Emerging adult versus adult status among college students: examination of explanatory variables.
Blinn-Pike, Lynn ; Worthy, Sheri Lokken ; Jonkman, Jeffrey N. 等
INTRODUCTION
Arnett (2000) proposed that individuals between approximately 18
and 25 years of age are in between adolescence and adulthood. He
referred to this stage as "conceptually, theoretically, and
empirically" different from the other two and labeled it
"emerging adulthood" (p. 463). Emerging adults are
characterized by less stable financial situations, interpersonal relationships, living arrangements, cognitive and emotional development,
and religious beliefs. In the study of the transition to adulthood, it
is often assumed that 18- to 25-year-olds are a homogeneous group and
are all emerging adults. Individuals in this age range who have attained
adulthood have received less attention than emerging adults. Arnett
(1997, 1998, 2001), Nelson (2003), and Nelson and Barry (2005) have
reported that some in this age group do report that they are adults but
their numbers are relatively small. According to Nelson and Barry
(2005), those in this group who perceive themselves as adults are in the
minority among their peers and represent a group that is worthy of
further study. The first purpose of this study was to report how many
undergraduate college students are classified as "emerging
adults," "undecideds" or "adults." The second
purpose was to determine how emerging adults and adults compare on
background characteristics, risk-taking behaviors, sensation-seeking
scores, and income.
LITERATURE
Classification as Adult versus Emerging Adult
Nelson and Barry (2005) systematically compared college students
who perceived themselves as adults with their peers who did not. They
attempted to identify college students who perceived themselves as
adults and to explore whether differences in adulthood criteria,
achievement of those criteria, identity issues, risk-taking behaviors,
and depression were based on perceived adult status. Nelson and Barry
(2005) asked if the students thought that they had reached adulthood.
The response choices were "yes," "no," and "in
some respects yes and in some respects no." Findings from 232
college students' responses revealed that there was a small group
of perceived adults (25%). When compared to their emerging adult peers,
those who perceived themselves to be adults (a) did not differ on the
adulthood criteria they used; (b) had a better sense of their overall
identity, as well as what type of person they wanted as a romantic
partner; (c) were less depressed; and (d) engaged in fewer risk
behaviors (e.g., illegal drug use and drunk driving). The researchers
reported that adults in this age range deserved more attention than has
been given in previous research.
Ethnicity and Emerging Adulthood
Arnett (2003) found several differences in conceptions of the
transition to adulthood among ethnic groups, African Americans, Latinos,
and Asian Americans were more likely than Whites to think that a man or
a woman should "become capable of supporting a family
financially" in order to be considered an adult. Arnett reported
that family obligations were valued more highly by American ethnic
minority groups than by Whites. He also found that ethnic minority
groups were more likely to assume collectivistic values than Whites by
emphasizing consideration for others. For example, they may be more
likely to avoid embarrassing or bringing trouble to the family or
extended family unit by driving under the influence of alcohol or drugs,
or having significant gambling debts.
Additionally, Arnett (2003) found differences between American
ethnic minority groups (African Americans, Latinos, and Asian Americans)
and Whites on values related to getting married, completing their
education, and becoming employed full time. Ethnic minorities placed
more value on achieving these "role transitions" than did
Whites. However, the percentage of respondents finding these
achievements important for adulthood was low across all groups.
According to Arnett (2003), all three ethnic groups felt that
independence was an important component of becoming a self-sufficient
person or adult. For individuals in ethnic minority groups, they may
feel they have reached adulthood earlier than their White peers, in part
due to having assumed greater responsibilities within their families of
origin.
Schwartz, Cote, and Arnett (2005) studied 332 university students
and found that the positive relationship between agency and identity was
consistent across ethnic groups (Whites, Blacks, and Hispanics). Agency
is defined as (a) having a sense of responsibility for one's life
course, (b) believing that one is in control of one's decisions and
is responsible for outcomes, and (c) being confident that one will be
able to overcome obstacles that may appear along the life course.
Flexibility, deliberate choice-making, and exploration were positively
related to agency. Avoidance of responsibility and aimlessness were
negatively related to agency. They found that emerging adults use agency
to varying degrees depending on the coherence of their identity and that
this was consistent across Whites, Blacks, and Hispanics.
Cote (2000) described two predictable ways that emerging adults
often pursue their development depending on the degree of agency or
self-direction that they possess. One pathway was labeled default
individualization because it involves decisions dictated by circumstance
and impulse, with little agentic effort. Such a course may leave the
individual unprepared to successfully navigate adult roles such as
marriage, employment, and parenthood. A second pathway was labeled
developmental individualization because it involves decisions that lead
to deliberate and continued growth. Developmental individualism may be
more available to individuals today given the decline of economic
barriers to education and career success due to gender, ethnicity, and
social class. There is greater freedom to pursue opportunities, such as
college attendance, that lead to self-improvement in a variety of
intellectual, occupational, and psychosocial areas (Schwartz, Cote,
& Arnett, 2005).
Sensation-seeking Risk-taking and Emerging Adulthood
There is some evidence that emerging adults have preferences for
higher sensation-seeking activities than do young adults (Zuckerman
(1979, p. 10) defined sensation-seeking as "the seeking of varied,
novel, complex, and intense sensations and experiences, and the
willingness to take physical, social, legal, and functional risks for
the sake of such experiences." Sensation-seeking has been shown to
be linked to the more general trait of impulsivity and has been shown to
influence a wide variety of behaviors, including cigarette smoking, drug
and alcohol use, and gambling. Zuckerman and Kuhlman (2000) examined the
relationship between sensation-seeking and gender, risky behaviors, and
gambling among 260 college students. They reported that gambling
correlated significantly with drinking and risky sexual experiences for
males, but did not correlate with any of the risk scales for females. In
addition, males scored higher on gambling risk and sensation-seeking
scales. Disinhibition is a sociopathic type of sensation-seeking that is
highly related to nonconformity and impulsivity. Disinhibition is
described as seeking sensation through social activities such as
parties, social drinking, and sex (Zuckerman, 1994). Carrol and
Zuckerman (1977) found that disinhibition was positively correlated with
both stimulant and hallucinogen drug use among 80 males in residential
drug treatment programs. Newcomb and McGee (1991) found that
sensation-seeking scores were highly correlated with legal and illegal
substance use among adolescents. They reported that for females, a
general sensation-seeking factor predicted alcohol use one year later.
The same was not true for males, among whom only disinhibition (a
sensation-seeking subscale) predicted alcohol use and only one aspect of
alcohol use: quantity.
Bradley and Wildman (2002) investigated risk and reckless behaviors
in emerging adults. In their sample of 375 emerging adults,
"risk" behaviors were found to be reliably predicted by high
sensation-seeking scores. Todesco (2005) found a significant
relationship between high disinhibition scores among emerging adults
compared to young adults. Todesco (2005) reported significant
developmental differences in risk-taking and sensation-seeking behaviors
between emerging adults (18 to 25 years old) and young adults (26 to 34
years old). Emerging adults were found to engage in more risk-taking and
sensation-seeking behaviors, as well as to view risky acts as less
harmful than do young adults, providing additional support for the
distinctiveness of emerging adulthood as a unique developmental period.
Income and Emerging Adulthood
Parents have been shown to be the most important source of
financial support for many emerging adults. Higher parental education
and income have been shown to be related to greater likelihood of
offspring pursuing higher education and making a successful transition
to adulthood (Sandefur, Eggerling-Boeck, & Park, 2005); Osgood,
Ruth, Eccles, Jacobs, & Barber, 2005; Schoeni & Ross, 2005).
Likewise, it has been reported that those growing up in low-income homes
face more difficulties in their transition to adulthood due to lack of
social and financial capitol (Berzin, DeMarco, Shaw, Unick, & Hogan,
2006; Besharov, 1999; Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). These
difficulties include higher rates of unemployment and social idleness,
earlier childbearing, lower college attendance, fewer total years of
education, more involvement in violence and criminality, and exposure to
unhealthy neighborhoods and crime. Cote (2002) reported that students
who pay for most of their own college education appear to make a more
rapid transition to adulthood.
Nobel and McNabb (1989) examined the relationship between
students' noncognitive characteristics and their performance on the
ACT assessment. Noncognitive characteristics included parental
education, family income, size of household, and race/ethnicity. Family
income and race/ethnicity were two of the variables found to be related
to ACT performance, over and above the variance explained by courses
taken and academic grades. Noble, Davenport, Schiel, and Pommerich
(1999) also examined the relationships between students'
noncognitive characteristics and their performance on the ACT assessment
and found that noncognitive characteristics impact students'
choices of high school coursework and the grades they earn in those
courses, which in turn, are strongly related to ACT scores and college
attendance.
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
As noted, the first purpose of this study was to report how many
college students in this sample of 18- to 25-year-olds, are classified
as "emerging adults," "undecideds," or
"adults." The second purpose was to determine the
relationships between their classifications as emerging adults versus
adults and (a) background characteristics (age, gender, parenthood,
race); (b) risk behaviors (alcohol consumption, binge drinking,
cigarette smoking, and gambling); (c) sensation-seeking scores
(disinhibition); and (d) income (family receipt of public assistance and
primary source of financial support for college). Three expectations
were articulated prior to the start of the study. First, fewer students
would be classified as adults versus undecideds or emerging adults.
Second, when those who were classified as adults were compared to
emerging adults, they would be more likely to be older, minority, and
parents. And third, when emerging adults were compared to adults, adults
would engage in fewer risk behaviors, have lower sensation-seeking
scores, and be more financially independent of their families.
METHOD
Sample
Survey data were collected from 450 undergraduate students enrolled
in introductory psychology classes at two state universities in a
southern state. The universities are approximately 100 miles apart. Data
collection took place during spring 2006 and the data from the two
colleges were pooled. The students received extra credit for
participating and were from a variety of majors (arts and sciences--45%,
business--18%, education--10%; applied and life sciences--10%,
engineering-6%, undecided--11%).
As noted below, only data from the 302 students who were classified
as either adult or emerging adult were used in this analysis. For
comparison purposes, the 148 students who were classified as
"undecided" about their status were excluded. The 302 students
had a mean age of 19.64 years (SD = 1.41); 78% were freshmen or
sophomores and 55% were female. In terms of race, 223 (74%) were White,
57 (19%) were African American, and 20 (7%) were "other."
Given the small numbers, the 7% of students who were "other"
were dropped from the remaining analyses when race was the variable of
interest; (54 or (18%) of the 302 reported receiving public assistance
(food stamps, Medicaid, and/ or free/reduced price lunch) while growing
up (36 or 63% of African Americans and 13 or 6% of Whites). Race and
receipt of public assistance during childhood were highly correlated
([chi square] = .0001). Two percent of the sample was married and 9%
were parents.
Measures
Adult status. The survey consisted of 149 short-answer questions
and took approximately 25 minutes to complete. Not all of the data from
the survey are included in this report. In addition to background
questions including age, race, gender, marital status, parenthood,
receipts of public assistance, and year in college, the students were
asked four questions that were scored on a five-point scale from
strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). A higher score meant the
student was classified as more of an adult and less of an emerging
adult. The questions asked whether: (a) I am ready for a serious
relationship or marriage, (b) I have things I want to do before settling
down (reversed), (c) I consider myself a self-supporting adult, and (d)
my parents and I have the same religious beliefs. These four questions
were based on an extensive review of the literature that points to
emerging adults as believing the following are markers for adulthood:
(a) marriage or settling down, (b) independent decision making, (c)
financial independence from parents, and (d) less questioning of
parental religious beliefs (Arnett, 1997, 1998, 2001, 2002).
Participants were placed in one of two groups (adult or emerging adult)
by the number of items out of the four that they answered as
"strongly agree" or "agree." The "emerging
adults" strongly agreed or agreed with none or one of the four
items. The "adults" strongly agreed or agreed with three or
four items. An additional intermediate group of 148 students answered
two of the four questions as strongly agree or agree and were labeled
"undecideds." In order to classify the groups as separate and
distinct in their responses and to maximize differences between adult
and emerging adult status, the undecided group was not included,
resulting in the sample of 302 students.
Risk behaviors. The risk-behaviors addressed in this survey were
alcohol consumption, binge drinking, cigarette smoking, and gambling.
Alcohol consumption, smoking cigarettes, and binge drinking (five or
more drinks in a row) were all assessed by asking how many times they
participated in the behavior in the previous 30 days. Gambling was
assessed with four questions (yes/no) as to whether they had visited
casinos, played cards for money, bet on sports, and/or gambled on the
Internet in the previous 12 months.
Sensation-seeking was measured with the ten items on the
disinhibition subscale of the Sensation-seeking Scale Form V (SSS-V);
(Zuckerman, 1994). The students were given ten pairs of items and asked
to select the one in each pair that best described them. One pair asked
if the student preferred "wild, uninhibited parties" versus
"quiet parties with good conversation." The SSS-V is one of
the most widely used instruments for the assessment of sensation-seeking
and was developed and revised primarily with undergraduate students at
the University of Delaware (Zuckerman, 1994). Zuckerman (1994) reported
the reliability of the disinhibition subscale (SSS-V) to range from .72
to .78. A Cronbach's alpha of .79 was computed as an index of
internal reliability for the current data. A higher summed score (range
0-10) represented a greater preference for sensation-producing
activities.
Income. The students were asked if their family received public
assistance (food stamps, Medicaid, and/or free/reduced price lunch)
while they were growing up (yes/no). Financial support for college was
measured by asking students to complete this sentence: "Most of my
money for college (i.e., books, fees, tuition) comes from--" They
were given five choices and asked to selected one: parents,
spouse/relatives, grants, student loans, or job/savings.
RESULTS
Classification
Table 1 shows how the 302 students were classified by group: adults
116 (38%) and emerging adults 186 (62%). It also shows that overall, the
students were most likely to report that they had the same religious
values as their parents (70%) and were least likely to report that they
were ready to settle down (19%).
Table 2 shows the background characteristics of the sample
classified as emerging adults or adults with respect to age, gender,
race, and parenthood using a chi-squared test of independence. For age,
the Wilcoxon signed rank test was used, because ages were not normally
distributed in the sample. Age differences among the groups were
statistically but not practically significant (p = 0.0164) at the .05
level, with the adults being slightly older. Adult status was not
significantly associated with gender (p = 0.0686), but was significantly
associated with race (p < 0.001). More African American students were
classified as adults compared to emerging adults (33% versus 12%,
respectively). There were no significant differences in the proportion
of the two groups who were parents.
Risk Behaviors and Sensation Seeking
Table 3 shows the mean frequencies of consuming alcohol, smoking
cigarettes, and binge drinking in the previous 30 days for emerging
adults and adults. These data were compared between the two groups using
the Wilcoxon signed rank test. The Wicoxon test was used instead of the
t-test because the responses were found not to be normally distributed.
The groups differed significantly with respect to all three risk-taking
behaviors: alcohol consumption (p = 0.0001), binge drinking (p <
0.0001), and cigarette consumption (p = 0.0340). Adults consumed
alcohol, binge drank, and smoked cigarettes less frequently than did
emerging adults. Adults and emerging adults also differed significantly
with regard to sensation seeking (4.07 versus 5.37, p = 0.0003), with
adults exhibiting significantly lower levels of disinhibition than
emerging adults. Alcohol use, binge drinking, cigarette use, and
sensation-seeking scores were all significantly correlated (p <
.0001).
Table 3 also shows the percentage of respondents in each group who
engaged in each type of gambling behavior (playing cards for money,
visiting casinos, and betting on sports) in the previous 12 months. None
of the students reported gambling on the Internet in the previous 12
months. Playing cards, gambling in casinos, and betting on sports were
all significantly correlated (p < .001). The proportions for each
type of gambling were compared between adults and emerging adults using
the chi-squared test. No significant differences were found between the
groups with regard to casino gambling or betting on sports. However, the
groups differed significantly with regard to playing cards for money (p
= 0.0115), with the lower percentage occurring among adults (24%) than
emerging adults (38%).
Income
Finally, in terms of income, adults were more likely than emerging
adults to report growing up in a family that received public assistance
(28% versus 11%, respectively, p < .0001). In terms of the
proportions of the five sources of financial support for college, Table
4 shows that the sample as a whole received the most support from
parents (57%) and the least support from spouse/other relatives or
jobs/savings (3% each). The chi-square for adult status by financial
support was significant (p < .0001). Adults were less likely to
report parents as their main source of financial support for college
(40%) compared to emerging adults (67%).
DISCUSSION
This study was unique for three reasons. First, because of what it
adds to our knowledge of the number and characteristics of college
students who are classified as emerging adults versus adults, as an
under-researched topic. Both the Nelson and Barry (2005) sample and the
present sample were surveyed in intact undergraduate classes such as
human development or psychology in large public universities and
represented diversity in college majors. In addition, the two studies
were similar in terms of the proportions of females and minorities.
Although both studies involved relatively small convenience samples, the
comparison between the two studies adds to the validity and
generalizability of the present findings. The most noteworthy finding
was that the proportions of college students who were classified as
having reached adulthood were similar (25% and 26%) in both studies.
(The 26% is based on the total sample size of 450 which includes 148
(strongly agreed or agreed with two of the four statements) who were not
included in the comparison between adults and emerging adults.)
Likewise, the findings in the two studies were similar to others
regarding fewer risk behaviors and lower sensation-seeking scores among
adults compared to emerging adults (Arnett, 1997; Bradley & Wildman,
2002). These similarities were found in spite of the fact that one took
place in the mid-Atlantic region and one in the southern region of the
United States.
Second, this research was unique in its cultural context. As Arnett
(2005, p.4) stated, many articles currently published in journals on
adolescence are based on American middle-class samples but draw
conclusions about adolescents in general, with no mention of cultural
context, as if adolescents anywhere are like adolescents everywhere.
However, there is a growing awareness among scholars on adolescence that
adolescent development can be understood only in the context of culture.
The present study took place in the Deep South and was noteworthy in
examining the relationship between family receipt of public assistance
and emerging adult versus adult status among college students. There was
a sufficient number of college students who grew up in poverty in this
sample to examine its relationship to classification as an adult.
The college students in this study who grew up in families
receiving public assistance may have successfully managed to bypass or
overcome the hurdles in their childhood and adolescence in order to be
enrolled as undergraduates in a four-year state university with little
financial support from their families or extended families. Pursuing
developmental individualism, as opposed to default individualism, may
have provided them with the resilience they needed to succeed as far as
they had and may have required that they make the transition to
adulthood earlier than their higher-income classmates. Their strong
familial or collectivistic values emphasizing consideration for others
may have resulted in their being (a) more likely to feel ready to settle
down in a serious relationship or marriage, (b) forced to be
financially, independent, and (c) able to avoid gambling, becoming
drunk, smoking cigarettes or binge drinking. It remains to be seen if
these disadvantaged students will remain in college or drop out due to
financial strains, particularly if their degrees require more than four
years of college. Arnett (2004) reported that emerging adults see their
education as something they expect to continue into their late twenties
or thirties. Financial and familial obligations may make spending an
extended time in college or attaining an advanced degree difficult for
students who grew up in poverty.
Third, this research was unique due to the inclusion of gambling
among other risk behaviors. A strong case can be made for including
gambling questions in future studies of transitions to adulthood,
particularly among college students. A meta-analysis was recently
conducted on the prevalence of disordered gambling among college
students and results showed that over 7% have gambling problems
(Blinn-Pike, Worthy, & Jonkman, 2007). LaBrie, Shaffer, and LaPlante
et al. (2003) surveyed 10,765 college students from 119 scientifically
selected U.S. colleges included in the 2001 Harvard Public Health
College Alcohol Study and found that (a) 52% of males and 33% of females
reported gambling at least weekly in the last school year; (b) students
over 21 years of age were more likely to gamble; (c) the availability of
gambling venues in the state influenced students' decisions to
gamble; and (d) the data supported the presence of an underlying problem
behavior syndrome among college students that suggests a tendency to
engage in several risky activities, including gambling, alcohol use, and
binge drinking.
CONCLUSION
It was expected that fewer students would be classified as adults
versus undecideds or emerging adults and this was supported. It was
hypothesized that when those who were classified as emerging adults were
compared to adults, the adults would be more likely to be: older,
minority, and parents; only minority status was supported. Finally, it
was expected that adults would engage in fewer risk behaviors, have
lower sensation-seeking scores, and be more financially independent of
their families; all three were supported.
The basis for these results appears to be related to, at least in
part, on the income status of the students' families. Additional
research needs to be conducted on the developmental paths low-income
college students follow. Do they skip being emerging adults? Do they
experience emerging adulthood differently from their higher income
peers? Do they experience emerging adulthood earlier than their higher
income peers?
REFERENCES
Arnett, J. J. (1997). Young people's conceptions of the
transition to adulthood. Youth & Society, 29, 1-23.
Arnett, J. J. (1998). Learning to stand alone: The contemporary
American transition to adulthood in cultural and historical context.
Human Development, 41, 295-315.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development
from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55,
469-480.
Arnett, J. J. (2001). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood:
Perspectives from adolecence to midlife. Journal of Adult Development,
8, 133-143.
Arnett, J. J. (2002). A congregation of one: Individualized religious beliefs among emerging adults. Journal of Adolescent Research,
17, 451-467.
Arnett, J. J. (2003). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood
among emerging adults in American ethnic groups. New Directions for
Child and Adolescent Development, 100, 63-75.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2005). The Vitality Criterion: A new standard of
publication for Journal of Adolescent Research. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 20, 3-7.
Berzin, S. C., De Marco, A. C., Shaw, T. V., Unick, G. J., &
Hogan, S. R. (2006). The effect of parental work history and public
assistance use on the transition to adulthood. Journal of Sociology and
Social Welfare, 33, 141-162.
Besharov, D. J. (1999). America's disconnected youth.
Washington, D.C.: Child Welfare League of America.
Blinn-Pike, L., Worthy, S. L., & Jonkman, J. (2007). Disordered
gambling among college students: A meta-analytic synthesis. Journal of
Gambling Studies, 23, 175-183.
Bradley, G., & Wildman, K. (2002). Psychosocial predictors of
emerging adults' risk and reckless behaviors. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 31, 253-265.
Carrol, E. N., & Zuckerman, M. (1977). Psychopathology and
sensation-seeking in "downers," "speeders," and
"trippers": A study of the relationship between personality
and drug choice. International Journal of Addictions, 12, 591-601.
Cote, J. E. (2000). Arrested development: The changing nature of
maturity and identity. New York: New York University Press.
Cote, J. E. (2002). The role of identity capital in the transition
to adulthood: The individualization thesis examined. Journal of Youth
Studies, 5, 117-134.
Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty,
welfare reform, and child development. Child Development, 65, 296-318.
LaBrie, R. A., Shaffer, H. J., LaPlante, D. A., & Wechsler, H.
(2003). Correlates of college student gambling in the United States.
Journal of American College Health, 52, 53-62.
Nelson, L. J. (2003). Rites of passage in emerging adulthood:
Perspectives of young Mormons. New Directions in Child Development, 100,
33-49.
Nelson, L. J., & Barry, C. M. (2005). Distinguishing features
of emerging adulthood: The role of self-classification as an adult.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 242-262.
Newcomb, M. D., & McGee, L. (1991). Influence of sensation
seeking on general deviance and specific problem behaviors from
adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 61, 614-628.
Noble, J. P., Davenport, M., Schiel, J., & Pommerich, M.
(1999). Relationships between the noncognitive characteristics, high
school coursework and grades, and test scores for ACT-tested students.
(ACT Research Report No. 99-4). Iowa City, IA: ACT, Inc.
Noble, J. P., & McNabb, T. (1989). Differential coursework and
grades in high school: Implications for performance on the ACT
assessment. (ACT Research Report No. 89-5). Iowa City, IA: ACT, Inc.
Osgood, W., Ruth, G., Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Barber,
B. L. (2005). Six paths to adulthood. In R. A. Setterstein, F. F.
Furstenberg, & R. G. Rumbaut (Eds.), On the frontier of adulthood
(pp. 320-355). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sandefur, G. D., Eggerling-Boeck, J., & Park, H. (2005). Off to
a good start? Postsecondary education and early adult life. In R. A.
Setterstein, F. F.
Furstenberg, & R. G. Rumbaut (Eds.), On the frontier of
adulthood (pp. 292-319). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Schoeni, R. F., & Ross, K. E. (2005). Material assistance from
families during the transition to adulthood. In R. A. Setterstein, F. F.
Furstenberg, & R. G. Rumbaut (Eds.), On the frontier of adulthood
(pp. 396-416). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Schwartz, S. J., Cote, J. E., & Arnett, J. J. (2005). Identity
and agency in emerging adulthood: Two developmental routes in the
individualization process. Youth & Society, 37, 201-229.
Todesco, P. (2005). Developmental differences in sensation-seeking,
risk-taking and perceptual appraisals. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Wayne State University.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation-seeking: Beyond the optimal level
of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of
sensation-seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and
risk-taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68,
999-1029.
This paper was presented at the Third Annual Conference on Emerging
Adulthood, Tucson, January 2007.
Lynn Blinn-Pike, Indiana University-Purdue University; Sheri Lokken
Worthy, Mississippi State University; Jeffrey N. Jonkman, Mississippi
State University; G. Rush Smith, University of Georgia.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Lynn Blinn-Pike, Professor,
Department of Sociology, 425 University Blvd., 303 Cavanaugh Hall,
IUPUI, Indianapolis, IN 46202. E-mail: pikelcaiupui.edu
Table 1. Four Classification Criteria by Groups
Criteria (%) Group
Emerging Adult Entire
Adult (n = 116) Sample
(n = 186) (n = 302)
I am ready for a serious 8.06 97.41 42.38
relationship or marriage
I have things I want to do 2.69 43.97 18.54
before I settle down
(reversed)
I am a self-supporting adult 9.68 85.34 38.74
I have the same religious 56.45 92.24 70.20
beliefs as my parents
Table 2. Characteristics of Students by Group
Group
Variable Emerging Adult Sign.
Adult (n = 116) p-value
(n = 186)
Age (M/SD) 19.45/ 19.96/ 0.0164
1.17 1.68
Gender (% Female) 51.35 62.07 0.0686
Race (% African-American) 12.00 33.64 <0.0001
Parenthood (% parents) 9.73 8.77 0.7581
Table 3. Risky Behaviors by Group
Risky Behavior Group
Emerging Adult Sign.
Adult (n = 116) (p-value)
(n = 186)
Alcohol (frequency) 9.22 5.96 0.0001
Binge Drinking 6.67 3.72 <0.0001
(frequency)
Cigarettes (frequency) 7.12 5.60 0.0340
Gambling- cards %) 38.17 24.14 0.0115
Gambling-casinos (%) 22.04 19.83 0.6468
Gambling-sports (%) 22.58 16.38 0.1917
Table 4. Primary Source of Financial Support for College by Group
Main Source of Financial Group
Support for College %)
Emerging Adult Entire
Adult (n = 116) Sample
(n = 183) (a) (N = 299)
Grants 16.67 22.12 18.73
Job/Savings 1.61 6.19 3.34
Parents 66.67 39.82 56.32
S use/Other Relatives 2.69 4.42 3.34
Student Loans 12.37 27.43 18.06
(a) missing data for three students