Is the world deteriorating?
Tikuisis, Peter ; Mandel, David R.
THE WORLD IS BECOMING A LESS CONFLICT-RIDDEN PLACE, AND IT HAS BEEN
for decades. Wars and violent conflicts are on the decline, according to
expert consensus. In particular, we note abundant evidence that suggests
that the world is not deteriorating from Steven Pinker's conclusion
of a long-term general decline in global violence (1) to Azar Gat's
analysis pointing to a decline in war since the mid-1990s (2) to the
2013 Human Security Report proclaiming a substantial decline in
conflicts since the early 1990s. (3)
It was for that reason that a systematic analysis of world crisis
data from a global leader in crisis tracking left us baffled. We
analyzed all of the International Crisis Group's monthly
CrisisWatch statistics for the crises it has tracked from its inception
in September 2003 to March 2014. Crisis Group's data paint a
startlingly different picture: a world that has deteriorated
substantially over the past decade. How could this be?
If Crisis Group was a mediocre crisis tracker, then we might grant
the incongruity to incompetence. (4) But it is well resourced, drawing
on multiple sources including over 100 analysts across five continents.
In addition, it boasts a stellar cast. Past and current board trustees
include a former UN High Commissioner; a former US under-secretary of
state and ambassador to the UN, Russia, India, Israel, Jordan, El
Salvador, and Nigeria; and a former UN deputy secretary-general and
administrator of the UN Development Programme. Crisis Group also prides
itself on "generally [being] regarded as the world's leading
source of information, analysis and policy advice on preventing and
resolving deadly conflict." (5) We can confidently rule out
incompetence as a source of incongruence with expert consensus, which
makes this puzzle all the more interesting.
First, the pieces: Crisis Group has tracked or is tracking 130
countries and regions (i.e., distinct conflict zones) over the 127-month
period we examined. Crisis Group does not track every zone in every
month. Some conflict zones were not on Crisis Group's radar when it
started; some that were are not anymore.
We first verified whether there was a change in the number of
conflict zones tracked over time. Contrary to the "improving
world" thesis, we found that there was a slight, but statistically
significant increase in the number of conflict zones that Crisis Group
reported on during roughly the past decade.
We then reasoned that, while the number of conflict zones reported
on had increased, Crisis Group might have been reporting more favorable
conditions over time within those zones. However, that was not the case.
In each month, Crisis Group reports whether the conflict zones that
it tracks have significantly deteriorated, improved, or on balance
remained more or less unchanged. Deterioration means a significant
increase in conflict; for example, "continued to worsen,"
"attacks significantly escalated," and "security further
deteriorated" are some of the descriptions used for this
assessment. Improvement means the opposite; for example,
"comprehensive peace agreement," "new constitution
signed," and "restore state authority." Unchanged means
there was no change in conflict status since the last report.
Over the period we examined. Crisis Group issued 9,378 such
assessments. Deteriorated situations outnumbered improvements in all but
3 of its 127 months of reporting history. And of the 130 conflict zones,
99 (76.2 percent) had a net surplus of deteriorations by March 2014 over
the period they were tracked. Stated differently, over more than a
decade of monthly reporting involving 130 conflict zones, the odds of
deterioration rather than improvement or stasis were 3:1 based on Crisis
Group's reporting.
What makes this puzzle more baffling is the fact that some of
Crisis Group's leaders have issued statements in support of the
improving world thesis. For example, in a 2012 opinion essay entitled
"The Global March Towards Peace," Crisis Group's
president emeritus, Gareth Evans, points out, "The bigger story has
been concealed, as ever, by the media's daily preoccupation with
current bloodshed: Over the last two decades, major wars and episodes of
mass violence worldwide have become much less frequent and deadly."
(6) Also in 2012, then-president Louise Arbour publicly expressed,
"It may surprise some of you that there is actually some good
news--namely that war may be on the retreat.... In fact over the past
two decades the number of conflicts across the world has
decreased." (7)
How could this puzzle be explained, and why was it not noticed
before? We believe the answer to these questions lies in an important
distinction that Nobel Prize-winning psychologist Daniel Kahneman and
colleagues make between what they call the "inside view"
versus the "outside view": two distinct information-processing
styles that people can adopt. (8) The inside view is case based,
nonstatistical, and has a narrative structure. It represents the story
we might tell about a specific conflict zone at a given time or about
that particular case as it develops over time.
In contrast, the outside view is category based, statistical, and
can be put into a narrative structure only with concerted mental effort.
Our analysis of Crisis Group's indicators is eminently based on an
outside view approach. The inferences we drew from its data came from
looking across all time points and all reported conflict zones. We were
not concerned with the narratives, as accurate as they may be, that
accompanied specific reports about individual conflicts, or with the
narrative trajectories of any particular conflict over time, but only
with what the big picture revealed based on an across-the-board
aggregation of its front-page indicators.
Therein lies the key to the puzzle we uncovered. The inside view is
the default explanatory system for humans. People are naturally adept at
telling (and listening to) stories and comparably poor at pulling out
inferences from large impersonal databases. It is why intelligence
analysts value good narrative over statistical prediction--or, as former
CIA veteran Sherman Kent lamented, a preponderance of "poets"
and a paucity of "mathematicians." (9)
It is also why people tend to be moved to action more by a single
evocative story of personal suffering than by a cold statistic about
mass casualties of genocide and other forms of collective violence--what
Paul Slovic calls "psychological numbing." (10) The inside
view, our cognitive default, actively and automatically promotes it
since we emotionally connect with a specific case of suffering more than
with even a staggering, but seemingly impersonal statistic.
As good as Crisis Group's conflict analyses might be, they
adopt an inside view approach. While they may report on many conflicts,
those analyses are still case focused. The problem is that the inside
view, although psychologically compelling, is prone to systematic biases
such as base rate neglect and overconfidence in forecasting. (11)
Switching to the outside view can offer useful consistency checks
on inside view thinking. In our study, it revealed a glaring
inconsistency between a world leader in conflict tracking and expert
consensus on the decline in violent conflict since the 1990s. That
inconsistency was not explicit in any Crisis Group report, but could be
discerned only by looking holistically and quantitatively across its
full reporting history. Indeed, as we have also noted, that
inconsistency is evident even within Crisis Group since some of its
leaders have noted the improving world trend while presiding over an
organization whose aggregate indicator analyses show an opposing
deteriorating trend.
That revelation is important. Policy advisers, such as Crisis
Group, that rely primarily on an inside view approach will benefit from
periodic review of what their reporting shows when it is subjected to an
outside view analysis. Anomalies or inconsistencies that are revealed
might point to the need for procedural reforms.
For instance, we examined when conflict zones disappeared from
Crisis Group's radar. We expected that reporting would stop only
after a zone had stopped deteriorating for at least a few months prior,
but we found no consistent pattern. In some cases, deterioration was
reported just before conflict reporting had stopped, by as little as one
month. When we looked across all zones where reporting had stopped for
six months or more, we found 36.1 percent ended with a surplus of
deteriorations to improvements compared to only 13.9 percent where
improvements exceeded deteriorations. That suggests that situations that
had changed prior to when reporting stopped were more likely to be worse
than when they began.
It is important to interrogate the data on any topic from both the
inside and the outside views and to triangulate the results from those
alternative perspectives. If they do not add up, there is a good chance
that overreliance on the inside view is responsible.
There is also the possibility that improvements, which likely are
not reported as frequently by the media as deteriorations, fall below
the detection threshold or acceptance criteria for consideration.
Although media bias, reflecting the fact that bad news sells, should not
be an overriding concern given that Crisis Group conducts its own
analyses, we acknowledge that even the best analysts may be subtly
influenced by the preponderance of negative reports.
There is another potentially biasing factor in reporting by Crisis
Group that ought to be noted; namely, Crisis Group's advocacy
mission, which seeks to prevent violence and stop violent conflicts that
are already in play. To influence the international community to take
conflict-mitigating action, it is more effective to emphasize the
negative than the positive. Indeed, it is a well-established fact that
negative events are more likely to capture people's attention and
motivate their subsequent action than positive events. (12) Thus, Crisis
Group's focus on deterioration may be partly strategic.
On a positive note, our further outside view analysis offers some
support for Crisis Group's Watchlist reporting, which "alerts
readers to situations where, in the coming month, there is a particular
risk of new or significantly escalated conflict, or a particular
conflict resolution opportunity." We explored whether more reports
of deteriorations versus improvements would follow risk alerts, and
whether more reports of improvements versus deteriorations would follow
resolution opportunities. We found thirty-six deteriorated situations
versus six improved situations in the month following a "Conflict
Risk Alert," and twelve improved situations versus seven
deteriorated situations in the month following a "Conflict
Resolution Opportunity." Hence, Crisis Group's forecasting
odds are 6:1 in favor of risk alerts leading to deterioration and 2:1 in
favor of resolution opportunities leading to improvements. Of course, we
also note that the forecasts and subsequent indicators are not from
independent sources. They are in fact from the same source--Crisis
Group. Nevertheless, the analysis indicates the sort of consistency
check that monitoring organizations could self-apply.
So, is the world deteriorating? It might appear so from a
conflict-specific inside view but, as pointed out earlier, this
perspective can distort the long-term and conflict-general outside view.
According to scholarly consensus, the outside view points to a gradual
de-escalation in global conflict prior to and during the period that
Crisis Group has been tracking conflicts. The high quality of Crisis
Group's work notwithstanding, we do not believe that its aggregate
indicator findings seriously challenge that conclusion.
[c] Her Majesty the Queen in Right of Canada, as represented by the
Minister of National Defence, 2015.
Notes
Peter Tikuisis is a senior scientist for the Socio-Cognitive
Systems Section at Defence Research and Development Canada, Toronto
Research Centre. He is also adjunct research professor at the Norman
Paterson School of International Affairs, Carleton University, Ottawa.
He has published research in bubble physics, thermal physiology, soldier
performance, and most recently on terrorism and state fragility. David
R. Mandel is a senior scientist for the Socio-Cognitive Systems Section
at Defence Research and Development Canada, Toronto Research Centre. He
is also adjunct professor of psychology at York University, Toronto. He
has published research in human thinking and reasoning, judgment and
decisionmaking, and the psychological bases of conflict and collective
violence.
(1.) Steven Pinker, The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence
Has Declined (New York: Viking Penguin Press, 2011).
(2.) Azar Gat, "Is War Declining--and Why?" Journal of
Peace Research 50, no. 2 (2012): 149-157.
(3.) Human Security Research Group, "Human Security Report
2013: The Decline in Global Violence: Evidence, Explanation, and
Contestation," Human Security Report Project, Simon Fraser
University, Vancouver, Canada, www.hsr
group.org/human-security-reports/2013/text.aspx, accessed 23 June 2014.
(4.) International Crisis Group, "Crisis Watch: A Monthly
Bulletin on Current and Potential Conflicts Around the World,"
Brussels (headquarters), Belgium,
www.crisisgroup.org/en/publication-type/crisiswatch.aspx, accessed 30
April 2014.
(5.) See www.crisisgroup.org/en/about.aspx, accessed 25 November
2014.
(6.) Gareth Evans, "The Global March Toward Peace,"
Project Syndicate, 2012, www.gevans.org/opeds/oped136.html.
(7.) Louise Arbour, "Crisis and Conflict: Global Challenges in
2012," International Crisis Group speech, 2012,
www.crisisgroup.org/en/publication-type/speeches
/2012/crisis-and-conflict-global-challenges-in-2012.aspx.
(8.) See, for example, Daniel Kahneman and Dan Lovallo, "Timid
Choices and Bold Forecasts: A Cognitive Perspective on Risk
Taking," Management Science 39, no. 1 (1993): 17-31.
(9.) Sherman Kent, "Words of Estimative Probability,"
1964, Central Intelligence Agency,
https://www.cia.gov/library/center-for-the-study-of-intelligence/csi-publications /books-and-monographs/sherman-kent-and-the-board-of-national-estimates -collected-essays/6words.html.
(10.) Paul Slovic, "If I Look at the Mass I Will Never Act:
Psychic Numbing and Genocide," Judgment and Decision Making 2, no.
2 (2007): 79-95.
(11.) Kahneman and Lovallo, "Timid Choices and Bold
Forecasts."
(12.) Shelley E. Taylor, "Asymmetrical Effects of Positive and
Negative Events: The Mobilization-minimization Hypothesis,"
Psychological Bulletin 110, no. 1 (1991): 67-85.