Internet use and academic achievement: gender differences in early adolescence.
Chen, Su-Yen ; Fu, Yang-Chih
As a new medium of learning in the twenty-first century, the
Internet has brought unprecedented opportunities to students. To
capitalize upon such opportunities, schools and families eagerly
facilitate Internet use, particularly in East Asia, where academic
achievement remains the top priority at school. At the same time,
however, the Internet has also become a major concern for parents,
because some online activities may seriously distract adolescents from
their homework. For parents and educators alike, therefore, it is
important to determine whether and how Internet use is linked to
academic achievement, a key outcome of school learning.
Previous studies have been inconclusive about the relation between
Internet use and academic achievement. Among high school students, for
example, the amount of time using the Internet has little to do with
individuals' academic achievement. Furthermore, students'
grade point averages (GPA) are not closely correlated with specific
activities, such as searching for information, E-mailing, and playing
games (Hunley et al., 2005). Among college students, however, searching
information online about course materials helps boost intellectual
development and facilitates preparation for future jobs. In contrast,
heavily indulging in online recreation has been closely linked to
impaired academic performance (Kubey, Lavin, & Barrows, 2001; Kuh
& Hu, 2001).
Internet use varies greatly by what students do online and how they
do it. Like many other domains in adolescente, the content and other
patterns of Internet use also differ widely between boys and girls. Does
such a gender gap account for the lack of consistent findings about how
these activities are linked with academic achievement? For example, does
any one specific online activity help boost academic achievement for
boys and girls alike? Or is it possible that boys benefit from one
activity while girls gain from another?
Following consistent findings about the gender gap in Internet use,
this paper examines whether and how male and female adolescents differ
in the ways various aspects of Internet use affect academic achievement.
These aspects include the overall frequency of using the Internet,
activities students engage in online (such as information seeking,
chatting and socializing with friends, and playing games), the location
where they use the Internet, and whether parents regulate such use. Data
were drawn from the Taiwan Youth Project, a panel survey series that has
followed 2,690 youths from grade 7 (age 13) since 2001.
BACKGROUND
Internet Use and Academic Achievement
Some studies have suggested a positive association between college
students' Internet use and their learning. In Suhail and
Bargee's (2006) survey study with 200 university students from
Pakistan, around three quarters of respondents noted positive effects of
Internet use on their learning in at least three aspects. First,
Internet use improved their grades. Second, the Internet expanded their
reading, writing, and information-processing skills. Third, the Internet
has proved a helpful tool in their studies. In another study, Kuh and Hu
(2001) used data (collected with the College Student Experiences
Questionnaire) from 71 four-year colleges and universities in the United
States (N = 18,344) and found that surfing the Internet for course
material had positive net effects on intellectual development and
vocational preparation, in addition to personal development.
Other studies have found a negative link between college
students' Internet use and academic performance. For example,
non-heavy Internet users had higher academic grades than heavy Internet
users as a group (Chen & Peng, 2008). In another study, at a large
public university in the United States (N = 572) significantly more
students believed that their academic performance had been impaired when
they were involved in heavy recreational Internet use, defined as usage
of synchronous, computer-mediated communication (CMC), such as
multi-user domains (MUDs) and Internet Relay Chat (IRC) (Kubey, Lavin,
& Barrows, 2001).
Although Internet use has been equally popular among high school
students, relatively fewer studies have explored how Internet use is
linked to academic achievement among these adolescents. One rare study
(Hunley et al., 2005) recruited 10th-grade students from science and
social studies classes at three public high schools in Ohio and asked
them to keep a log of their computer use for one full week. Using GPA as
the indicator, the study found no significant relation between academic
achievement and the amount of time spent on the Internet. Nor did such
achievement have any noticeable association with such online activities
as searching for information, playing games, or Emailing.
In view of the lack of consistent evidence supporting an
association between Internet use and academic achievement, it appears
that the Internet may not play a major role in adolescents'
learning. As suggested earlier, however, previous studies may have
overlooked gender differences in Internet use among adolescents. Taking
such a key factor into account, the current study aims to compare and
contrast what aspects of Internet use affect the school learning among
boys and girls, respectively.
Gender Differences in Internet Use
Studies have suggested that even though the gender gap in computer
use is closing among adolescents, boys and girls still differ greatly in
what they do online (Clemente, 1998; Imhof, Vollmeyer, & Beierlein,
2007; Odell, Korgen, Schumacher, & Delucchi, 2000). Whereas more
female adolescents use the Internet to search for information (Chen
& Peng, 2008; Lin & Yu, 2008; Odell et al., 2000) and for E-mail
(Chen & Peng, 2008; Lin & Yu, 2008; Odell et al., 2000; Sherman
et al., 2000), more male adolescents use the Internet to play games
(Chen & Peng, 2008; Griffiths, Davies, & Chappell, 2004; Lin
& Yu, 2008; Odell et al., 2000; Sherman et al., 2000).
Such gender differences prevail from elementary school through
college in some societies. Ina study of 5th and 6th graders in Taiwan,
for example, Lin and Yu (2008) found that boys tended to spend a little
more time than girls in terms of weekly use of the Internet. They also
differed significantly in their top three online activities: the
percentages of time girls spent searching for homework information and
using e-mail were higher than those of boys; in" contrast, boys
played games more often than did girls.
The patterns remain about the same among college students, at least
in the United States and Taiwan. For example, Odell et al. (2000)
surveyed American college students from five states and found that more
female than male students used the Internet for E-mailing and research,
while more male students played online games. Sherman et al. (2000) also
investigated the Internet gender gap among American college students by
comparing the usage patterns of three student cohorts in 1997, 1998, and
1999. Male college students participated more in WWW surfing,
newsgroups, MUDs (multi-user, real-time virtual world online gaming),
and chat groups, while female students reported significantly higher
E-mail use. Based on a large national survey in Taiwan, Chen and Peng
(2008) also found that whereas males spent more time playing online
games than did females, females spent more time searching for academic
information, as well as making friends and chatting.
Male and female adolescents also differ markedly in terms of where
they access the Internet. As revealed in study after study, boys visit
Internet Cafes more often than their female counterparts, who use the
Internet mostly at home and at school (Hsu & Chuang, 2008; Lin &
Yu, 2008; Wu & Cheng, 2007). Internet Cafes may indeed provide a
convenient environment and fast access to the Internet so that customers
can concentrate on their work without interference from others (Wu &
Cheng, 2007). Such a setting, however, has also become a place for
adolescents to indulge in online games. While Internet Cafes are seen as
a masculine gaming space and are thus considered highly gendered (Wu
& Cheng, 2007; Hsu & Chuang, 2008), parents and teachers may
become concerned that those who overly indulge in the Internet,
especially boys, will tend to lag behind academically. As found in a
large survey, high school students in Taiwan who spent more time playing
online games had lower academic achievement in later school years (Chen
& Lu, in press). Although the association is only marginally
significant, it raises an important issue as to the role of gender
differences in understanding how Internet use is correlated with
academic achievement.
HYPOTHESES
Previous studies have shown some associations between Internet use
and academic achievement, while gender differences in online activities
and the location of use of the Internet are well documented. By taking
into account gender differences in patterns of Internet use, the current
study aims to reexamine the association between Internet use and
academic achievement. Based on the above review, the following
hypotheses were formulated.
Hypothesis 1. Adolescents' academic achievement partly depends
on their activities on the Internet earlier in their school years.
1a. Academic achievement will be higher if they use the Internet
more often to search for information.
1b. Achievement will be lower if they use the Internet to
socialize.
1c. Achievement will be lower if they use the Internet to play
games.
Hypothesis 2. Academic achievement will be impaired if adolescents
use the Internet Cafe more often for online activities.
Hypothesis 3. The association between Internet use and academic
achievement differs between male and female adolescents.
METHOD
Sample and Data Collection
Data were taken from the Taiwan Youth Project (TYP), a panel study
based at the Academia Sinica in Taiwan. The project was started in the
year 2000 and has conducted 8 waves of interviews as of the end of 2008.
Students were sampled from middle schools (ages 13-15) located in the
northern part of Taiwan: Taipei City, Taipei County, and Yi-Lan County,
using the multi-stage, stratified cluster sampling method. For the first
wave of the survey, 40 middle schools were randomly selected. In each
school, two classes of 7th graders were chosen at random. All students
in these classes, one of each of their parents, and their homeroom teachers were asked to complete self-administered questionnaires.
The initial successful samples included 2,690 students. The
respondents were re-interviewed each year afterwards, with 2,683, 2,663,
and 2,354 students retained in the second, third, and fourth waves of
the surveys, respectively. For the purpose of this study, data were
drawn from the second wave of the student survey when the student
respondents were in the 8th grade. Only the dependent variables, the
standardized test score of the high school entrance examination, was
taken from the data collected in the third wave. This test score
measures students' academic achievement at the end of 9th grade.
Since many students did not report their scores, the number of
valid cases dropped to 1,409. We conducted t-tests and Chi-square
analyses to check for the differences in any aspect of Internet use
between the respondents that provided their test score and those that
did not. The only difference that turned out to be significant was the
use of Internet Cafes: more students who did not provide their test
scores visited Internet Cafes than those who did provide their scores (p
< .05). Although the remaining valid cases are somewhat biased toward
the non-Internet Cafe users, we expect the findings to be useful for the
understanding of how Internet use is associated with academic
achievement among most adolescents.
Measures
Dependent variable. As noted, the dependent variable--the
self-reported test score in the high school entrance exam--measures
students' academic achievement at the end of the 9th grade as the
major outcome. The valid scores ranged from 30 to 289 in the sample,
with a mean score of 168.1 (see Table 1).
Independent variables. All independent variables were taken from
the students' survey in the 8th grade. For overall frequency of
Internet use, respondents reported how often they used the Internet:
"at least once a day," "two or three times a week,"
"once a week," "seldom," or "never." The
answers were recorded in reverse order so that a higher number indicated
more frequent use. For online activities, respondents were asked what
activities they usually engaged in on the Internet. They were allowed to
choose more than one activity, from: searching for information, chatting
and socializing with friends, playing games, and other activities. Each
activity was coded as a dummy variable. The first three activities
attracted about the same number of students, ranging from 40% to 47%.
The location where one gained access to the Internet most often was
divided into three categories: at home, at school, at the Internet
Cafes, in addition to "others" (e.g., at cram schools, at
someone else's homes). Only the Internet Cafe (about 11%) was used
as a dummy variable in the analysis. Finally, respondents were asked
whether their parents set rules about certain aspects of their life,
including the amount of time spent on Internet use or on computer games.
Over 60% of the students had such parental regulation.
Control variables. To take prior academic achievement into account,
we controlled for the respondents' class ranking in the 8th grade
(i.e., 1 = ranked in the last part of the class, to 2 = the latter part,
3 = ranked between 16-25, 4 = 6-15, or 5 = ranked top 1-5). Since the
class size ranged between 30 and 40 in nearly all sampled classes, the
ordinal categories should be close to a universal measure for prior
achievement. Other controls included gender (male = 1) and father's
and mother's education (i.e., 1 = elementary and lower, 2 = middle
school, 3 = high school and higher).
Data Analyses
After describing the patterns of Internet use among Taiwanese male
and female 8th graders, we first checked gender differences with
Chi-square analyses. Then Pearson correlation was used to identify the
intercorrelations among the variables. Finally, and most importantly, we
performed regression analyses to examine how respondents' academic
achievement in the 9th grade varied on the patterns of Internet use in
the 8th grade, while controlling for background variables and academic
achievement in the 8th grade. To verify if any of such variations
differed between boys and girls, the regression analyses were repeated
after the sample was split by gender.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Most of the respondents used the Internet two or three times a
week. Compared to females, males were online more frequently (see Table
2). In terms of averages, males and females differed markedly in every
aspect of Internet use. For example, more than half the female
respondents used the Internet to search for information, as well as to
chat and socialize. Only about one fourth indicated they used it to play
online games. By contrast, playing games was the most popular Internet
activity for male respondents, with 66% reporting that they usually used
the Internet for games, followed by searching for information (43%), and
chatting and socializing (25%). Gender differences were apparent, as
indicated by Chi-square analyses (see Table 2): More female students
searched for information and chatted and socialized with friends than
did their male counterparts; while more males noted playing games as an
activity they usually engaged in on the Internet. Likewise, a
significantly greater percentage of male respondents reported that they
mostly used the Internet in Internet Cafes. Regarding parental
regulation, more male students said their parents had established rules
about how much time they could spend on the Internet or playing computer
games.
Table 3 presents the intercorrelations among variables. All
variables related to the patterns of Internet use in the 8th grade were
significantly correlated with academic achievement in the 9th grade.
While overall frequency of Internet use, Internet use for searching for
information, and parental regulation of time spent on Internet use were
positively correlated with later academic achievement, chatting and
socializing, playing games online, and going to Internet Cafes were
negatively correlated.
Some of these associations remained significant after background
variables were taken into account. All of these background variables,
gender, father's and mother's education, and prior academic
achievement (in the 8th grade) helped explain academic achievement in
the 9th grade, accounting for 58.5% of the total variance (Model 1,
Table 4). As one would expect, academic achievement in the 8th grade had
the greatest impact on subsequent academic achievement.
When all the background variables were held constant, the frequency
of Internet use alone turned out to be a nonsignificant factor in
understanding how well a student performed on the high school entrance
exam. What they did on the Internet, however, remained critical to how
they performed academically. For example, those who used the Internet to
search for information outscored those who did not by 11.28 points (p
< .001, Model 2). By contrast, students who used the Internet for
chatting and socializing underperformed by an average of 6.32 points.
Those who played online games also scored 6.35 lower, on average (p <
.01, Model 2). Thus, regardless of gender, parents' education, and
how well they were doing academically a year ago, what the adolescents
did on the Internet continued to clearly distinguish who scored better
on the high school entrance exam. The findings confirm Hypotheses 1a,
1b, and 1c. Spending time on the Internet per se had no definite
implication for students' academic achievement, but the types of
online activities indeed played a key role.
The same negative effect also lingered from the use of Internet
Cafes. Even among students who shared a similar background and engaged
in the same Internet activities, those who went to Internet Cafes
consistently performed more poorly on the entrance exam, lowering the
score by 7.39 points, on average (p < .05, Model 3, Table 4). Such an
effect remained significant even after we also controlled for parental
regulation (a factor that helped raise the score itself). Therefore,
even when these adolescents were identical or similar in background
factors, prior academic achievement, the frequency and the activities
they did online, and under similar parental regulation, going to
Internet Cafes alone weakened their academic performance. Hypotheses 2
is thus confirmed.
Although the combined contribution of Internet use was relatively
small, given the rigorous statistical controls we employed and the
prospective longitudinal design of the study (which involved a one-year
gap between independent variables and the dependent variable), it is
impressive that their regression coefficients reached the level of
significance at p < .05 or p < .001. Consistent with the
zero-order correlation, Internet use for searching for information was
positively linked with later academic achievement, whereas chatting and
socializing with friends, playing games, and the use of Internet Cafes
were negatively linked to that achievement.
Previous studies have suggested that male and female adolescents
differ markedly in what they do on the Internet and where they go
online. To disentangle any gender differences in the association between
Internet use and academic achievement, therefore, we split the full
sample by gender and proceeded with further analyses within each group.
As expected, the effects from the background variables remained very
important and substantial in both groups. How well boys and girls did on
the high school entrance exam was definitely affected by both
parents' education and the students' class ranking in the 8th
grade (Table 5), consistent with findings in Table 4. The effects of
Internet use, however, differed somewhat between males and females.
For boys, searching for information online proved to be a positive
and important factor in the exam score (p < .001, Models 1 & 2,
Table 5). Among the three other factors pertaining to Internet use that
resulted in negative impacts on the full sample, however, only playing
online games had a significant and negative effect among boys (Model 1).
Part of such an effect was determined by where the male adolescents
played the games and the extent of parent regulation (Model 2). That is,
although that negative effect lost part of its significance after
Internet Cafe and parental regulation were added into the model (Model
2), playing online games remained the only factor in using the Internet
that hindered academic achievement. Within the male subsample,
furthermore, parental regulation turned out to be another factor that
helped students score better on the entrance exam, an effect that only
emerged after splitting the full sample.
The female subsample revealed a somewhat different pattern as to
how Internet use was linked to academic achievement. Like boys, girls
who went online to search for information also scored significantly
better than those who did not (p < .001, Model 3). Unlike their male
counterparts, however, female adolescents did not score worse if they
used the Internet to play games. Rather, poor performance on the exam
was significantly linked to earlier Internet use for chatting and
socializing (p < .05, Model 3). Also unlike boys, parental regulation
did not help girls score better (Model 4).
Therefore, not only did male and female adolescents differ in the
kinds of activities they mostly engaged in on the Internet and where
they went online, but the ways such Internet use linked to later
academic achievement also varied. While both boys and girls gained from
using the Internet as a main source of information, only boys suffered
by going online for playing games, and only girls scored poorly if they
used the Internet mainly for social purposes. Thus, the previous
findings with the full sample are partly correct, and the current
findings with split subsamples partly confirm Hypothesis 3. These
findings call for modifications along the gender line: The positive
effects of Internet use on academic achievement apply to boys and girls
alike, but one needs to stipulate the negative effects more
discriminately in terms of gender. Using academic performance as the
yardstick, an overuse of the Internet for social purposes makes girls
particularly vulnerable whereas indulging in online games is especially
harmful to boys.
CONCLUSION
Gender differences in online activities are substantial among
Taiwanese adolescents, a finding consistent with a study on Taiwanese
5th and 6th graders (Lin & Yu 2008) and with Western studies on high
school and college students (Chen & Peng, 2008; Griffiths, Davies,
& Chappell, 2004; Odetl et al., 2000; Sherman et al., 2000). On
average, male students use the Internet more frequently than do female
students. They use the Internet for recreational purposes (e.g., online
games) more often than their female counterparts. In contrast, female
students use the Internet more to search for information and to chat and
socialize with friends. Boys also visit Internet Cafe more often to gain
access to the Internet and have more parental regulation of their
Internet use.
Patterns of Internet use are closely linked to academic achievement
later in middle school. Although the frequency of using the Internet in
the 8th grade is not a key factor in distinguishing who scores better on
the high school entrance exam, what students do online clearly
distinguishes such academic performance. While using the Internet to
search for information is positively linked with later academic
performance, Internet use for recreational and social purposes exerts a
negative impact on academic achievement. These results are generally
consistent with prior studies of American college students (Kubey,
Lavin, & Barrows, 2001; Kuh & Hu, 2001). In addition, using
Internet Cafes as the location for accessing the Internet exerts a
negative effect on later academic achievement.
Most importantly, according to further regression analyses, males
and females differ not only in their patterns of Internet use, but in
how these patterns affect their academic performance. For female
students, the most popular online activity in the 8th grade is chatting
and socializing. The more time they spend on this activity, the lower
their scores on the high school entrance exam a year later, an effect
absent among boys. By contrast, it is gaming, the most popular online
activity for males, that significantly lowers boys' scores, but not
girls'. Furthermore, Internet Cafes and parental regulation of
Internet use partly explain why gaming lowers male students' later
test scores, a further modification unique to boys. As a recent study
found in the United Kingdom, boys who play computer games often are more
likely than girls to quarrel with their parents when facing parental
regulation (Livingstone (2007). While parental regulation helps boost
performance on entrance exams, playing online games alone hurts scores,
even after taking into account such regulation and other aspects of
Internet use.
Thus, among students who share similar background characteristics
and are at the same academic level in 8th grade, what they do on the
Internet, rather than how often they go online, has important
implications for how well they will achieve on one of the most important
exams of their lives. During such a process, not only does gender make a
difference in the patterns of Internet use, but it also plays a key role
in differentiating what kinds of online activities help or hinder students' academic achievement in middle school. In studying how
the Internet affects learning or how well students perform in early
adolescence, then, gender remains a critical factor that deserves
further examination. When more cross-national or cross-cultural data
become available, it would be even more fruitful to examine the linkage
between Internet use and academic achievement. Such a comparative
perspective would further identify the extent to which the current
findings can be applied in various social and cultural contexts.
REFERENCES
Chen, S. Y., & Lu, L. (in press). After-school time use in
Taiwan: Its effects on educational achievement and well-being.
Adolescence.
Chen, Y. F., & Peng, S. S. (2008). University students'
Internet use and its relationships with academic performance,
interpersonal relationships, psychosocial adjustment, and
self-evaluation. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11, 467-469.
Clemente, P. C. (1998). State of the Net: The new frontier. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Griffiths, M. D., Davies, M. N. O., & Chappell, D. (2004).
Online computer gaming: A comparison of adolescent and adult gamers.
Journal of Adolescence, 27, 87-96.
Hsu, Y. C., & Chuang, O. L. (2008). Are Internet Cafe gendered
spaces? CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11(2), 224-226.
Hunley, S. A., Evans, J. H., Delgado-Hachey, M., Krise, J., Rich,
T., & Schell, C. (2005). Adolescent computer use and academic
achievement. Adolescence, 40(158), 307-318.
Imhof, M., Vollmeyer, R., & Beierlein, C. (2007). Computer use
and the gender gap: The issue of access, use, motivation, and
performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 23 (2823-2837.
Kubey, R. W., Lavin, M. J., & Barrows, J. R. (2001). Internet
use and collegiate academic performance decrements: Early findings.
Journal of Communication, 51, 366-382.
Kuh, G. D., & Hu, S. (2001). The relationships between computer
and information technology use, selected learning and personal
development outcomes, and other college experiences. Journal of College
Student Development, 42, 217-232.
Lin, C. H., & Yu, S. F. (2008). Adolescent Internet usage in
Taiwan: Exploring gender differences. Adolescence, 43, 317-331.
Livingstone, S. (2007). Strategies of parental regulation in the
media-rich home. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 920-941.
Odell, P. M., Korgen, K. O., Schumacher, P., & Delucchi, M.
(2000). Internet use among female and male college students.
CyberPsychology & Behavior, 3, 855-862.
Sherman, R. C., End, C., Kraan, E., Cole, A., Campbell, J.,
Birchmeier, Z., & Kluasner, J. (2000). The Internet gender gap among
college students: Forgotten but not gone? Cyber Psychology &
Behavior, 3, 885-894.
Suhail, K., & Bargees, Z. (2006). Effects of excessive Internet
use on undergraduate students in Pakistan. CyberPsychology &
Behavior, 9(3), 297-307.
Wu, C. H., & Chen, F. F. (2007). Internet Cafe addiction of
Taiwanese adolescents. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(2), 220-225.
This article drew upon data from the Taiwan Youth Project (TYP), a
panel study funded by the Academia Sinica as a thematic program from
2004 to 2007 (grant number AS-93-TP-C01, http://www.typ.sinica.edu.tw/).
Part of the research framework of this paper was also taken from the
first author's research project sponsored by the National Science
Council, Taiwan (grant number NSC 98-2410-H-007-004-MY2).
Fu, Yang-Chih, Ph.D., Institute of Sociology, Academia Sinica,
Taiwan
Requests for reprints should be sent to Su-Yen Chen, Ph.D. Center
for Teacher Education, National Tsing Hua University, 221 Education
Building, 101 Sec. 2, Kuang-Fu Rd., Hsinchu, TAIWAN, 30013. E-mail:
[email protected]
Table 1 Summary of Variables
Variables Means S.D. Min. Max.
Independent variables
Male 0.506 0.500 0 1
Father's educational level
(1=elementary or lower,
2=middle school, 3 = high 1.754 0.718 1 3
school or higher) Mother's
educational level (1 =
elementary or lower,
2 = middle school, 3 = high 1.630 0.659 1 3
school or higher)
Frequency of Internet use 2.166 1.255 0 4
Internet use for searching 0.475 0.500 0 1
information
Internet use for chatting 0.403 0.491 0 1
and socializing with friends
Internet use for playing games 0.464 0.499 0 1
Interest use in Interest Cafe 0.112 0.316 0 1
Parental regulation on 0.604 0.489 1 1
Interest use
8th academic achievement 3.540 1.045 1 5
(ranking in the class)
Dependent variable
9th grade academic achievement 168.1 53.5 30 289
(test scores of high-
school entrance exam)
Table 2 Gender Differences in Patterns of Internet Use
Male Female
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Frequency of Internet use 2.26 (1.276) 2.07 (1.226)
Internet use for searching
information 0.43 (0.496) 0.52 (0.500)
Internet use for chatting and
socializing with friends 0.25 (0.436) 0.56 (0.497)
Internet use for playing games 0.66 (0.475) 0.27 (0.442)
Internet use in Internet Cafe 0.16 (0.364) 0.07 (0.256)
Parental regulation on
Internet use 0.70 (0.459) 0.51 (0.500)
[chi square]
Frequency of Internet use 17.138 *
Internet use for searching
information 8.332 *
Internet use for chatting and
socializing with friends 115.978 ***
Internet use for playing games 189.333 ***
Internet use in Internet Cafe 25.777 ***
Parental regulation on
Internet use 53.798 ***
* p <.05, ** p <.01 *** p < .001
Table 3 Inter-correlations Among Variables
1 2 3 4
1. Gender 1.000
2. Father's
education 0.033 1.000
3. Mother's
education 0.010 0.622 *** 1.000
4. Prior
achievement -0.100 *** 0.106 *** 0.141 *** 1.000
5. Freq. of
Internet use 0.079 * 0.148 *** 0.152 *** 0.128 ***
6. Search for
info. -0.082 * 0.079 * 0.138 *** 0.179 ***
7. Chat and
socialize -0.307 *** -0.037 -0.007 -0.019
8. Play
games 0.392 *** -0.017 -0.049 -0.115 ***
9. Internet
Cafe 0.137 *** -0.066 * -0.091 ** -0.062 *
10. Parental
regulation 0.197 *** 0.080 * 0.071 * -0.007
11. Academic
achievement -0.031 0.331 *** 0.345 *** 0.719 ***
5 6 7 8
1. Gender
2. Father's
education
3. Mother's
education
4. Prior
achievement
5. Freq. of
Internet use 1.000
6. Search for
info. 0.035 1.000
7. Chat and
socialize 0.149 *** -0.022 1.000
8. Play
games 0.118 *** -0.167 *** -0.057 * 1.000
9. Internet
Cafe 0.027 -0.131 *** 0.041 0.218 ***
10. Parental
regulation 0.118 *** 0.009 -0.016 0.075 *
11. Academic
achievement 0.151 *** 0.250 *** -0.086 * -0.134 ***
9 10
l. Gender
2. Father's
education
3. Mother's
education
4. Prior
achievement
5. Freq. of
Internet use
6. Search for
info.
7. Chat and
socialize
8. Play
games
9. Internet
Cafe 1.000
10. Parental
regulation 0.026 1.000
11. Academic
achievement -0.108 *** 0.069 *
* p <.05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Table 4 Regression Analyses of Academic Achievement
in the 9th Grade
Model 1 Model 2
Male 3.59 (1.91) 5.03 (2.30) *
Father's education 11.87 (1.69) *** 11.45 (1.75) ***
Mother's education 12.08 (1.84) *** 11.17 (1.91) ***
Prior academic 35.03 (0.94) *** 33.80 (1.02) ***
achievement
Frequency of 0.04 (0.97)
Internet use
Use the Internet to:
search for information 11.28 (2.06) ***
chat and socialize -6.32 (2.17) *
play games -6.35 (2.22) *
Go to Internet Cafe
Parental regulation
Constant 2.23 (4.24) 9.04 (5.10)
N 1283 1142
R-square 0.587 0.592
Adjusted R-square 0.585 0.590
F-value 453.66 *** 205.90 ***
Model 3
Male 4.67 (2.35) *
Father's education 11.30 (1.75) ***
Mother's education 10.78 (1.91) ***
Prior academic 33.80 (1.02) ***
achievement
Frequency of -0.22 (0.97)
Internet use
Use the Internet to:
search for information 10.77 (2.06) ***
chat and socialize -6.05 (2.18) *
play games -5.49 (2.24) *
Go to Internet Cafe -7.39 (3.10) *
Parental regulation 4.18 (2.10) *
Constant 8.81 (5.22)
N 1142
R-square 0.596
Adjusted R-square 0.592
F-value 166.82 ***
* p <.05 ** p <.01 *** p <.001
Table 5 Regression Analyses of
Academic Achievement in the 9th Grade by Gender
Males
Model 1 Model 2
Father's 10.75 (2.40) *** 10.21 (2.38) ***
education
Mother's 12.16 (2.59) *** 12.25 (2.58) ***
education
Prior academic 34.15 (139) *** 34.10 (137) ***
achievement
Freq. of 0.76 (1.34) 0.58 (1.34)
Internet use
Use the
Internet to:
search 10.90 (2.87) *** 10.33 (2.85) ***
for info
chat and -3.00 (3.24) -2.89 (3.21)
socialize
play -10.51 (3.00) *** -8.99 (3.11) *
games
Go to -6.86 (3.77)
Internet
cafe
Parental 8.68 (3.06) *
regulation
Constant 12.67 (6.87) 8.19 (7.17)
N 576 576
R-square 0.632 0.639
Adjusted 0.628 0.634
R-square
F-value 139.41 *** 111.51 ***
Females
Model 1 Model 2
Father's 1234 (2.57) *** 12.31 (2.57) ***
education
Mother's 10.02 (2.83) *** 9.73 (2.86) **
education
Prior academic 32.97 (1.52) *** 32.96 (1.53) ***
achievement
Freq. Of -0.80 (1.41) -0.99 (1.42)
Internet use
Use the
Internet to:
search 10.88 (2.97) *** 10.53 (2.99) ***
for info
chat and -9.31 (2.96) * -8.89 (3.04) *
socialize
play -1.72 (3.32) -1.83 (3.32)
games
Go to -5.13 (5.56)
Internet
cafe
Parental 1.14 (2.94)
regulation
Constant 15.08 (7.56) * 15.85 (7.77) *
N 566 566
R-square 0.554 0.554
Adjusted 0.548 0.547
R-square
F-value 198.82 *** 176.84 ***
* P <.05 ** p <.01 *** p <.001