Acceptability of dating violence among late adolescents: the role of sports participation, competitive attitudes, and selected dynamics of relationship violence.
Merten, Michael J.
Beginning with Makepeace's (1981) pioneering work on dating
violence, research has continued to present a sobering picture of the
extent to which violence occurs in dating and courtship relationships
(Perry & Fromuth, 2005). This violence is not strictly confined to
one gender, as both men and women are victimized with women using as
much or more violence against their male partners (Marcus & Swett,
2002). For instance, O'Keefe and Treister (1998) report that 45.5%
of males and 43.2% of females have experienced at least one incident of
physical aggression from dating partners during the course of their
dating. The high prevalence rates for dating violence, first noted over
20 years ago by Roscoe (1985), indicate the degree to which violence in
dating continues to be acceptable (Pleck, 2004).
A number of factors that may contribute to the acceptance of dating
violence have been identified in previous research. These include
parental violence experienced as a child (Foshee, Ennett, Bauman,
Benefield, & Suchindran, 2005; Lichter & McCloskey, 2004), the
seriousness, importance, and length of the dating relationship (Neufeld,
McNamara, & Ertl, 1999; O'Keefe, 1997), being humiliated by a
partner (Taylor & Sorenson, 2005), and justness of retaliation to
violence initiated by a partner (Frieze, 2005). However, additional
factors that may be critical to examine based on the extensive media
attention they have generated, are those relating to sports
participation.
Several studies have focused on the relationship between
acceptability of dating violence and sports participation (e.g., Forbes,
Adams-Curtis, Pakalka, & White, 2006; Bloom & Smith, 1996;
Crosset, Ptacek, McDonald, & Benedict, 1996; Mintah, Huddleston,
& Doody, 1999). According to Forbes et al. (2006), males who were
active in high school sports were more approving of dating violence and
physical aggression. Unfortunately, these studies focused on athletic
involvement as a global, uni-dimensional concept. In actuality, athletic
involvement is a complex variable, comprising several dimensions, some
of which may be more salient to the acceptability of dating violence
than others. It may be that only some of the characteristics of athletic
involvement are associated with the acceptability of violence.
Interestingly, Rossi, Schuerman, and Budde (1999) found
characteristics of the individual (respondent-level characteristics) to
be less important than characteristics of the situation (case-level
characteristics) in decisions regarding abuse and neglect. That is,
characteristics of violent interactions themselves were found to be more
important in judgments about violence than the personal characteristics
of the respodents making the judgments. This would suggest that
characteristics of athletic involvement may be less important predictors
of the acceptability of dating violence than characteristics reflecting
the dynamics of dating violence interaction.
Because of the complexity of factors influencing individuals'
perceptions of violence, Miller and Bukva (2001) argue that respondent-
and case-level characteristics need to be examined concurrently. This
strategy is followed in the present study. In addition to the three
respondent-level characteristics of athleticism (i.e., athletic
participation, competitiveness, and need to win), three characteristics
of violent couple interactions that may relate to the acceptability of
violence are also included in the study: initiator's violence,
recipient's reaction, and the initiator-recipient gender
combination.
Athletics and Acceptability of Dating Violence
Perhaps no social issue in sports has received more media attention
in the past decade than male athletes' violence against women. A
significant number of male athletes have made more headlines off the
field than on due to their violence in intimate relationships (Crosset,
1999). Media stories about the aggressive acts of high school,
collegiate, and professional athletes continue to appear in the news
(e.g., "Cases involving athletes and sexual assault," 2003).
Surprisingly, even though discussion about athletes' involvement in
intimate relationship violence is a common topic within the media, Craig
(2000) notes that relatively few empirical studies have focused on this
issue.
Although media-based reports suggesting a connection between
athletic participation and relationship violence are dramatic, not all
athletes are violent in their intimate relationships. The assumption
that relationship violence is a result of athletic participation in
general may be a misrepresentation. Other characteristics of an athlete
may lead to an increased level of acceptability of violence,
characteristics that are not simply their level of participation in
athletics. In addition to athletic participation, it is important that
we examine potential factors such as competitiveness and need to win
attitudes, factors that may accompany athletic participation, but have
not been specifically examined by previous literature--which may account
for variance in the acceptability of violence.
Athletic participation. In the 1990s, theorists began to emphasize
the unique way that sports activity legitimizes certain forms of violent
behavior (e.g., Nack & Munson, 1995). Studies also began examining
athletic participation as a factor in the use of violence in contexts
other than sports (e.g., Nixon, 1997; Lenzi, Bianco, Milazzo, Placidi,
& Castrogiovanni, 1997). A major focus developed on the
"spillover theory of violence," a perspective which suggests
that violence used in sports "spills over" into the
interpersonal relationships of the athletes. This theory received
support in several studies (Forbes et al., 2006; Bloom & Smith,
1996; Crosset et al., 1996).
A critical component of athletes' participation in sport
activities is their belief about the acceptability of the use of
violence. Cauffman, Feldman, Arnett Jensen, and Jenson Arnett (2000)
suggest that high rates of violent behavior reflect the development over
time of attitudes regarding the acceptability of violence, fostered by
environments that support such development. Thus, exposure to violence
may lead to an increased acceptance of violent behavior, and thereby may
increase the likelihood of an individual engaging in violent or
aggressive activity. This relationship was examined by Gardner and
Janelle (2002) who found that aggressive behavior both within and
outside sports becomes more acceptable as individuals' years of
experience in sports increase. As an example, it could be expected that
aggressive behavior within and outside of sports is more acceptable
among those who compete in collegiate athletics as opposed to those who
have participated in sports at the high school level. Therefore, it is
hypothesized that an increase in the level of athletic participation
will lead to a greater acceptance of violence in intimate dating
relationships among athletes.
However, participation in athletics by itself may not be the only
influence on and beliefs about the acceptability of violence. More
important than participation per se may be certain factors associated
with athletic participation. Two of the most important of these are
competitiveness, and the need to win.
Competitiveness and need to win. Many consider competitiveness to
be the core motivating factor underlying athletic participation.
However, conceptualizing competitiveness as a single dimension may be an
oversimplification. Houston, McIntire, Kinnie, and Terry (2002)
emphasize the importance of not treating competitiveness as
uni-dimensional and urge researchers to use greater precision when
assessing and defining the construct. For instance, according to Gill
and Deeter (1988), competitiveness reflects a desire to enter an
activity and strive for success, regardless of who wins. They also
introduce a second concept which they term "win orientation":
the desire to win or avoid losing in participatory activities.
Ryckman and Hamel's (1992) concept of personal development
competitiveness is similar to Gill and Deeter's (1988) definition
of competitiveness, which reflects a desire to enter an activity and
strive for success, regardless of who wins. They define personal
development competitiveness as an attitude in which the primary focus is
not on the outcome or the desire to win but instead on the enjoyment or
mastery of the task. For both Ryckman and Hamel (1992) and Gill and
Deeter (1988) the emphasis is on the desire to do one's best and
strive for success, not on the outcome of winning or defeating another
person.
Morey and Gerber (1995) differentiate between two types of
competitiveness: goal competitiveness and interpersonal competitiveness.
They define goal competitiveness as the desire to be and do one's
best and to excel, a definition consistent with Gill and Deeter's
(1988) and Ryckman and Hamel's (1992) concepts. Morey and Gerber
(1995) define their other type of competitiveness, interpersonal, as the
desire to do better than others and to win or defeat others, similar to
Gill and Deeter's (1988) definition of win orientation.
Perhaps the earliest conceptualization of the need to win is the
construct originated by Homey (1937) known as hypercompetitiveness,
which she defined as the need to win at any cost and to avoid losing.
Hypercompetitiveness has been examined in several studies performed by
Ryckman and colleagues (Ryckman et al., 1996, 1997, 2002). In one study
(Ryckman et al., 2002) they examined the relationship of
hypercompetitiveness to various aspects of heterosexual romantic
experiences among college students. Results indicated that those who
were more hypercompetitive reported higher levels of conflict with their
partner, greater need to control, and greater infliction of physical
pain. In another study (Ryckman et al., 1996) they also found
hypercompetitiveness to be positively related to aggression. Ryckman and
colleagues' (1996, 1997, 2002) construct of hypercompetitiveness is
similar to Gill and Deeter's win orientation concept. The key
underlying theme for both is the desire or need to win and avoid losing,
even at great cost.
In the process of revising their competitiveness index, Houston,
Harris, McIntire, and Francis (2002) provided evidence for a need to win
dimension, albeit indirectly. A total of six items were dropped from
their original 20-item scale. The revised scale was positively
correlated with the competitiveness subscale of Gill and Deeter's
(1988) Sport Orientation Questionnaire (SOQ). Interestingly, each of the
six items they omitted includes a common element about winning at all
costs and being the best, items resembling those of Gill and
Deeter's (1988) win orientation sub-scale.
In summary, a number of studies suggest that competitiveness and
the need to win are two distinct dimensions. For instance, some
individuals who have a strong desire to be successful in sports
activities (competitiveness) may attach relatively little importance to
winning (need to win). They may enthusiastically involve themselves in
the activity, enjoying their competitor's skillful moves and
counter moves as much as their own regardless of the outcome of the
contest. Others may be less competitive but have a high need to win.
Their interest in the process and skills of the activity may be limited
only to those actions that will allow them to win, including unethical and prohibited behaviors. They may even engage only in activities where
they are assured they will not lose.
These studies highlight the importance of separating dimensions of
competitiveness that reflect a need to win from dimensions that reflect
striving for success or enjoyment of competition. Confounding the
definition of competitiveness (striving for success) with that of need
to win may lead to inaccurate generalizations regarding competitiveness
and interpersonal violence. In this study we hypothesize that the need
to win is a stronger influence than competitiveness on the acceptability
of violence in dating relationships.
Respondent Gender
An additional factor that may influence the acceptability of
violence in dating relationships is gender. Previous studies have shown
males to be more accepting of violence than females, both within and
outside the sports context (Forbes et al., 2006; Gardner & Janelle,
2002). The current study explores differences among males' and
females' acceptance of violence in dating relationships.
Respondent-level vs. Case-level Characteristics
Although the acceptability of violence in dating relationships may
be associated with athletic participation, competitiveness, and need to
win, these respondent characteristics may not account for as much
variance in the acceptability of violence in dating relationships as
characteristics associated with dating violence interactions themselves.
For instance, Rossi et al. (1999) found that case characteristics
accounted for significantly more variance than individual
characteristics in respondents' decisions regarding abuse and
neglect. Miller and Bukva (2001) argue that respondent and case
characteristics need to be examined concurrently because of the
complexity of factors that enter into and influence judgments about
violence. Three characteristics of violent couple interactions that may
influence acceptability of violence are examined in this study: severity
of initiator's violence, severity of recipient's reaction to
the violence, and the initator-recipient gender combination. These
case-level characteristics are embedded in written vignettes depicting
violence in dating relationships (described below).
Characteristics of Violent Couple Interactions
Initiator's act of violence and recipient's reaction.
Walking away from acts of violence including verbal or physical
altercations is perhaps the ideal response to interpersonal conflict.
However, mutual or bi-directional violence is a common occurrence among
dating partners. For instance, Gray and Foshee (1997) found that 66% of
dating partners were involved in mutual violence, which is typically
characterized by an interactional dynamic in which one of the partners
initiates aggressive behavior toward the other who reciprocates in kind.
As a result, within a dating couple, a partner can be both perpetrator and victim (Lewis & Fremouw, 2001). Gray and Foshee (1997) report
that couples experiencing mutual violence experience more severe
aggression and more injuries than those in one-sided violent
relationships.
In dating relationships, both the level of the initiators violence
as well as the level of the recipient's reaction may be heavily
dependent on how acceptable such violent actions and reactions are.
Further, the acceptability of the recipient's reaction may be an
even more important factor than the acceptability of the
initiator's act. This is due to the fact that the level of
retaliation is more likely to affect whether the conflict ceases or
escalates (Ferguson & Rule, 1988). An accepting attitude by a
recipient may actually increase and prolong the violence in the dating
relationship.
Level of violence. The level or severity of the violent act has
been found to explain a significant proportion of variation in
individuals' judgments about the acceptability of intimate
violence. Miller and Bukva (2001) found that the seriousness of the
injury sustained was an important predictor of how seriously violence
was perceived by respondents. James, West, Deters, and Armijo (2000)
found that pushing, shoving, grabbing, slapping, punching, biting, and
kicking were the most common forms of violence among dating couples.
Leonard, Quigley, and Collins (2002) found this to be true with college
students as well, reporting that the majority of violence perpetrated by
college students consisted of these behaviors. Shook, Gerrity, Jurich,
and Segrist (2000) found that college students most often experienced
pushing, slapping, or hitting with an object as the primary forms of
physical abuse in their dating relationships. Slapping and pushing
between dating partners represent a somewhat moderate level of violence
(Bethke & DeJoy, 1993). Punching and hitting with objects represent
higher levels of violence more likely to inflict injury.
Gender vignette combination. Another characteristic of violent
dating interactions that may influence the acceptability of violence is
gender combination of the individual initiating the violent act and the
individual reacting to that act. Men and women have both been found to
initiate violence in interpersonal relationships (e.g., Leonard et al.,
2002). However, there are differences in the acceptability of violence
initiated by either gender (Forbes, Jobe, White, Bloesch, &
Adams-Curtis, 2005). Bookwala, Frieze, Smith, and Ryan (1992) note that
males' initiation of violence against their female partners is
generally viewed as less acceptable than females' initiation of
violence against their male partners. O'Keefe (1997) also found
that both sexes were more accepting of females' use of dating
violence. In addition, violence inflicted by males on their female
partners tends to be viewed as more serious by both men and women.
The purpose of this study is to examine both respondent- and
case-level characteristics as potential predictors of the acceptability
of dating violence. The complexity of factors regarding the
acceptability of dating violence requires a multi-level design to
examine respondent characteristics as well as case characteristics.
First, four respondent-level dimensions are included in the current
study: athletic participation, competitiveness, need to win, and gender.
It was anticipated that among these four factors there would be
different levels of concern with the seriousness of violent acts in a
dating relationship. Second, other research suggests that case-level
characteristics reflecting the dynamics of the violent interaction
itself may be even more influential than respondent-level
characteristics. Three important case-level characteristics that may be
salient in judgments about acceptability of violence are: severity of
initiator's violence, severity of the recipient's reaction,
and the genders of initiators and recipients. The working hypotheses in
the present study are the following:
1. Individuals with a higher need to win attitude will rate acts of
violence in dating relationships to be more acceptable than those with a
lower need to win attitude.
2. Males will rate acts of violence in dating relationships to be
more acceptable than females.
3. As the level of violence of the initiator's act increases,
acceptability of the recipient's reaction will increase.
4. As the level of violence of the recipient's reaction
increases, the acceptability of the recipient's response will
decrease.
5. A female recipient's response to a male's initial act
of violence will be more acceptable than a male recipient's
response to a female's initial act of violence.
METHOD
Sample and Procedure
Participants in this study consisted of 266 male and 393 female
students at a large public university in the Midwestern United States.
Participants' ages ranged from 17-22. Students enrolled in any one
of four social science classes at the university completed a 51-item
questionnaire regarding attitudes about competitiveness and desire to
win. Acceptability of violence was assessed through responses portrayed in nine vignettes of relationship violence. Questions were also included
about participation in high school and collegiate athletics along with
others asking for demographic information. A modified consent form was
solicited and questionnaires were administered during class.
Measures
Acceptability of violence. Acceptability of violence in dating
relationships was assessed using a set of 18 vignettes that depicted situations involving violence between dating partners (see Appendix).
Many studies examining attitudes and perceptions regarding dating
violence have used such written vignettes (Bethke & DeJoy, 1993;
Carlson, 1996; Harmon, Hall, Nash, Formati, & Hopson, 2000; Miller
& Bukva, 2001). Vignette methodology allows for systematic and
controlled manipulation of any number of independent variables. In
addition, vignettes also eliminate concern for extraneous factors that
may make findings less credible and increase the internal validity of
the research design (Campbell, 1957).
The vignettes used in the current study varied along three factors:
severity of the initiator's act, severity of the recipient's
response, and initiator-recipient gender combination. Severity of
initiator's act consisted of three categories: low (yelling at
partner), moderate (pushing/ shoving partner), and high (punching the
arm]hitting partner in the back). Severity of recipient's response
was categorized as: moderate (pushing/shoving partner); high (punching
the arm/hitting partner in the back); and very high (kicking partner in
stomach/punching partner in the face). Within each dating scenario, the
recipient's response to the initiator's violence was at the
same level or higher than the level of the initiator's action. The
severity range for a recipient's reaction was higher than the
severity range for initiator's act due to the current study's
focus on the acceptability of increased violence in a recipient's
reaction to initial violence. We propose that individuals will respond
to an initial act of violence with an equal or higher degree of
violence. As a result of this hypothesis, the severity range for a
recipient's reaction is greater. The two initiator-recipient gender
combinations were male as initiator and female as recipient of violence
and female as initiator and male as recipient. All wording in the
vignettes other than changes in the levels of the three factors was
identical to ensure that only the changes in factor levels would account
for differences in acceptability ratings of the different vignettes.
The combination of levels of the three factors produced a 3 x 3 x 2
full factorial design. To reduce redundancy and possible response
fatigue, a modified one-half fractional factorial design was used to
create four different sets of 9 vignettes. A one-half fractional
factorial design allows for administration of fewer vignettes to each
person, but still allows us to show all main and two-way interaction
effects for all variables. Each set contained all factor level
combinations for initiator and respondent violence, and either five
M-to-F/four F-to-M gender combinations, or four M-to-F/five F-to-M
gender combinations. Each of the eighteen vignettes was systematically
pre-assigned to be present in two of the four sets of nine vignettes
that were distributed to the participants. The order in which these nine
vignettes appeared in each questionnaire was determined by a random
selection process. Each respondent received one of the four sets of
vignettes that were randomly distributed. Respondents were asked to read
each vignette and indicate how acceptable they believed a particular
individual's response was to the violence depicted. Responses
ranged from 1 (totally unacceptable) to 6 (totally acceptable).
Cronbach's alpha for the vignettes was .80.
Athletic participation. Respondents' participation in high
school and collegiate athletics was assessed using a series of seven
questions. These focused on the number of years of participation in high
school athletics, number of high school sports, and the favorite high
school sport. In addition, individuals were asked whether they were
presently participating in intramural or Division I athletics. The
athletic participation index developed for this study consisted of a
scale ranging from 0 = "No participation" to 6 =
"Participation in a Division I sport" (see Table 2 for full
list of categories).
Competitiveness and need to win. The Sport Orientation
Questionnaire (SOQ) developed by Gill and Deeter (1988) was used to
measure both competitiveness and need to win in participatory
activities. Competitiveness and win orientation are two major sub-scales
of the SOQ. Each five-category item in the index ranges from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Scores for each subscale are derived by
summing across their respective items. The 13-item competitiveness
subscale measures a desire to enter and strive for success in a
participatory activity. This scale includes such items as: "I
thrive on competition" and "I look forward to competing."
The win orientation subscale consists of 6 items measuring desire to
win. Examples of items for this sub-scale include: "I hate to
lose" and "The only time I am satisfied is when I win."
Cronbach's alpha for the 25-item SOQ administered was .95; alpha
coefficients for the competitiveness and need to win sub scales were .94
and .86, respectively. These reliability coefficients compare favorably to reliabilities for the SOQ in previous studies (Gill & Deeter,
1988; Wartenberg & McCutcheon, 1998).
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics
The sample was predominantly White (91%); other ethnicities
included African American (5%), Asian American (2%), and Hispanic (2%).
Class standing consisted of Freshmen (19%), Sophomores (21%), Juniors
(21%), and Seniors (39%). In addition, approximately one-third of both
males and females report their high school graduating class as having
100 students or less. In regard to current relationship status among
males, 42% indicated they were not currently in a dating or marital
relationship, 18% were in a current relationship of between one month
and one year in length, 17% were in a current relationship of 1-2 years
duration, 20% were in a current relationship of 2-5 years duration, and
3% of the sample were in a current relationship of over 5 years
duration. For females, 36% indicated they were not currently in a dating
or marital relationship, 25% were in a current relationship between one
month and one year in length, 20% were in a current relationship of 1-2
years duration, 15% were in a current relationship of 2-5 years
duration, and 4% of the sample were in a current relationship of over 5
years duration (see Table 1).
Table 2 displays athletic characteristics of the participants in
the current study. A high number of respondents reported involvement in
high school and college sports activities. Only 8% of males and 15% of
females state that they have never participated in sports. The majority
of the sample reported continued involvement, with 46% of the males and
25% of the females involved in college intramural sports, and 14% of the
males and 9% of the females being current participants in Division I
college athletics. For those who reported any sports participation,
team-only was the preferred sport for 61% of the males and 51% of the
females. The number of sports and the type of sport (e.g., team,
individual, football, soccer) were not significantly correlated with the
acceptability of dating violence among men and women in this study.
Men's and women's mean scores for competitiveness, need
to win, and acceptability of violence are shown in Table 3. Mean
competitiveness scores for men (M = 3.96, SD = .79) are higher than
those for women (M = 3.44, SD = .83) (t = 8.03, p < .001). Mean need
to win scores for men (3.58, SD = .82) are also higher than mean scores
for women (M = 3.25, SD = .84) (t = 4.98,p < .001). Scores were
averaged across the 18 dating vignettes to produce acceptability of
violence means. Table 3 shows that males (M = 2.24, SD = .87) are more
accepting of violence than are females (M = 2.01, SD = .81) (t = 3.50, p
< .001).
Multi-level random intercept regression analyses were run to
examine the independent effects of respondent and case characteristics
on the acceptability of violence in dating relationships (Table 4). Due
to the complex nature of the research design, which includes a
multilevel design (vignettes nested within individuals), this study uses
the SAC PROC MIXED procedure for multi-level modeling. This approach
allows us to examine the unique influence of both case-level variables
and respondent-level variables. The between and within individual
variances of acceptability of dating violence were 0.56 and 1.55,
respectively. We then estimated several nested models to test
hypotheses.
Model 1 in Table 4 examines the four respondent-level
characteristics of athletic participation, competitiveness, need to win,
and respondent gender. No relationship was found between acceptability
of violence and two of the three athletic variables, athletic
participation and competition. However, the third athletic variable,
need to win, has a significant relationship acceptance of violence in
dating relationships ([beta] = .24, SE = .06, p < .001). Respondent
gender also had a significant relationship with the acceptability of
violence, with males being more accepting of violence than women. Using
Raudenbush and Bryk's (2002) method to compute explained variance for respondent-level variables, these four respondent characteristics
account for 8% of the variance in acceptability of violence scores.
Model 2 adds the first case-level variable, initiator act, to the
equation. This variable is a significant predictor of the acceptability
of violence in dating relationships ([beta] = .35, SE = .02, p <
.001), with violent responses by the recipient to more aggressive acts
initiated by the dating partner being viewed as more acceptable than
violent responses to less aggressive initiator acts. The association
between the case-level initiator act and acceptability of violence is
independent of the association between the respondent-level factors and
acceptability of violence previously tested, indicated by the stability
of the respondent-level betas and [R.sup.2] when the case-level variable
is added to the model. This single case-level initiator act accounts for
an additional 5% of the variance in acceptability of violence. The
percentage of explained variance accounted for by case-level
characteristics was computed using a method derived by Snijders and
Bosker (1999).
In Model 3 the second case-level variable is added to the model,
recipient's reaction to initiator's act of violence.
Recipient's reaction has a significant negative association with
the acceptability of violence in dating relationships ([beta] = -.55, SE
= .02, p < .001). The direction of the relationship indicates that
the recipient's response becomes less acceptable the more severe
the recipient's reaction. Recipient reaction accounts for an
additional 15% of the total variance of acceptability of violence
scores.
Model 4 adds the third case-level variable to the model,
initiator-recipient gender combination. Results indicate that,
controlling for level of response, a female recipient's violence
toward a male initiator is more acceptable than a male recipient's
violence toward a female initiator ([beta] = .73, SE = .03, p <
.001). Initiator-recipient gender combination accounts for a further 10%
of the variance in acceptability of violence.
These three case-level variables, reflecting important aspects of
violent couple interactions, are independent of the respondent-level
variables. This is indicated by the stability of the respondent-level
betas and [R.sup.2] values as the case-level variables are successively
added to the general model. Together they explain 30% of the variance in
the acceptability of violence, which is three times that accounted for
by the respondent-level athleticism variables.
As a final step, all interactions among case and respondent
characteristics were tested. However, only the three interactions
reaching statistical significance are included in the model (Model 5,
Table 4). The three interaction effects involve the initiator-recipient
gender combination, two with case-level and one with a respondent-level
variable. First, the direction of the initiator act by
initiator-recipient gender combination interaction coefficient ([beta] =
.18, SE = .03, p < .001) indicates that the relationship between
initiator act and acceptability of the recipient's reaction is
stronger for the M-to-F than for the F-to-M gender combination. That is,
the severity of the initiator's violence has a stronger
relationship with the acceptability of the recipient's reaction to
the violence for male initiators against female recipients than for
female initiators against male recipients.
Next, the coefficient for the recipient reaction by
initiator-recipient gender combination interaction ([beta] = -.09, SE =
.03, p < .01) indicates that the relationship between the
recipient's reaction and the acceptability of violence is stronger
for the F-to-M combination than for the M-to-F combination. The severity
of male reactions to female-initiated violence is more influential in
judgments about the acceptability of violence than the severity of
female reactions to male-initiated violence.
For the interaction involving the respondent-level variable, need
to win by initiator-recipient gender combination, the coefficient
([beta] = .15, SE = .03, p < .001) indicates that need to win's
relationship with the acceptability of violence is stronger for the
M-to-F combination than for F-to-M. In other words, participants with a
high need to win attitude are more supportive of female reactions to
male-initiated violence than are those with a low need to win attitude.
The decreasing Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), a goodness of fit measure, illustrates the increasing fit to the data across incremental
models throughout Table 4. In addition, the respondent-level variable,
need to win, remains statistically significant throughout all models
indicating that this variable has a significant relationship with the
acceptability of violence independently of case-level characteristics.
DISCUSSION
A popular debate has arisen concerning the role of organized sport
participation in the development and promotion of prosocial attitudes in
young people (Gough, 1998). Participation in sports has been linked
positively to self-esteem (Kavussanu & Harnisch, 2000) and
achievement attitudes (Curry, Snyder, Cook, Ruby, & Rehm, 1997).
However, competitive sports may also be linked to detrimental aspects of
development in young people. The current study adds an important
dimension to the understanding of athletes and violence in interpersonal
relationships. First, as hypothesized, only need to win is related to
acceptability of violence, not athletic involvement or competitiveness.
This finding supports the notion that competitiveness and need to win
should not be considered a single construct, but two distinct
constructs. It is individuals' need to win attitude, not their
competitiveness or mere participation in athletic activities that leads
to a more accepting attitude toward interpersonal violence. These
findings suggest that the relationship between sports and dating
violence promoted by the media and others may be an overgeneralization,
one that unjustly lumps all athletes together.
This popular stereotype linking athletes and dating violence may be
harmful in two ways. First, it does not distinguish those athletes who
are purely competitive from those who must win the contest. These
athletes who enjoy and strive to do their best in their sport activity
while being equally appreciative of the best efforts and skills of their
opponents, are not distinguished from those athletes who may disregard
rules, engage in unethical behavior, and even seek to harm their
opponents in their efforts to win.
Second, the need to win may be an attribute evident in a variety of
contexts involving gradation and selection, and not limited to the
traditional sport environment. For example, the need to win may be a
strong motivator for some in the field of music and art, in education
and academic settings, in personal dress and acquisition, and especially
in relationships requiring decision making, problem solving, and
conflict resolution. The relationship between athleticism and the
acceptability of violence in interpersonal relationships may be spurious to some degree; rather, it may be the need to win that motivates an
individual to be accepting of violence in both sport and non-sport
environments. Future studies focusing on interpersonal violence and
athletes should examine this dimension further. In particular, studies
should attempt to uncover how and when this need to win attitude is
instilled in many athletes and why other individuals may not require
this attitude at all or to a lesser degree.
However, it is important to note that the respondent-level need to
win variable explains only a smaller portion of the acceptability of
violence among respondents of this study. Of even more significance are
the case-level characteristics of severity of initiator violence,
severity of recipient's response, and the initiator-recipient
gender combination. Of these three factors, recipient reaction appears
to have the most influence, indicating that when assessing acceptability
of dating violence, how victims of violence reacts to their
partner's initial act may be more important than the initial act of
violence. Only slightly less influential were initiator-recipient gender
combinations, accounting for 10% of the variation in the acceptability
of violence. The initiator act accounted for the least amount of
variation in the acceptability of violence (5%) among the three case
characteristics examined in this study. The finding that both male and
female participants are more accepting of dating violence perpetrated by
females than by males is supported by previous research (O'Keefe,
1997). Further, results of this study show males to be even more
accepting of interpersonal violence than females in situations where a
female reacts to a male's initial act of violence. Overall, the
gender structure of the initiator and the recipient of violence appear
to have a major impact on the acceptability of violence, both directly
and indirectly as it interacts with the other major variables in this
study.
A limitation of this study relates to the homogeneous sample
population, which consisted predominantly of single, white, adolescents.
Future studies that include non-student samples and other ethnic
populations are needed to determine the generalizability of the findings
of this study to broader populations. Future research should also
examine the perceptions of same-sex violence, as we have come to realize
that violence is an issue of concern for all types of couples (Beyers,
Leonard, Mays, & Rosen, 2000).
This study provided important new information about the
acceptability of violence in dating relationships among late
adolescents. The unique relationships of both respondent and case-level
characteristics with the acceptability of dating violence call for
additional studies to implement research designs that account for the
multi-level factors that influence perceptions of interpersonal
violence. Multi-level research designs helps us better identify and
understand the various factors that help us to advance our knowledge of
interpersonal violence. These designs allow us to assess simultaneously
the relative importance that multiple factors from various domains
(i.e., family, individual, or case-level variables) may have on
individuals' perceptions of violence.
APPENDIX
Acceptability of Dating Violence Vignettes
1. Adam pushed Sara away when she yelled at
him about his new job hours.
How acceptable is Adam's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
2. Sara pushed Adam away when he yelled at
her about her new job hours.
How acceptable is Sara's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
3. Jason hit Amy in the back when she
yelled at him about the way he was acting at the
party.
How acceptable is Jason's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
4. Amy hit Jason in the back when he
yelled at her about the way she
was acting at the party.
How acceptable is Amy's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
5. Josh punched Heather in the face while
she was yelling at him about his recent behavior.
How acceptable is Josh's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
6. Heather punched Josh in the face while
he was yelling at her about
her recent behavior.
How acceptable is Heather's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
7. Matt shoved Lisa after she pushed him
away while the two were
arguing about the cost of the trip.
How acceptable is Matt's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
8. Lisa shoved Matt after he pushed her away
while the two were arguing about the cost of
the trip.
How acceptable is Lisa's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
9. James got up and punched Melissa in
the arm after she pushed him into the chair
following his comments about one of her friends.
How acceptable is James's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
10. Melissa got up and punched James in the arm
after he pushed her into the chair following her
comments about one of his friends.
How acceptable is Melissa's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
11. John kicked Rebecca in the stomach
after she shoved him away during a dispute
over where they were going to spend Thanksgiving.
How acceptable is John's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
12. Rebecca kicked John in the stomach after
she shoved him away during a dispute over where
they were going to spend Thanksgiving.
How acceptable is Rebecea's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
13. Kevin pushed Julie away after she
walked over and punched him in
the arm during a recent disagreement.
How acceptable is Kevin's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
14. Julie pushed Kevin away after he
walked over and punched her in
the arm during a recent disagreement.
How acceptable is Julie's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
15. Brian turned around and punched Jennifer
in the arm after she hit him in the back when
she realized the keys were locked in the car.
How acceptable is Brian's response?
1 2 3 4 5 6
Totally Totally
Unacceptable Acceptable
REFERENCES
Bethke, T. M., & DeJoy, D. M. (1993). An experimental study of
factors influencing the acceptability of dating violence. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 8(1), 36-51.
Beyers, J. M., Leonard, J. M., Mays, V. K., & Rosen, L. A.
(2000). Gender differences in the perception of courtship abuse. Journal
of Interpersonal Violence, 15(5), 451-466.
Bloom, G. A., & Smith, M. D. (1996). Hockey violence: A test of
cultural spillover theory. Sociology of Sport Journal, 13, 65-77.
Bookwala, J., Frieze, I. H., Smith, C., & Ryan, K. (1992).
Predictors of dating violence: A multivariate analysis. Violence and
Victims, 7(4), 297-311.
Campbell, D. T. (1957). Factors relevant to the validity of
experiments in social settings. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 297-312.
Carlson, B. E. (1996). Dating violence: Student beliefs about
consequences. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 3-18.
"Cases involving athletes and sexual assault." (2003,
December 22). USA Today, p. A.08
Cauffman, E., Feldman, S. S., Arnett Jensen, L., & Jensen
Arnett, J. (2000). The (un)acceptability of violence against peers and
dates. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15(6), 652-673.
Craig, K. M. (2000). Defeated athletes, abusive mates: Examining
perceptions of professional athletes who batter. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 15(11), 1224-1232.
Crosset, T. W. (1999). Male athletes' violence against women:
A critical assessment of the athletic affiliation, violence against
women debate. Quest, 51, 244-257.
Crosset, T. W., Ptacek, J., McDonald, M. A., & Benedict, J. R.
(1996). Male student-athletes and violence against women: A survey of
campus judicial affairs offices. Violence Against Women, 2(2), 163-179.
Curry, L. A., Snyder, C. R., Cook, D. L., Ruby, B. C., & Rehm,
M. (1997). The role of hope in academic and sport performance. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1257-1267.
Ferguson, T. J., & Rule, B. G. (1988). Children's
evaluations of retaliatory aggression. Child Development, 59(4),
961-968.
Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., Pakalka, A. H., & White, K.
B. (2006). Dating aggression, sexual coercion, and aggression-supporting
attitudes among college men as a function of participation in aggressive
high school sports. Violence Against Women, 12(5), 441-455.
Forbes, G. B., Jobe, R. L., White, K. B., Bloesch, E., &
Adams-Curtis, L. E. (2005). Perceptions of dating violence following a
sexual or nonsexual betrayal of trust: Effects of gender, sexism,
acceptance of rape myths, and vengeance motivation. Sex Roles, 52(3/4),
165-173.
Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T., Bauman, K. E., Benefield, T., &
Suchindran, C. (2005). The association between family violence and
adolescent dating violence onset: Does it vary by race, socioeconomic
status, and family structure? Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 317-344.
Frieze, I. H. (2005). Female violence against intimate partners: An
introduction. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29(3), 229-240.
Gardner, R. E., & Janelle, C. M. (2002). Legitimacy judgments
of perceived aggression and assertion by contact and noncontact sport
participants. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 33, 290-306.
Gill, D. L., & Deeter, T. E. (1988). Development of the sport
orientation questionnaire. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
59(3), 191-202.
Gough, R. W. (1998). A practical strategy for emphasizing character
development in sport and physical education. The Journal of Physical
Education, Recreation & Dance, 69(2), 18-22.
Gray, H. M., & Foshee, V. (1997). Adolescent dating violence:
Differences between one-sided and mutually violent profiles. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 12(1), 126-141.
Hannon, R., Hall, D. S., Nash, H., Formati, J., & Hopson, T.
(2000). Judgements regarding sexual aggression as a function of sex of
aggressor and victim. Sex Roles, 43, 311-322.
Horney, K. (1937). The neurotic personality of our time. New York:
Norton.
Houston, J. M., Harris, P., McIntire, S., & Francis, D. (2002).
Revising the competitiveness index using factor analysis. Psychological
Reports, 90, 31-34.
Houston, J. M., McIntire, S. A., Kinnie, J., & Terry, C.
(2002). A factorial analysis of scales measuring competitiveness.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 284-298.
James, W. H., West, C., Deters, K. E., & Armijo, E. (2000).
Youth dating violence. Adolescence, 35(139), 455-4565.
Kavussanu, M., & Harnisch, D. L. (2000). Self-esteem in
children: Do goal orientations matter? British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 70(2), 229-242.
Lenzi, A., Bianco, I., Milazzo, V., Placidi, G., &
Castrogiovanni, P. (1997). Comparisons of aggressive behavior between
men and women in sport. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 139-145.
Leonard, K. E., Quigley, B. M., & Collins, R. L. (2002).
Physical aggression in the lives of young adults: Prevalence, location,
and severity among college and community samples. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 17(5), 533-550.
Lewis, S. F., & Fremouw, W. (2001). Dating violence: A critical
review of the literature Clinical Psychological Review, 21(1), 105-127.
Lichter, E. L., & McCloskey, L. A. (2004). The effects of
childhood exposure to marital violence on adolescent gender-role beliefs
and dating violence. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28 344-357.
Makepeace, J. M. (1981). Courtship violence among college students.
Family Relations, 30, 97-102.
Marcus, R. R., & Swett, B. (2002). Violence and intimacy in
close relationships. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17(5), 570-586.
Miller, J., & Bukva, K. (2001). Intimate violence perceptions:
Young adults' judgments of abuse escalating from verbal arguments.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16(2), 133-150.
Mintah, J. K., Huddleston, S., & Doody, S. G. (1999).
Justifications of aggressive behavior in contact and semicontact sports.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 597-605.
Morey, N., & Gerber, G. L. (1995). Two types of
competitiveness: Their impact on the perceived interpersonal
attractiveness of women and men. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
25(3), 210-222.
Nack, W., & Munson, L. (1995). Sports' dirty secret.
(Special report). Sports Illustrated, 83(5), 62-73.
Neufeld, J., McNamara, J. R., & Ertl, M. (1999). Incidence and
prevalence of dating partner abuse and its relationship to dating
practices. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14(2), 125-137.
Nixon, H. L. (1997). Gender, sport, and aggressive behavior outside
sport. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 21(4), 379-391.
O'Keefe, M. (1997). Predictors of dating violence among high
school students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12(4), 546-568.
O'Keefe, M., & Treister, L. (1998). Victims of dating
violence among high school students. Violence Against Women, 4(2),
195-223.
Perry, A. R., & Fromuth, M. E. (2005). Courtship violence using
couple data: Characteristics and perceptions. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 20(9), 1078-1095.
Pleck, E. H. (2004). Domestic tyranny: The making of American
social policy against family violence from colonial times to the
present. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, S. A. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.
Roscoe, B. (1985). Courtship violence: Acceptable forms and
situations. College Student Journal, 19, 389-393.
Rossi, P. H., Schuerman, J., & Budde, S. (1999). Understanding
decisions about child maltreatment. Evaluation Review, 23(6), 579-598.
Ryckman, R. M., & Hamel, J. (1992). Female adolescents'
motives related to involvement in organized team sports. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 147-160.
Ryckman, R. M., Hammer, M., Kaczor, L. M., & Gold, J. A.
(1996). Construction of a personal development competitive attitude
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(2), 374-385.
Ryckman, R. M., Libby, C. R., van den Borne, B., Gold, J. A., &
Lindner, M. A. (1997). Values of hypercompetitive and personal
development of competitive individuals. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 69(2), 271-283.
Ryckman, R. M., Thornton, B., Gold, J. A., & Burckle, M. A.
(2002). Romantic relationships of hypercompetitive individuals. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21(5), 517-530.
Shook, N. J., Gerrity, D. A., Jurich, J., & Segrist, A. E.
(2000). Courtship violence among college students: A comparison of
verbally and physically abusive couples. Journal of Family Violence,
15(1), 1-22.
Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An
introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
Taylor, C. A., & Sorenson, S. B. (2005). Community-based norms
about intimate partner violence: Putting attributions of fault and
responsibility into context. Sex Roles, 53(7/8), 573-589.
Wartenberg, L., & McCutcheon, L. (1998). Further reliability
and validity of data on the sport orientation questionnaire. Journal of
Sport Behavior,. 21(2), 219-221.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Michael J. Merten,
Department of Human Development and Family Science, Oklahoma State
University, 1111 Main Hall, Tulsa, Oklahoma 74106. E-mail:
[email protected]
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the participants included
in the study, by gender
Males Females
Age
17-18 14% 15%
19-20 43% 45%
21-22 38% 37%
Older than 22 5% 3%
Year in School
Freshman 17% 25%
Sophomore 22% 21
Junior 19% 22%
Senior 42% 32%
Ethnicity
White-Caucasian 90% 92%
African-American 5% 4%
Asian-American 4% 2%
Hispanic 1% 2%
High School Graduating Class
50 or less 9% 16%
51-100 25% 19%
101-250 28% 24%
251-350 18% 17%
351 or more 20% 24%
Current Relationship Status
Not currently dating anyone 42% 36%
Less than 1 year 18% 25%
1-2 years 17% 20%
2-5 years 20% 15%
More than 5 years 3% 4%
Note: N=266 for males; N=393 for females.
Table 2. High school and collegiate athletic participation
characteristics, by gender
Males Females
Highest level of participation (a)
No participation 8% 15%
Freshman (HS) 3% 5%
Sophomore (HS) 3% 7%
Junior (HS) 4% 8%
Senior (HS) 22% 31%
College Intramurals 46% 25%
College Division I 14% 9%
Number of high school sports
None 11% 19%
One 20% 21%
Two 22% 21%
Three 23% 20%
Four 17% 12%
Five or more 7% 7%
Favorite type of participation (b,c)
Individual/team sport 39% 49%
Team-only sport 61% 51%
(n = 236) (n = 319)
Favorite individual/team sport (d)
Cross-country 14% 14%
Golf 18% 11%
Gymnastics 2% 4%
Swimming/Diving 9% 16%
Tennis 8% 13%
Track & Field 17% 23%
Wrestling 26% --
Other 6% 19%
(n = 94) (n = 155)
Table 3. Means for competitiveness, need to win, and acceptability
of violence, by gender
Males Females t
Competitiveness
Mean 3.96 3.44 8.03 ***
Standard Deviation. 0.79 0.83
(n = 266) (n = 393)
Need to win
Mean 3.58 3.25 4.98 ***
Standard Deviation 0.82 0.84
(n = 266) (n = 393)
Acceptability of Violence
Mean 2.24 2.01 3.50 ***
Standard Deviation 0.87 0.81
(n = 266) (n = 393)
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 4. Unstandardized multi-level random intercept regression
coefficients for respondent and case characteristics on the
acceptability of dating violence (standard errors in parentheses)
Predictor M1 M2 M3
Respondent Characteristics
Athletic Participation -.01 -.01 -.01
(.02) (.02) (.02)
Competitiveness -.06 -.06 -.06
(.06) (.06) (.06)
Need to Win .24 *** .24 *** .24 ***
(.03) (.03) (.03)
Gender (0=male) -.20 ** -.20 ** -.20 **
(.03) (.04) (.04)
Case Characteristics
Initiator Act .35 *** .35 ***
(.02) (.02)
Recipient Reaction -.55 ***
(.02)
Gender Combination (0=F-M)
Interactions
Initiator Act*Gender
Combination
Recipient Reaction*Gender
Combination
Need to Win*Gender
Combination
Respondent-level variance
(null model=.56) .50 .51 .53
Explained variance, [R.sup.2] .09 .09 .09
Case-level variance
(null model=1.55) 1.54 1.46 1.23
Explained variance, [R.sup.2] .00 .05 .20
AIC 20,366 20,059 19,169
Predictor M4 M5
Respondent Characteristics
Athletic Participation -.02 -.02
(.02) (.02)
Competitiveness -.06 -.06
(.06) (.06)
Need to Win .24 *** .21 ***
(.03) (.02)
Gender (0=male) -.20 ** -.13 *
(.04) (.02)
Case Characteristics
Initiator Act .35 *** .35 ***
(.02) (.02)
Recipient Reaction -.55 *** -.55 ***
(.02) (.02)
Gender Combination (0=F-M) .73 ** .73 **
Interactions
Initiator Act*Gender .18 ***
Combination (.03)
Recipient Reaction*Gender -.09 **
Combination (.03)
Need to Win*Gender .15 ***
Combination (.03)
Respondent-level variance
(null model=.56) .54 .54
Explained variance, [R.sup.2] .10 .10
Case-level variance
(null model=1.55) 1.08 1.07
Explained variance, [R.sup.2] .3 .31
AIC 18,503 18,435
* p<.05 ** p<.01 *** p<.001