrained, restricted to a few fixed
pathways, suggesting low personalization (Ryan et al., 1995; Nuttin,
1980) and leading to controlled forms of behavior regulation. In short,
it is suggested that without personally valued goals, control beliefs
are not sufficient for overcoming stressful situations.
Relations Between Attachment Strategies and Motivational Strategies
Although the theoretical model of attachment does not clearly
explain the relation between attachment and motivational strategies, it
is plausible to assume that the quality of attachment may have
meaningful implications for individuals' development and for the
quality of motivational behavior (Maslin-Cole & Spieker, 1990). In
the motivational literature, these relationships have been addressed
only conceptually, suggesting an interdependence among the satisfaction
of the three basic psychological needs for competence, autonomy, and
relatedness. Our empirical examination of the relation between
attachment and motivational strategies suggests that secure students
demonstrate better motivational strategies at school than do insecure
students.
Students with a secure strategy showed a smaller proportion of
disorganized motivational strategies when compared to those who are
insecure. In contrast, students with insecure attachment strategies
exhibit less flexible motivational strategies. About two-thirds of
flexible students reveal secure attachment strategies, and about
two-thirds of the disorganized students reveal insecure attachment
strategies.
These results seem to support theoretical assumptions about
attachment. Youngsters with a secure attachment strategy can deal with
separation situations with confidence in themselves and in their
attachment figures, and are more able to cope constructively with the
problem, and to re-establish contact with these figures. According to
Maslin-Cole and Spieker (1990), these subjects learned in the context of
their relationships with attachment figures how to regulate their
emotions in difficult situations and how to feel secure enough to
explore the environment. Their involvement and the support received from
the attachment figures may contribute to the development of high levels
of perceived control and autonomy, which will allow them to deal
positively with challenge/threat situations to meet the need for
competence and self-determination. In contrast, most students with
insecure attachment strategies display disorganized motivational
strategies. In this case, a common pattern seems to underlie both
(attachment and motivation) strategies, including lack of co-ordination
and incoherence between behaviors and emotions, hindering the
satisfaction of psychological needs in the relational and motivational
domains. These youngsters have difficulty in controlling their feelings
when they face obstacles to satisfying their needs of attachment,
competence, and self-determination.
The passive motivational strategy seems to be compatible with the
secure strategies as well as with insecure strategies. Secure students
would be expected to show less passive strategies (cf. Maslin-Cole &
Spieker, 1990). However, Skinner and Wellborn (1994) indicate that the
subject may experience different levels of threat about different basic
psychological needs, in a rather independent way. This suggests the
possibility that some subjects may be able to behave adaptively at a
social-emotional level, and display less adequate strategies at the
motivational level. Moreover, the classroom atmosphere may also
contribute to the emergence of passive strategies in secure students. If
the classroom structure does not foster experiences of personal
causality (de Charms, 1984), students may feel that their activities and
even their achievements are not their own responsibility, thus prompting
them to give up goal setting and planning may result in adopting a more
passive attitude, even though, in less externally constrained
circumstances they may show more initiative. Although most insecure
students showed poor motivational strategies, some reveal constructive
strategies. This finding may be in line with the conclusions pointed out
by Maslin-Cole and Spieker (1990); some insecure subjects seem to find a
compensation mechanism due to their unsatisfied need of being attached
and close to the other, by investing in an active and determined way in
academic achievement.
In short, secure subjects, globally, show more constructive
strategies, particularly flexible strategies, in comparison to those who
are insecure. Insecure students present more disorganized strategies.
However, many secure subjects display passive strategies and some
insecure subjects display constructive strategies. Factors associated
with the classroom environment and the operation of compensatory
mechanisms may explain these findings. The educational system seems to
foster students' passivity (Lemos, 1993). Frequently, competence
and self-determination are poorly encouraged in favor of good academic
achievement. The idea of compensatory mechanisms is frequently referred
to in the literature on stress and coping. In their motivational
approach to coping, Skinner and Wellborn (1994) acknowledge that it is
possible to feel, for example, threatened in the attachment domain but
not in the competence or self-determination domains. This assumption
implies the existence of different cognitive processes associated with
each of the three needs, which will be reflected in the quality of
action taken in situations that threaten each need.
Conceptualizing human behavior in terms of psychological
necessities allowed us to form an empirical scheme for evaluating and
examining the relationships between attachment and motivational
strategies. We assumed an exploratory approach in this study,
considering that the literature about attachment and motivation did not
deeply explore the relationship between the attachment quality and the
quality of motivational functioning in youngsters.
Table 1
Differences Between Secure and Insecure Groups
on the Rating Scales for Attachment Security
Secure Insecure
Scale M SD M SD p
Emotional 7.307 0.679 5.333 0.594 .000
openness
Dismissing of 2.269 0.777 3.944 1.211 .000
attachment
Self-blame 6.423 1.331 5.000 1.909 .004
Resistance 6.692 1.319 5.666 0.686 .006
Preoccupied anger 8.576 0.808 7.611 1.719 .053
Displacement of 7.653 0.561 6.666 0.840 .000
feelings
Anxiety 5.307 1.407 3.722 1.178 .000
Coherence of 7.000 0.979 5.333 0.970 .000
transcript
Solutions 7.192 0.849 5.777 1.477 .000
Table 2
Motivational Strategies of Students with Low (Group 1)
and High (Group 2) Control Beliefs
Motivational
Strategies Group 1 Group 2 Total
Flexible 1 (5.0%) 10 (41.7%) 11
Rigid 5 (25.0%) 2 (8.3%) 7
Passive 8 (40.0%) 8 (33.3%) 16
Disorganized 6 (30.0%) 4 (16.7%) 10
Total 20 (100.0%) 24 (100.0%) 44
Note. [chi square] = 8.758, p = .032
Table 3
Relations Between Motivational and Attachment Strategies
Motivational Secure Insecure
Strategies Attachment Attachment Total
Constructive 13 (50.0%) 5 (27.8%) 18
Passive 10 (38.5%) 6 (33.3%) 16
Disorganized 3 (11.5%) 7 (38.9) 10
Total 26 (100.0%) 18 (100.0%) 44
Note. [chi square] = 4.862, p = .088
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation:
Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation.
Child Development, 41, 49-67.
Ainsworth, M. (1989). Attachment beyond infancy. American
Psychologist, 44, 709-716.
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Almeida, C. (1998). Estrategias de vinculacao e estrategias de
motivacao em pre-adolescents. [Attachment and motivational strategies in
early adolescence]. Faculty of Psychology and Education, Universidade of
Coimbra, Portugal. Unpublished masters dissertation.
Almeida, C., Soares, I., & Martins, C. (1996). Manual portugues
de cotacao e classificacao do Separation Anxiety Test--SAT [Portuguese
Manual for the Separation Anxiety Test]. Department of Psychology,
Universidade of Minho. Unpublished manuscript.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unified theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment. New York: Basic
Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation. New York: Basic
Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss, sadness and
depression. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. London: Routledge.
Cicchetti, D., Cummings, E., Greenberg, M., & Marvin, R.
(1990). An organizational perspective of attachment beyond infancy:
Implications for theory, measurement and research. In M. Greenberg, D.
Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years.
Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: The Chicago University
Press.
Connell, J. (1990). Context, self, and action: A motivational
analysis of self-system processes across the life span. In M. Beeghly
(Ed.), The self in transition: Infancy to Childhood. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Connell, J., & Ryan, R. (1984). A theory and assessment of
children's self-regulation within the academic domain. University
of Rochester, NY. Unpublished manuscript.
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. (1991). Competence, autonomy,
and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self-system processes. In M.
Gunnar & A. Sroufe (Eds.), Self processes in development: Minnesota
Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 23). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
deCharms, R. (1984). Motivation enhancement in educational
settings. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in
education (Vol. 1). Student motivation. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Ford, M. E. (1992). Motivating humans: Goals, emotions, and
personal agency beliefs. London: Sage.
Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1991). Attachment quality as
an organizer of emotional and behavioral responses in a longitudinal
perspective. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris
(Eds.),Attachment across the life-cycle. Tavistock, UK: Routledge.
Grossmann, K. E., Grossman, K., & Zimmermann, P. (1999). A
wider view of attachment and exploration: Stability and change during
the years of immaturity. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
of attachment: Theory, research and, clinical applications. New York:
Guilford.
Hansburg, H. (1972). Adolescent separation anxiety: A method for
the study of adolescent separation problems. Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
Harter, S. (1981). A model of mastery motivation in children:
Individual differences and developmental change. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),
Minnesota symposium on child psychology, Vol. 14. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Harter, S., & Connell, J. P. (1984). A model of children's
achievement and related self-perceptions of competence, control, and
motivational orientation. In J. Nicholls & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances
in motivation and achievement (Vol. 3). The development of achievement
motivation. London: JAI Press.
Klagsbrun, M., & Bowlby, J. (1976). Responses to separation
from parents: A clinical test for young children. British Journal of
Projective Psychology, 21(2), 7-27.
Kobak, R. (1999). The emotional dynamics of disruptions in
attachment relationships: Implications for theory, research, and
clinical intervention. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York:
Guilford.
Kobak, R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence:
Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and
others. Child Development, 59, 135-146.
Lemos, M. S. (1993). A motivacao no processo de
ensino/aprendizagem, em situacao de aula [Motivation in the classroom
learning situation]. Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of
Porto. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Lemos, M. S. (1996). Students' and teachers' goals in the
classroom. Learning and Instruction, 6(2), 151-171.
Lemos, M. S. (1999). Students' goals and self-regulation in
the classroom. International Journal of Educational Research, 31,
471-485.
Lemos, M. S., & Almeida, C. (1995). Procedimentos de
administracao e analise da entrevista sobre estrategias de motivacao, em
contexto de sala de aula para pre-adolescentes [Procedures for analyzing
the interview about motivational strategies in the classroom context for
pre-adolescents]. Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of
Porto. Unpublished manuscript.
Lemos, M. S., & Goncalves, T. (1995). Questionario de Crencas
de Controlo, Meios-Fins e Eu-Meios [Questionnaire about control
beliefs]. Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Porto.
Unpublished manuscript.
Main, M. (1983). Exploration, play, and cognitive functioning
related to infant-mother attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 6,
167-174.
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Adult attachment scoring and
classification system. University of California at Berkeley. Unpublished
manuscript.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in
infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move to the level of representation.
In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment
theory and research. Monographs of the society for research in child
development, Vol. 50 (Serial No. 209).
Maslin-Cole, C., & Spieker, S. (1990). Attachment as a basis
for independent motivation: A view from risk and nonrisk samples. In M.
Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the
preschool years. Theory, research and intervention. Chicago. The
University of Chicago Press.
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, A. (1978). Continuity of
adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of
attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547-556.
Nuttin, J. (1980). Motivation, planning and action. Leuven
University Press & Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Patrick, B., Skinner, E., & Connell, J. (1993). What motivates
children's behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control
and autonomy in the academic domain. Journal of Personality and social
Psychology, 65(4), 781-791.
Resnick, G. (1991a). Attachment and self-representation during
early adolescence. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the
Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.
Resnick, G. (1991b). Measuring attachment in early adolescence: A
manual for the administration, coding and interpretation of the
Separation Anxiety Test for 11-to 14-year-olds. Auburn University,
Department of Family and Child Development. Unpublished manuscript.
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E., & Grolnick, W. (1995). Autonomy,
relatedness, and the self: Their relation to development and
psychopathology. In D. Cichetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental
psychopathology, Vol. 1: Theory and methods. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Skinner, E., & Belmont, M. (1993). Motivation in the classroom:
Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the
school year. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(4), 571-581.
Skinner, E., & Wellborn, J. (1994). Coping during childhood and
adolescence: A motivational perspective. In D. Featherman, R. Lerner,
& M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior, Vol. 12.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Skinner, E., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P. (1988). Control,
means-ends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its
measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54(1), 117-133.
Soares, I. (1996). Representalcao da vinculacao na idade adulta e
na adolescencia. Estudo intergeracional: Mae-filho(a) [Attachment
representation in adulthood and adolescence: An intergenerational study
with adolescents and their mothers]. Braga: Education and Psychology
Center, Universidade of Minho.
Sroufe, A. (1998). The role of infant-caregiver attachment in
development. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical
implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sroufe, A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of
early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches
to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363-1373.
Suess, G., Grossmann, K., & Sroufe. A. (1992). Effects on
infant attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in
preschool: From dyadic to individual organization of self. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 15(1), 43-65.
Thompson, R. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In J.
Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications. New York: The Guilford Press.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and
emotion. New York: Springer.
Weinfield, N., Sroufe, A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (1999).
The nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In
J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford.
Wellborn, J. G. (1995). Engaged and disaffected action: The
conceptualization and measurement of motivation in the academic domain.
UMI Dissertation Services.
White, R. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297-333.
Wright, J., Binney, V., & Smith, P. (1995). Security of
attachment in 8- 12-year-olds: A revised version of the Separation
Anxiety Test, its psychometric properties and clinical interpretation.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(5), 757-774.
Marina Lemos, Associate Professor, Faculty of Psychology and
Education, University of Porto, Portugal.
Cristina Almeida, School Psychologist, Secondary School of Vila da
Feira, Portugal.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Isabel Soares, Associate
Professor Department of Psychology, University of Minho, Campus de
Gualtar, 4100 Braga, Portugal. E-mail:
[email protected]