New expressions of racism among young people in Spain: an adaptation of the Meertens and Pettigrew (1992) prejudice scale.
Rodriguez, Francisco J. ; Herrero, Juan ; Ovejero, Anastasio 等
Prejudice is a complex phenomenon and a relevant issue in
today's society insofar as it is related to social exclusion and
discrimination (Pettigrew, 2008). This social character of prejudice has
been highlighted by many authors, who suggest that direct and continuing
contact with different ethnic groups can give rise to a perception of
being threatened, both real--in terms of the distribution of resources
and welfare--and symbolic--in terms of values and traditions (Stephan
& Stephan, 2000). Moreover, the influence of the different types of
threats on prejudice varies as a function of the exogroup and the
historical and social context. In this sense, many observers have
reiterated how certain public expressions of discrimination and
prejudice toward other ethnic groups have been the object of public
sanctions (even at the legal level) in Western societies (Pettigrew,
1998). Certain types of behavior and opinions about the exogroup which
indicate blatant forms of prejudice and discrimination in terms of race,
sex, age or sexual condition are increasingly being considered as
"politically incorrect." In these societies, the foreseeable
disappearance from the public sphere of "traditional"
prejudice has given rise to an important theoretical view. In this
sense, McConahay (1983, 1986) has argued that a new form of prejudice
exists: modern prejudice. According to this author, modern prejudice
will be sustained by beliefs that have nothing to do with social
matters, that is, the new racism arises from the ambivalence between the
values of one ethnic group (equality and liberty) and the negative
feelings aroused by members of another group. Meertens and Pettigrew
(1992) and Pettigrew and Meertens (1995) provide a European perspective
on this issue, where immigrants are faced with differences in their
country of destination (language, religion, and culture) and have their
status as citizens questioned, often as a function of the socioeconomic
necessities of the receiving country.
Racist behavior appears when social norms are weak or ambiguous, so
that prejudice is attributed to factors other than the ethnic group. For
this reason, it is not surprising that prejudice is constantly evolving
toward new forms which are more difficult to recognize and combat. A
distinction is thus made between blatant and subtle prejudice. Blatant
prejudice has two forms: a perception that the exogroup is threatening,
with the result that this group is rejected; and opposition to close or
intimate contact with members of the exogroup. Subtle prejudice, on the
other hand, refers to and is identified with traditional values, the
exaggeration of cultural differences, and the negation of positive
emotions (Meertens & Pettigrew, 1997). Prejudice can therefore be
thought of as a complex phenomenon which can be seen as a mode of
resolving problems and tensions which have come about in different
contexts of interracial contact: in the experience lived by members of
the racist group who find in the group that is the object of their
discrimination an escape valve for their social and psychological
difficulties (Wievorka, 1992). Prejudice is also difficult to evaluate
because in modern democratic societies there have been systematic
campaigns against prejudice, racism, and xenophobia which have led
people to seek to appear to be tolerant without abandoning their
prejudiced attitudes (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977; Meertens &
Pettigrew, 1997; Saucier, Miller, & Doucet, 2005). Prejudice has
traditionally been evaluated through questionnaires which attempt to
measure the degree of agreement with certain statements related to
racial issues (explicit measures of prejudice). In recent times it has
been observed that this methodology does not capture social realities
because people yearn for social desirability and hence offer politically
correct responses.
Faced with this problem, in the final decades of the 20th century,
less reactive questionnaires were designed. Of particular note is the
work of Meertens and Pettigrew (1992) whose aim was to make better
predictions of behavior in situations where social desirability is
important; that is, when people have the opportunity (sufficient time)
motivation--both external (they do not want to appear prejudiced) and
internal (existence of values contrary to prejudice)--to evaluate the
consequences of explicit attitudes, they are no longer the best
predictors of behavior toward members of the other group. In other
words, the automatic activation of negative attitudes toward different
groups may be modified by the social context (Kawakami & Dovidio,
2001).
Interest in the implicit aspects of prejudice thus comes from the
prevailing norm in our society that open expression of our prejudices
should be avoided. This interest has in turn been reinforced by
methodological advances which have made it possible to measure the
implicit attitudes not captured by traditional methods (Meertens &
Pettigrew, 1997). While it is possible therefore to continue using
traditional self-report questionnaires, new items capable of identifying
subtle prejudice should be included. In Europe, the increasing immigrant
population has brought ethnic question to the forefront, with racist and
xenophobic attitudes being detected in growing segments of the
population. There is therefore a more pressing need to improve the
detection of racist and xenophobic attitudes and trends in order to be
able to predict and correct them. As Michel Wieviorka (1992) noted some
years ago, in our view correctly, our duty at present is not so much to
explain, directly or indirectly, the wave of racism which threatens
Europe. What is important is to construct the instruments necessary for
understanding these types of phenomena. This is the objective of the
present study--to analyze and adapt a scale of subtle and blatant
prejudice toward the exogroup based on adaption of Rueda and Navas
(1996) using a new, broad sample of adolescents in the Autonomous
Community of the Principality of Asturias (Spain). This region has a
small inflow of immigrants (overwhelmingly Latin American) and a small
settlement of gypsies.
METHOD
Participants
The participants in the study are 1,782 high school adolescents in
the region of Asturias, Spain of both sexes (48.7% male and 51.3%
female), aged between 12 and 18 (mean = 15.16; SD = 1.39), and the
majority of whom have the perception of coming from a medium-level
socioeconomic background (89.8% compared to 7.6% who feel they come from
a high-level background, and 2.6% from a low level). With regard to
level of education, 68 (3.8%) were in the first year of Obligatory
Secondary Education (E.S.O.), 237 in the second year (13.3%), 296 in the
third year (16.6%), 859 in the fourth year (48.2%), and 322 in the last
two years of high school (18.1%). This range permits the detection of
social and racial prejudice during one of the most challenging periods
of development from a social and educational point of view.
Variables
Blatant and subtle prejudice (Items 1-20). The Meertens and
Pettigrew (1992) scale of blatant and subtle prejudice, translated into
Spanish by Rueda, Navas, and Gomez-Berrocal (1995), and adapted to
juvenile realities in the context of the research was used. This scale
consists of 20 items, 10 of which evaluate subtle prejudice (1, 3, 5, 6,
11, 12, 14, 16, 18, 19) and the other 10 evaluate blatant prejudice (2,
4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 13, 15, 17, 20), following the response format used by
Meertens and Pettigrew (5-point Likert scale where 1 = complete
disagreement and 5 = complete agreement).
Feeling toward the exogroup (Item 21). Respondents have to indicate
their attitude toward the group on a scale of 10 to 100, where 10
indicates a "highly unfavorable attitude" and 100 a
"highly favorable attitude." The use of a single item to
obtain an index of the attitude toward an exogroup has been used in the
research on attitudes toward African Americans (Stangor, Sullivan, &
Ford, 1991), as well as in the measurement of attitudes toward gypsies
in Spain (Gomez-Berrocal & Navas, 2000).
Political ideology. This is an evaluation of the political views of
adolescents according to where they place themselves on a 10-point
scale, ranging from the extreme right (1) to the extreme left (10).
Socio-demographic characteristics. Information was obtained on age,
gender (1 = male; 2 = female), socioeconomic level (1 = high, 2 =
medium; 3 = low), and the residence (1 = rural; 2 = urban).
Procedure
The questionnaire was distributed as part of the campaign of
sensitization against racism and xenophobia of the Movement for Peace,
Disarmament, and Freedom-Asturias (MPDL-A) during the academic years
2004-05 and 2005-06 in several high schools in the region of Asturias.
Participants were from 20 secondary-level schools in Asturias, 14 in
urban areas, and 6 in rural areas. Participants were selected using
stratified sampling in two stages, with the schools being selected in
the first stage and the students in the second. The schools selected are
representative of the region of Asturias, with greater representation of
urban-based schools.
Following a series of contacts with principals, a common agenda for
applying the instruments was established. During these contacts, the
researchers explained the objectives of the study and emphasis was
placed on the voluntary nature of participation. A total of 1,782
adolescents took part in the study.
Analysis
The analysis was carried out in two stages. In the first stage, the
factor structure ofthe blatant and subtle prejudice scales was analyzed
and checked whether the structure observed for the adult population was
maintained in the adolescent population. In this stage the psychometric properties of the scales were explored. In the second stage, the main
covariates of subtle and blatant prejudice were analyzed. In this stage
multilevel regression techniques were used due to the hierarchical
structure of the data (students nested in schools). This technique
permits standard errors to be more reliably estimated because the
clustering of the data in level 1 (schools) is taken into account
(Raudenbusch & Byrk, 2002).
RESULTS
Factor Structure
We first tried to replicate the factor structure of the blatant and
subtle prejudice scales. Following Meertens and Pettigrew (1997), both
scales contain various sub-scales in the adult population. Thus, the
blatant prejudice scale comprises a sub-scale of Rejection and Threat (6
items) and another of Intimacy (4 items). The subtle prejudice scale, on
the other hand, is comprised of a sub-scale of Traditional Prejudice (4
items), another on Cultural Prejudice (4 items), and a sub-scale of
Affective Prejudice (2 items). To check the consistency of this factor
structure in the data, a Principal Component Analysis with orthogonal
rotation (Varimax) was carried out separately for the 10 items of
blatant prejudice and the 10 items of subtle prejudice, with the
criterion of extraction being the number of original factors postulated
by the authors. The results are presented in Table 1.
Focusing initially on blatant prejudice, it can be observed that
items 8, 17, and 15 have very similar loadings for both factors, leading
to interpretation problems. The internal consistency (Cronbach alpha)
for the original scales in the sample is adequate (0.78). Regarding the
subtle prejudice scale, it was found that items 05 and 03 were not
clearly assigned to the original factors, which again caused problems of
interpretation. Moreover, internal consistency is low for the
Traditional and Affective factors, and moderate for the Cultural factor.
These preliminary results raise doubts as to whether the structure of
the sub-scales observed for the adult population is maintained in the
adolescent population.
While these factors were extracted to reflect the structure
proposed by Meertens and Pettigrew (1997), once it was confirmed that
this structure did not adequately mirror the sample data, a principal
component analysis with Promax rotation for each scale with extraction
for values greater than or equal to 1 was carried out. Following this,
items with a low communality (less than 0.30) were eliminated. This led
to the elimination of items 19, 18, 06, and 09 from the scales, which
provided a satisfactory factor solution, with loadings greater than 0.40
and cross-loadings less than 0.30 in other factors. The results are
presented in Table 2.
As can be seen in Table 2, the factor structure is now reduced to
one factor per scale, with no differentiation between the original
subscales. Moreover, the loadings of the items in the factor are
satisfactory, which suggests that for the sample analyzed, the single
factors of blatant and subtle prejudice adequately describe the
structure of the data.
Multi Level Analysis
The next step was to analyze the covariates of blatant and subtle
prejudice with statistical techniques which permit the hierarchical
structure of the data to be taken into account. We estimated two
multilevel regression models with blatant and subtle prejudice as
dependent variables and gender, age, socioeconomic status, affection
toward the exogroup, political ideology, and belonging to a rural or
urban area as covariates. The results for each of these models are
presented in Table 3.
Regarding blatant prejudice, it can be seen that the fixed effects
analyzed are significant, except for socioeconomic status. Adolescents
who are male, who lean ideologicaily toward the right, and who live in a
rural environment tend to be more in agreement with expressions which
reflect blatant prejudice. Younger adolescents and those who have more
negative feelings toward the exogroup also show higher levels of blatant
prejudice.
With regard to subtle prejudice, we observed that the set of
explanatory variables present a different pattern. Age, socioeconomic
status, and living in an urban or rural environment are not found to
have a statistically significant relationship with subtle prejudice. As
with blatant prejudice, male adolescents, those with a right-leaning
ideology, and those who have less positive feelings toward the exogroup
are most in agreement with statements which reflect subtle prejudice.
Finally, after accounting for the clustering of the participants in
schools, both regression models show that the means of blatant and
subtle prejudice vary significantly. This can be interpreted as
indicating that there is room for new variables which could explain this
variation in racism across schools.
DISCUSSION
This research focused on the analysis of two scales of blatant and
subtle prejudice in a sample of 1,782 adolescents in the region of
Asturias, Spain. The main objective was to analyze whether the
psychometric characteristics and factor structure of the original scale
obtained in other studies with adult populations can be generalized to
the school-based adolescent population. Another aspect of the study was
the analysis of different covariates of blatant and subtle prejudice.
Regarding the first objective, it was observed that the data do not
allow us to confirm that the structure proposed for the adult population
is comparable to that of the school-based adolescent population. In
contrast to the two general scales of blatant and subtle prejudice and
their corresponding sub-scales which have been studied in the
literature, we observed a uni-dimensional structure for each scale.
These results, obtained through a process of depuration of the scales
and progressive elimination of items with low commonality, suggest that
the items corresponding to blatant prejudice and those corresponding to
subtle prejudice evaluate a single dimension--prejudice--subtle or
blatant. With regard to blatant prejudice, no differences were found
between the items referred to as blatant hostility (threat and
rejection) and those which are related to intimacy; nor are there
differences between items which refer in a "subtle" way to
both traditional and cultural prejudices or to affective prejudice. In
fact, these items appear not to belong to the sub-scale of subtle
prejudice itself, at least with respect to the school-based adolescents
analyzed in this study.
Regarding the second objective, namely the analysis of the
covariates of blatant and subtle prejudice, different patterns were
observed between the scales as well as coincidences. Among the
coincidences, we note that both types of prejudice are more
characteristic of males, those who have a right-leaning ideology and
those who have negative affection toward the exogroup. The latter two
variables (ideology and affection toward the exogroup) have been
consistently highlighted in the literature as important explanatory
variables of prejudice, both subtle and blatant.
The key difference found is that blatant prejudice seems to be more
prevalent among younger adolescents and those who live in rural
environments, whereas these relationships are not observed for subtle
prejudice. If, in accordance with the conceptualization of both types of
prejudice, blatant prejudice is a form of prejudice toward the exogroup
that societies appear to censure and is thus less likely to be
manifested in public (Meertens & Pettigrew, 1992), the results
indicate that this type of prejudice would be more typical of rural
environments and of younger adolescents. This sociological distribution
was not observed for subtle prejudice, with the levels being similar in
rural and urban environments and among adolescents of different ages.
Although results in this study suggest that the two scale scores of both
blatant and subtle prejudice might be useful in differentiating racist
and xenophobic attitudes among school-based adolescents in Spain, we
should be cautious about the generalizability of results. However, some
limitations of this study preclude going beyond what our data tell us.
First, participants in the study do not represent the general population
of school-based adolescents in Spain since our sample is not
probabilistic and therefore not representative of the population.
Second, it is unclear if the exogroups used (gypsies and immigrants) to
which the items of the self-report questionnaire refer in this study
would have influenced the adolescents' responses. In this sense, in
other cultural contexts with ethnic minority groups that are neither
immigrants nor gypsies (African American and native American in the USA
or Indian natives in some parts of South America, to mention a few)
results might be different.
In conclusion, although public expressions of racism are less
frequent in Western societies--that is, in societies which are
destinations for persons of different ethnic groups--it has yet to be
confirmed whether this old style of racism has evolved into new forms.
There is a general tendency in these receiving societies to make an
effort to erradicate racist attitudes and behaviors, which has resulted
in a drastic reduction in the percentage of people who, in surveys, say
they are in agreement with racist views. However, more than just a
reduction in prejudice, what seems to be happening is a profound
transformation which is giving rise to new forms of racism and prejudice
(Pettigrew, 1987, 2008) while at the same time racism and prejudice are
becoming more subtle (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977; Meertens &
Pettigrew, 1997; Pettigrew & Meertens, 1995). The reliable
measurement of this new form of racism is, in our opinion, necessary for
identifying racist attitudes in the social context. Once this is
achieved, we might be able to design and implement intervention programs
which promote multiculturalism and which are based on reliable data on
the prevalence of racist attitudes in society.
REFERENCES
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1977). The subtlety of white
racism, arousal and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 20, 218-222.
Gomez-Berrocal, C., & Navas, M. (2000). Predictores del
prejuicio manifesto y sutil hacia los gitanos. Revista de Psicologia
Social, 15(1), 3-30.
Kawakami, K., & Dovidio, J. F. (2001). The reliability of
implicit stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2(2)
212-225.
McConahay, J. B. (1983). Modern racism and modern discrimination.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 551-558.
McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern
racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice,
discrimination and racism. New York: Academic Press.
Meertens, R. W., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1992). Le racism voile:
Dimensions et mesure. In M. Wieviorka (Ed.), Racism et modernite (pp.
109-126). Paris: La Decouverte.
Meertens, R. W., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Is subtle prejudice
really prejudice? Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 54-71.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1987). New patterns of racism: The different
worlds of 1984 and 1964. Rutgers Law Review, 1, 673-706.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Reactions towards the new minorities in
Western Europe. Annual Review of Sociology, 24, 77-103.
Pettigrew, T. F. (2008). The social science study of American race
relations in the twentieth century. Social and Personality Psychology
Compass, 2, 318-345.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Meertens, R. W. (1995). Subtle and blatant
prejudice in Western Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25,
57-75.
Raudenbusch, S., & Byrk, A. (2002). Hierarchical linear and
nonlinear models: Applications and data analysis methods. London: Sage.
Rueda, J. F., & Navas, M. (1996). Hacia una evaluacion de las
nuevas formas del prejuicio racial: Las actitudes sutiles del racismo.
Revista de Psicologia Social, 11, 131-149.
Rueda, J. F., Navas, M. S., & Gomez-Berrocal, C. (1995). Las
nuevas expresiones del racismo: Adaptacion de una escala de prejuicio
sutil. In J. C.
Sanchez & A. M. Ullan (Eds.), Procesos psicosociales basicos y
grupales (pp. 357-370). Salamanca: Eudema.
Saucier, D. A., Miller, C. T., & Doucet, N. (2005). Differences
in helping whites and blacks: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 9(1), 2-16.
Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective
and cognitive determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9(4),
359-380.
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2000). An integrated threat
theory of prejudice. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and
discrimination (pp. 23-45). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wieviorka, M. (1992). El espacio del racismo. Barcelona: Paidaos.
Juan Herrero, Department of Psychology. University of Oviedo,
Oviedo, Spain
Anastasio Ovejero, Department of Psychology, University of
Valladolid, Palencia Spain
Andrea Torres, Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo,
Oviedo Spain
Requests for reprints should be sent to Franscisco J. Rodriguez,
Departamento de Psicologia, Facultad de Psicologia, despacho 215, Plaza
Feijoo, s/n, 33003-Oviedo, Spain, E-mail:
[email protected]
Table 1. Original (1) and obtained factor structure of the blatant and
subtle prejudice scales (N = 1,782).
Blatant Prejudice
Threat and
Rejection Intimacy
Item 10 .67# .42
Item 09 .69# .18
Item 08 .44# .49
Item 02 .75# .06
Item 17 .39# .43
Item 15 .54# .58
Item 20 .27 .69#
Item 07 .05 .78#
Item 04 .31 .64#
Item 13 .17 .79#
[alpha] .78 .78
Subtle Prejudice
Traditional Cultural Affective
Item 05 .30# .49 .39
Item 01 .76# -.02 .09
Item 14 .76# .16 .01
Item 03 .44# .48 .33
Item 11 .18 .73# .20
Item 06 .24 .61# .30
Item 12 .05 .72# .06
Item 16 -.09 .69# .04
Item 19 .07 .17 .82#
Item 18 .05 .13 .81#
.61 .70 .62
(1) Bolded loadings correspond to the original items for each
subscale.
Note: Bolded loadings correspond to the original items for each
subscale is indicated with #.
Table 2. Principal component analyses with Promax rotation for blatant
and subtle
prejudice scales (1) (N = 1,782)
Blatant Prejudice Subtle Prejudice
Item 10 .56 Item 05 .50
Item 08 .60 Item 01 .72
Item 17 .61 Item 03 .69
Item 15 .65 Item 11 .71
Item 20 .81 Item 12 .60
Item 07 .65
Item 04 .87
Item 13 .73
alpha .85 .70
(1) Loadings lower than .40 not shown
Table 3. Results of multilevel regression analyses of covariates of
blatant and subtle prejudice (N = 1,782)
Blatant Prejudice
B S.E. P
Fixed Effects
Intercept 33.64 2.06 <.001
Male (a) 1.09 0.29 <.001
Age -0.30 0.12 .011
Socioeconomic status -0.67 0.44 .131
Affection towards Exogroup -0.21 .01 <.001
Ideology 0.24 0.07 <.001
Rural (a) 2.01 0.49 <.001
Random Effects
Schools 31.00 1.08 <.001
Subtle Prejudice
B S.E. p
Fixed Effects
Intercept 20.99 1.51 <.001
Male (a) 0.42 0.18 .020
Age -0.09 0.08 .244
Socioeconomic status -0.26 0.28 .368
Affection towards Exogroup -0.08 0.01 <.001
Ideology 0.14 0.04 .001
Rural (a) 0.80 0.73 .299
Random Effects
Schools 12.69 0.45 <.001
(a) Category references are: female and urban