Mitigating circumstances in death penalty decisions: using evidence-based research to inform social work practice in capital trials.
Schroeder, Julie ; Guin, Cecile C. ; Pogue, Rene 等
Social workers are increasingly assuming the role of mitigation
specialist and becoming central figures in multidisciplinary capital
defense teams (Guin, Noble, & Merrill, 2003; Schroeder, 2003;
Stetler, 1999). Those who advocate for social workers' entry into
this emerging field argue that the profession is rooted in and
distinguished by traditions of advocating for the powerless and working
for social justice (Roberts & Brownell, 1999). Advanced clinical
skills (Alfonso & Baur, 1986; Andrews, 1991; McCoy, 1999) as well as
training in systems and ecological theoretical approaches (Schroeder)
appear to be very useful in uncovering and interpreting the
biopsychosocial realities of individuals charged with capital offenses.
In fact, the gold standard for mitigation practice is reliance on
focused investigation and presentation of evidence about a
defendant's life and character that is sufficient to compel jurors
to consider a complexity of personal, social, and ecological issues in
deciding attribution of responsibility that mitigate defendants'
legal accountability in capital offenses. This article presents a
conceptual framework of problems that surround mitigation practice and
argues that better practices can be crafted from a broader systemic
approach that is grounded in empirical evidence and practice and goes
beyond current parameters of mitigation practice.
EMERGENCE OF CONTEMPORARY MITIGATION PRACTICE STANDARDS
In the incidence of capital cases, the U. S. Supreme Court has
recognized what research has long shown: Jurors often make sentencing
decisions prematurely, and they often base their decisions on their
personal reactions to the defendant, their confusion about the rules of
law, and their lack of understanding regarding their own role and
responsibilities (Bowers, Sandys, & Steiner, 1998; Sandys, 1995). In
an effort to safeguard against jurors' "unguided emotional
response[s]" when determining punishment for capital defendants,
the Court set forth a legal standard that requires defense teams in
capital cases to conduct thorough investigations into the
defendants' backgrounds. If the defendant is found guilty, the
information gleaned from that investigation must then be presented as
mitigating evidence during the penalty phase of the trial (Furman v.
Georgia, 1972). The Court's monumental decision has led attorneys
to seek the aid of social workers to conduct extensive social history
investigations on clients who are defendants in capital cases. As noted
by Schroeder (2003):
The capital mitigation process comes to life
when a social worker, using a life history model
of investigation, assumes the role of mitigation
specialist, who, by capitalizing on social work
theory and research, practice knowledge and
skills yields vital information that, through objective
presentation of fact, guides sentencing
decisions. (p. 424)
The life history model relies on traditional practice methods
including case history development, multidimensional assessment, record
review, mediation, and multidisciplinary teamwork. This extensive case
investigation yields rich historical data that are then considered in
light of factors empirically linked with criminal behavior. This method
provides the legal team with a consistent defense theme that is infused
throughout both the evidence and penalty phases of the trial. A
mitigation outcome is considered ineffective when the jurors fail to
comply with the rules and the roles that are imposed on them as jurors,
for example, in making premature decisions.
CHALLENGING TRADITIONS
The established mitigation strategy that relies on social histories
and life-stories survives and thrives as a legal mandate and preferred
approach in capital cases despite of a lack of empirical research that
demonstrates it is an effective strategy. In fact, available empirical
legal research suggests the contrary: Jurors often disregard or
misunderstand mitigating circumstances when making sentencing decisions
(Diamond 8,: Levi, 1996; Eisenberg, Garvey, & Wells, 1996, 1998;
Garvey, Johnson, & Marcus, 2000; Luginbuhl & Howe, 1995;
McDougall v. Dixon, 1990). The evidence against the potency of
presenting defendants on the witness stand to influence sentencing
decisions is compelling and warns attorneys and social worker mitigation
specialists that current legal and practice standards are perhaps too
unreliable and ineffective to be considered the best possible practice.
When practice and empirical evidence from a range of disciplines that
hold stake in jurisprudence is considered, problems with currently
preferred tactics are more visible, and a more complex set of practice
methods and strategies is suggested. Furthermore, the literature
suggests certain implications for research and practice, including how
mitigation activities are conceptualized and conducted and the
effectiveness of the social work mitigation specialist in presenting the
information that has been developed.
PENALTY-PHASE JUROR DECISION MAKING
Professional literature demonstrates that many disciplines have an
interest and a stake in the legal process and outcomes related to
capital offenses. The existing body of knowledge, however, is at once
complex and disjointed, but provides a conceptual framework of the
problems that surround mitigation work and potential best practices to
evolve effectiveness. From one perspective, cognitive psychologists seek
to understand the role that strong emotions play in creating feelings
often tied to pre-existing beliefs, which in turn lead to actions that
help to dissipate those strong feelings (Werner, 1986). In addition,
attribution theorists examine the manner in which people interpret and
explain events (Fincham & Jaspars, 1980), leading to their
assignment of responsibility and blame (Coates & Penrod, 1981). This
provides a foundation for understanding how and why jurors decide to
impose the ultimate punishment. These processes are elaborated by
descriptive studies that identify how jurors' strong emotional
reactions to capital defendants often result in a death verdict
(Eisenberg et al., 1998, Steiner 1999; Sundby, 1998; Tomes, 1997). At
times made prematurely, jury decisions appear to be based largely on
personal values and emotional responses that rarely relate to the facts
of the case and the potential mitigating influences on criminal
behavior. Jurors often make decisions on the basis of their personal
reactions to the defendant that are developed during the guilt phase of
the trial (Garvey, 1998; Garvey, 2000). Furthermore, there appears to be
misunderstanding of the statutory obligations that specifically dictate
juror roles and responsibilities.
Juror Emotion
Jurors have reported a full range of emotional responses, from
fear, anger, and disgust to sympathy and positive regard (Eisenberg et
al., 1998; Garvey, 2000; Slobogin, 2003; Sundby, 1998; Tomes, 1997).
Such a complicated mixture of emotions combined with the
often-incomprehensible language of capital punishment statutes often
results in a recipe for errors in judgment on life and death decisions
(Eisenberg et al., 1998; Steiner, 1999). In attempting to understand the
interaction of personal values and emotions with decision making, the
field of cognitive psychology offers a possible explanation for the
complex activities that may take place in the mind of the juror. Taken
together, these findings advise social work practitioners who act as
mitigation specialists to consider the role personal values and
emotional reactions play in jury decision making as well as the effect
juror ignorance of process may produce.
The literature demonstrates that other factors complicate jury
decision making. For example, Luginbuhl and Howe (1995) found that
jurors expressed fear about making a decision in a capital case because
they were confused about the statues regarding sentencing options.
Jurors' fear about a defendant's future dangerousness mixed
with erroneous beliefs concerning sentencing limits was found to lead to
an erroneous belief that capital defendants may serve a sentence of less
than 20 years and may be afforded parole opportunities. Similarly,
Eisenberg and colleagues (2001) and Sandys (1995) found that fear about
parole was cited as an important rationale behind some jurors' vote
for death. As a whole, these findings suggest that part of mitigation
process and practice should involve clarifying, in practical terms, the
actual consequences of sentencing options and other complex legal
considerations surrounding death penalty decisions.
Juror Perception
Defendant behavior while in custody (Sundby, 1998) and demeanor
during the trial (Eisenberg et al., 1998; Tomes, 1997) have been
demonstrated as additional complicating features of the problems that
surround death penalty decisions and that often give rise to anger among
jurors. In fact, these two factors were shown to receive jurors'
greatest consideration during deliberations. Jurors were also often
angered when a defendant appeared bored or uninterested in the trial
proceedings, and their anger often translated into a belief that the
defendant lacked remorse. These perceptions that defendants lack remorse
invoked anger, often mixed with disgust, regarding aggravating
circumstances and were associated with death verdicts, regardless of the
mitigating evidence produced during the penalty phase (Eisenberg et al.,
1998). A remorseful defendant, who appears to take responsibility for
his or her actions, evokes juror sympathy and support (Tomes). Remorse
appears to work in the defendant's favor in two ways. First, jurors
view defendants' apparent remorse as being the correct moral
reaction to the crime. Second, perceived remorse on the part of a
defendant is viewed as evidence that the defendant will not be dangerous
in the future (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Sundby hypothesized that some
jurors determine remorse by the manner in which the case is presented to
them, rather than the actual remorse shown by the defendant in the
courtroom. Nonetheless, remorse is a significant issue to consider when
communicating mitigating influences to a jury. Taken together, evidence
from these studies cautions those who act as mitigation specialists to
help defense teams find ways to frame evidence and highlight reported
and observable behavior in ways that suggest and underscore a
defendant's remorse.
Statutory Disregard or Confusion
Luginbuhl and Howe (1995),Tiersma (1995), Ritter (2004), and Wiener
and colleagues (2004) argued that jurors do not understand mitigation
because the instructions they receive are presented in legal language
that is difficult for laypeople to understand. When jurors cannot rely
on the guiding legal principles, they often fall back on prior or folk
knowledge as a basis for decision making (Steiner, 1999; Wiener et al.).
Furthermore, capital jurors report frequent confusion over the
issues of aggravating and mitigating circumstances and the level of
agreement needed for each. Such confusion often results in a death
verdict (Eisenberg & Wells, 1993; Wiener et al., 2004). When Garvey
(1998) queried jurors about what, according to statute, and in their
minds, constituted aggravating factors, respondents reported that
especially brutal murders, child victims, future dangerousness, and lack
of remorse were the most salient aggravating circumstances. The
defendant's prior criminal record and history of alcohol or drug
abuse were lesser, yet pertinent, aggravating circumstances.
The legally required level of agreement about aggravating
circumstances has been found to be especially confusing to jurors when
weighing them in the punishment phase of a trial. Legal scholars have
found that jurors incorrectly believed aggravating factors did not have
to be proven beyond a reasonable doubt (Eisenberg & Wells, 1993),
and they mistakenly believed anything could be considered aggravating,
even evidence not put forth in the state's argument (Luginbuhl
& Howe, 1995). Furthermore, many jurors believed that the death
penalty was absolutely required when crimes were considered heinous (Eisenberg et al., 1996; Garvey, 2000; Garvey et al., 2000; Sandys,
1995).
Jurors' confusion about aggravating circumstances is also
evident in research on the effect of mitigating evidence. Jurors stress
the importance of certain mitigating factors considered during
deliberations, including residual doubt about the defendant's
guilt, mental retardation, and youthfulness of the defendant at the time
of the crime, circumstances out of the defendant's control, and
circumstances that diminish the defendant's responsibility for the
crime (for example, multiple defendants). Factors receiving less
consideration, yet still considered to be mitigating, include victims
with criminal histories and defendants with histories of mental illness,
child abuse, poverty, and time spent in state institutions (Garvey,
1998). Many jurors incorrectly believe mitigating factors must rise to
the reasonable doubt burden of proof when in fact mitigating factors
need only outweigh the state's presented aggravating circumstances
(Luginbuhl & Howe, 1995). Jurors also mistakenly assume mitigating
circumstances require the jury's unanimous vote when in fact, only
one juror need consider a life circumstance to be mitigating (Garvey,
2000; Luginbuhl & Howe).
In theory, legal process in capital cases is expected to proceed in
a stepwise fashion. In practice, jurors appear to "multitask"
in ways that undermine the intent of prescribed process and
significantly influence juror decision making. For example, the law
requires jurors to withhold consideration of punishment during the guilt
phase and make this decision after aggravating and mitigating
circumstances are presented during the penalty phase of the trial
(Furman v. Georgia, 1972). Yet, nearly one-half of jurors in one study
(Bowers et al., 1998), and 70 percent of jurors in another (Sandys,
1995) indicated they had decided on punishment during the guilt phase of
the trial before aggravating and mitigating circumstances were
presented. Of those jurors who decided the punishment during the guilt
portion of the trial, more than half of them maintained that decision
throughout the trial. Jurors who reported reaching early decisions about
punishment did so because of their predisposition about crime,
punishment, the death penalty, and experiences during the guilt phase.
Surprisingly, on hearing the state's evidence, a significant number
of jurors decided that the death penalty was not only acceptable
punishment for first-degree murder, but also for other types of murder
as well. These findings advise mitigation practices that ensure
jurors' thorough understanding of the legal issues that surround
individual cases and options of legal response and, as important, trial
strategies that remind jurors over and over again of their specific
responsibilities associated with each step of the judicial process.
Abdication of Responsibility
Jurors who do not understand what is legally expected of them in
capital trials are more likely to abdicate the responsibility for their
decisions to others. When jurors fail to maintain a sense of
responsibility for deciding verdicts and sentences, they are also less
likely to critically consider the influence of mitigating circumstances
on criminal behavior. Jurors tend to abdicate independent responsibility
in a number of ways. For example, it is the sole responsibility of each
juror to independently vote for life, life without parole, or death.
Empirical evidence suggests however, that some capital jurors who vote
for the death penalty are likely to rationalize a death vote by
abdicating responsibility for that decision to the trial judge, the
community, other jurors, the law, and the defendant (Eisenberg et al.,
1996; Hoffman, 1995; Sherman, 1995).
Abdicating independent responsibility to the judge occurs more
frequently among premature decision makers (Eisenberg et al., 1996), who
believe that their verdict is merely a recommendation (Hoffman, 1995;
Sherman, 1995). Sherman also argued that when jurors consider the judge
as the final decision maker, they feel less responsibility for the
verdict. Moreover, jurors who believe that they are merely
representatives of the greater community feel less responsible for their
decisions about punishment. Yet, they may paradoxically see themselves
as only one vote and look to the voting behavior of their fellow jurors
as a means of relinquishing personal responsibility.
There are a number of other risky situations when jurors abdicate
their independent responsibility in decision making that should concern
practitioners and legal advocates. One risk is that jurors may rely on
the legal system to avoid taking personal responsibility for their
decision. For example, jurors who falsely believe that the law requires
a verdict of death for certain types of murder (Eisenberg et al., 1996;
Sherman, 1995) frequently feel that the responsibility for a death
sentence belongs to the legal system. In addition, beliefs about how the
judicial system's appeal process operates have also been found to
influence jurors' the level of personal responsibility. The appeals
process is touted as a lengthy, time-consuming procedure, and jurors who
do not think executions are actually carried out on a regular basis are
less likely to accept responsibility for the verdict. Finally, research
indicates that two critical factors appear to be associated with the
ease with which a juror abdicates responsibility for his or her
decision. These are when the juror views the crime as being especially
brutal or premeditated (Sherman). Because crimes that lead to capital
charges often dehumanize defendants proportionately to the crimes
against their victims, it is especially important that social workers
serving as mitigation specialists present evidence with some finesse
that humanizes defendants and clearly informs jurors about the scope of
consequences that will arise from their decision.
In summary, it is clear that life and death verdicts are made for a
variety of reasons unrelated to the actual facts of the case, the legal
responsibilities of the jury, and the weight of the mitigating evidence
that is presented. The literature clearly delineates the problem areas
that can guide the interventions by social work practitioners involved
in mitigation work and suggests important areas for forensic social work education and training.
A MODEL TO INFORM PRACTICE
When practice and empirical evidence is considered, the problems
that surround juror decision making and mitigation practice are more
complex than implied by the legal remedy (Furman v. Georgia, 1972) that
requires evidentiary presentation of a thorough investigation of a
defendant's background. Based on issues identified in a broad body
of literature, the conceptual problem model shown in Figure 1
demonstrates the complicating feature of the problems that surround
death penalty decisions. During preparation for trial, social workers
must remain cognizant of how jurors' personal values and emotions
will likely affect their decision making.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Research and Practice Implications
The model delineates the areas that social work mitigation
specialists are advised to consider when devising ways in which to
improve practice outcomes in capital trials. It is clear that practice
and empirical evidence on juror decision making suggests certain
practical implications for social work mitigation specialists and legal
professionals, allowing us to expand on traditional methods of inquiry
and investigation and develop ways to use social work skills and
advanced professional competencies to counter the effects of juror
emotion, confusion, and abdication of roles and responsibilities. By
broadening the focus of intervention beyond a psychosocial investigation
of mitigating factors in a defendant's background, social workers
create possibilities for intervening at multiple points in the legal
system and process. A more systems-oriented approach requires social
workers who act as mitigation specialists to take a more proactive role
in the entire defense process. Going beyond the interpersonal work
required to develop mitigating evidence to using education and advocacy
strategies that provide richer, more fruitful consultancy to attorneys
and the courts. A multidimensional, systems-oriented approach should
help social work mitigation specialists apply the practice and empirical
evidence discussed earlier to carve out a larger role in capital cases
and improve both the mitigation investigation and work as part of a
defense team. Proactive intervention at multiple points in the system
and in the legal process promises better prospects for creating a link
among the client, the defense team, and the jury. In the end, a larger
role that applies practice and empirical evidence to social work and
legal processes can help protect against the possibility that the
evidence-based factors identified in our model will prevent jurors from
concentrating on the mitigating circumstances detailed in the
defendant's life story.
Intervening in the System
Intervening with Defense Attorneys. To ensure that defense
strategies that neutralize factors negatively influencing juror decision
making, we advise social workers to use relationship-building strategies
that encourage a larger advisory role. Educating attorneys about the
full range of social work skills and qualifications facilitates a more
maximal social work role in the defense process. Informing attorneys
about the risks outlined in professional literature and depicted in our
model helps them plan trial strategies that may offset undesired
decision-making factors and processes. Likewise, helping defense
attorneys to develop improved relationship-building and interviewing
skills not only enhances their ability to connect with potential jurors
during voir dire (jury selection), but also results in thoughtful
answers during voir dire on potentially life-threatening issues. This
personal connection, and the resultant information gleaned from
potential jurors, could assist the defense team in selecting individuals
who will remain cognizant of the impact that their emotions have on
their ability to make decisions. Jurors relate to the best communicator,
and social workers can assist attorneys in learning how to communicate
to the laypeople who will eventually become jurors (Caldwell, Perrin,
& Frost, 2002).
Intervening with the Team. Through relationship building and
education strategies, social workers can increase their role as a vital
link between the defendant and members of the defense team (Schroeder,
2003). Using basic and advanced interpersonal skills, social workers
acting as mitigation specialists can expand their role by facilitating
communication and collaboration and helping counsel recognize the
effects of the existing power differential among themselves, the
defendant, and jurors (Alfonso & Baur, 1986). Affecting inherent
power differentials, social workers must model advocacy skills to help
attorneys identify with and advocate for jurors in need of clarification
as well as assist in creating an atmosphere that promotes supportive
development of relationships. It is vital that judges provide clear
instructions, and if they do not, jurors must be empowered to keep
asking questions until they fully understand their obligations under the
law. Attorneys must also advocate for the defendant, and this commitment
must be clear to the defendant so he or she will cooperate with the
attorney and the mitigation specialists during the trial. Finally,
social workers must model advocacy skills to help attorneys identify
with and advocate for changes in the legal system that must be made to
deal with problems such as juror confusion and misunderstanding. The
practitioner's role as advocate should extend to state legislatures
where court procedures regarding jury instructions, jury questioning of
the judge and witnesses, and .juror note taking must be addressed by
statute.
Intervening with the Jury
Selecting the Jury. Like their highly trained forensic psychologist
counterparts who have been working with attorneys to select juries for
many years (Cutler, 1990; Fischoff, 2003; Vore, 1989), social workers
who--as a group--have been long employed to complete legally mandated
defendant background investigations are increasingly being used by
public defenders. Consequently, a growing number of social workers will
undoubtedly find themselves in the position of assisting defense
attorneys in jury selection. Social workers can most effectively
intervene in this point of the legal system by applying best practices
in assessment and in questionnaire development to the jury selection
process to identify and screen out jury candidates who will be most
likely to demonstrate potential for severe or erroneous decision making
identified in the literature and our proposed model. For example, a
large body of research suggests that early decision makers are more
likely to vote for death, largely because they have not considered
mitigating factors. So, it is vitally important to identify people with
these tendencies during voir dire.
Using best research practices, survey questions should be
structured in ways to identify jurors who have authority and compliance
issues. These qualities can be determined by asking questions about
issues not related specifically to the law but regarding biopsychosocial
factors that jurors might respond to more readily and comfortably, such
as medication compliance, paying taxes, and dealing with coworkers or
neighborhood associations. To be sure, many potential jurors with
compliance issues will be deemed appropriate (death qualified) for
service. Therefore, it is vital for the defense team to identify the
most "dangerous" of that group using peremptory challenges to
exclude them from service. Being close to the jury selection process
also helps mitigation specialists when developing the penalty phase
testimony of experts, family, and friends. Knowing the jurors'
attitudes, values, and personal life experiences provides details that
can be used to personalize the penalty phase testimony.
Educating the Jury. Social work mitigation practitioners should
develop educational interventions for the defense team to use to improve
juror understanding of confusing issues, such as aggravating and
mitigating circumstances. Clear language and rephrasing techniques
should be used to guarantee that each, juror has a clear understanding
of and can explain, in his or her own words, what constitutes mitigating
and aggravating circumstances.
Life history timeline diagrams, models of pathways into criminal
behavior, and photographs are commonly used to help jurors come to know
the defendant during the penalty phase (Guin & Merrill, 2000).
Ogloff's ("Jurors Can't Understand," 1998)
preliminary use of decision trees to help jurors understand substantive
instructions in making their decisions offers promise and could prove
useful in assisting jurors in using mitigating evidence in their
decision making in capital trials. Visual reinforcement of the rules and
their application helps jurors use all of the information presented to
them in an objective manner and promotes the integration of mitigating
evidence into their overall deliberations. Decision trees can provide
structure and organization to illustrate, explain, and simplify the
penalty phase evidence and issues guiding jurors' decisions with
clarity that words alone cannot accomplish.
Furthermore, by sharing with the defense team knowledge of human
behavior, social workers can help the team predict and develop trial
strategies that are likely to minimize or neutralize jurors'
responses to complicated and often gruesome information about violent
criminality and victimization. In addition, practitioners must also
recommend competent experts who can discuss troublesome issues that
often result in confusion and misunderstanding. For example,
identification and recruitment of a correctional expert to testify about
the realities of sentencing (life without the benefit of parole) and
prison security issues during the penalty phase of the trial should
alleviate juror confusion, frustration, and the desire "to know
what the punishment would actually be if they did not impose a death
sentence" (Bowers & Steiner, 1999, p. 605).
Intervening with Defendant Clients. Work with the defendant must
include efforts to address negative pretrial demeanor and trial behavior
that negatively affect juror's decisions. Remorse and
responsibility are exceptionally difficult issues for many defendants to
demonstrate, particularly defendants who suffer from mental illness or
retardation and those who maintain their innocence. Helping clients
understand the effects of their verbal and nonverbal communication styles and the defendant's active involvement in improving on those
elements may increase the likelihood that he or she might build a more
positive relationship with jurors. Bem's (1978) pioneering research
purported that an individual's body language reveals more than
two-thirds of the emotional meaning of a message. It is vitally
important that social workers help defendants understand what their
nonverbal behavior communicates to jurors and that those jurors may
ascribe meaning to each and every gesture or expression both intentional
and unintentional.
Often defendants admit to having some involvement in the crime for
which they are being tried. It is important that the client admit being
present or partially responsible for the outcome of the incident, if
this is consistent with the legal strategy. Clients must understand the
gravity of their behavior and the feelings of loss that the
victim's loved ones are experiencing. In this way, jurors can see
that although the client maintains that he or she did not kill the
victim, he or she admits to playing some role in the victim's
demise. Honest and sincere client reaction to the aggravating
circumstances must reflect feelings of sadness, regret, remorse, and
responsibility.
Attorneys often have difficulty working with clients to develop
these feelings so they can be appropriately displayed on the witness
stand; thus, the issue must be dealt with very early in the
investigation and relationship-building process. Investigating the
client's feelings regarding the crime may help elicit genuine
responses that can be developed through journaling, artwork, and flank
discussion later taken into the courtroom to mitigate the effects of the
aggravating circumstances.
The defendants themselves often do not understand how they can
participate in their own defense. Social workers who have developed a
solid relationship with their client can assist him or her in
understanding what he or she can do to affect the sentencing outcome.
Some will have little problem with this; however, many capital
defendants are poorly educated, have borderline intelligence, or are
mentally retarded or mentally ill. They will not make a positive
impression in the courtroom without a great deal of education on the
courtroom setting and roles and responsibilities of various court
personnel, followed by practice or role playing. Role playing is
especially helpful for the undereducated or disabled client to assist
him or her in speaking clearly with proper tone and rate. Public
speaking skills will also enhance the defendant's acceptability
during pretrial status hearings if the judge speaks directly to the
defendant. Proper grammar and syntax will give the client confidence and
work to humanize him or her.
Later, during voir dire, if the defendant has the opportunity to
speak before the jury selection pool, proper elocution gives potential
jurors the opportunity to develop a larger construct of who this
individual is. A person without a voice remains a mystery. A person
charged with a heinous killing who is not allowed a voice becomes a
silent beast to jurors as the evidence is put forth during the trial.
Judgments are often made about people based on their physical appearance
and verbal communication skills. The social worker is well equipped to
assist difficult clients in presenting themselves in a manner that could
be helpful to their defense.
CONCLUSION
Because of the advocacy and social justice traditions of the
profession as well as their specialized education, training, and
experience, social workers are particularly well suited to work as part
of a defense team in capital cases. Increasingly, social workers are
becoming widely accepted and employed to complete legally mandated
investigations, convey relevant information to officers of the court,
and provide evidentiary presentations of death penalty defendants'
background to juries. In this respect, social workers have gained entry
into an area of practice that can be significantly improved by expanding
their role to include interventions at multiple points in the legal
system and process.
However, most social workers are added to defense teams composed of
public defenders representing indigent clients. Consequently, there is a
risk that social workers' role will be restricted because of
misconceptions about the full range of their potential as defense team
members. In contrast to higher paid psychologist--often in private
practice--who act as trial and jury selection specialists, social
workers may suffer the social bias and prejudice about competency and
range of skill that comes from being viewed as public servants. We
advise social workers to use their professional skills to gain the
standing--enjoyed by too few in our profession--that is necessary to
engage fully in mitigation practice that applies a systems approach and
evidence-based knowledge.
The empirical evidence discussed here clearly delineates how juror
emotion, confusion and disregard, and abdication of responsibility can
lead juries to overlook mitigation evidence in capital trials. We
propose a greater role for social workers as mitigation specialists in
death penalty cases by using a systems approach that is informed by
practice and empirical evidence to intervene at multiple points in the
legal system and process. To accomplish this aim, social workers must be
cognizant of the factors that influence juror decision making and the
skills required to intervene to ensure a just legal process. A critical
requirement of best practice in this arena is that social workers remain
up to date on current legal research on juror decision making in capital
trials, use traditional social work methods most effectively, and
develop new competencies to meet the needs of the defense team, the
client, and society.
Armed with an intellectual understanding of the value of mitigating
circumstances, the practitioner must build relationships with the client
and the client's friends and family to dig deeply into the recesses
of spoken and recorded history to discover stories that depict the
client accurately throughout the court process. Strong relationships
must be built among defense team members and everyone involved in the
client's life. These relationships may place the client at greater
ease, improving his or her demeanor with resultant interactions
witnessed by jurors working to humanize the defendant. By developing the
conceptual problem model depicting the causal factors underlying the
ineffectiveness of mitigation evidence in capital trials, it is hoped
that social workers serving as mitigation specialists begin to link
traditional social work skills to these points of intervention. From the
initial meeting with the client until the last appeal is denied in a
capital case, social workers must use their skills to educate, advocate,
and build important relationships to ensure that the critical human
issues that propel a person into criminality are made very clear
throughout each phase of the trial. This may include the use of
pictures, graphics, compelling expert testimony, and life history
information that demands the undivided attention of jurors so they will
retain key facts and carefully consider the issues.
The problem conceptual model must eventually lead to a multilevel
intervention framework that can be formalized and tested using
multivariate analysis to determine the effectiveness of mitigation when
the issues identified in this article are actively addressed. Effective
interventions will create opportunities for classroom instruction, field
placement, and interdisciplinary collaboration among social work and law
students, as well as much needed postgraduate and continuing education for the social work mitigation specialists and attorneys who currently
fight for just decisions in capital trials.
Original manuscript received September 9, 2002 Final revision
received July 29, 2004 Accepted August 15, 2005
REFERENCES
Alfonso, C., & Baur, K. (1986). Enhancing capital defense: The
role of the forensic clinical social worker. The Champion, 6, 6-29.
Andrews, A. B. (1991). Social work expert testimony regarding
mitigation in capital sentencing proceedings. Social Work, 36, 440-445.
Bem, P. A. (1978). Social behavior: Fact and falsehoods about
common sense, hypnotism, obedience, altruism, beauty, racism and sexism.
Chicago: Nelson Hall.
Bowers, W. J., Sandys, M., & Steiner, B. D. (1998). Foreclosed
impartiality in capital sentencing: Jurors' predispositions,
guilt-trial experience and premature decision making. Cornell Lau,
Review, 83, 1476-1541.
Bowers, W. J., & Steiner, B. D. (1999). Death by default: An
empirical demonstration of false and forced choices in capital
sentencing. Texas Law Review, 77, 605-717.
Caldwell, H. M., Perrin, L. T., & Frost, C. L. (2002). The art
and architecture of closing argument. Tulane Law Review, 76, 961-1072.
Coates, D., & Penrod, S. (1981). Social psychology and the
emergence of disputes. Law and Society Review, 15, 665-659.
Cutler, B. L. (1990). Introduction: The status of scientific jury
selection in psychology and law. Forensic Reports, 3,227-232.
Diamond, S. S., & Levi, J. N. (1996). Improving decisions on
death by revising and testing jury instructions. Judicature, 79,
224-232.
Eisenberg, T., Garvey, S. P., & Wells, M. T. (1996). Jury
responsibility in capital sentencing: An empirical study. Buffalo Law
Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from,
http://www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/ death/cjp_publ.htm
Eisenberg, T., Garvey, S. P., & Wells, M. T. (1998). But was he
sorry? The role of remorse in capital sentencing. Cornell Law Review.
Retrieved April 20, 2001, from,
http://www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
Eisenberg, T., Garvey, S. P., & Wells, M. T. (2001).
Forecasting life, and death: Juror race, religion, and attitude toward
the death penalty. Journal of Legal Studies. Retrieved January 28, 2002,
from http:// www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
Eisenberg, T., & Wells, M. T. (1993). Deadly confusion: Juror
instructions in capital cases. Cornell Law Review. Retrieved April 20,
2001, from http:// www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
Fincham, F. D., & Jaspars, J. M. (1980). Attribution of
responsibility: From man the scientist to man as lawyer. Experimental
Social Psychology, 13, 81-82.
Fischoff, S. (2003). Stack and sway: The new science of jury
consulting. Political Psychology, 24, 628-631.
Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238 (1972).
Garvey, S. P. (1998). Aggravation and mitigation in capital cases:
What do jurors think? Columbia Law Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001,
from http://www. lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Garvey, S. P. (2000). The emotional economy of capital sentencing.
New York University Law Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http://www. lswschool.cornell.edu/library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Garvey, S. P., Johnson, S. L., & Marcus, P. (2000). Correcting
deadly confusion: Responding to jury inquiries in capital cases. Cornell
Law Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from http://www.
lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Guin, C. C., & Merrill, T. S. (2000, May). Life or death? Using
multi-disciplinary life history research in forensic social work. Paper
presented at the meeting of the National Organization of Forensic Social
Workers, Palm Springs, CA.
Guin, C. C., Noble, D., & Merrill, T. S. (2003). From misery to
mission: Forensic social workers on multidisciplinary mitigation teams.
Social Work, 48, 362-373.
Hoffman, J. L. (1995). Where's the buck? Juror misperception of sentencing responsibility in death penalty cases. Indiana Law
Journal. Retrieved April 20, 200l, from
http://www.lswschool.cornell.edu/ library/death/cjp/publ.htm
Jurors can't understand instructions, study finds. (1998,
November, 19). Simon Fraser News, 13(6). Retrieved November 20, 2002,
from http://www.sfu.ca/ mediapr/sfnews/1998/Nov 19/oglogg.html
Luginbuhl, J., & Howe, J. (1995). Discretion in capital
sentencing instructions: Guided or misguided? Indiana Law Journal.
Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http://www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
McCoy, D. (1999). Mitigation strategies in death penalty cases. The
Advocate, 21 (1). Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http:/dpa.state.ky.us/library/advocate/jan99/ mitigation, html
McDougall v. Dixon, 921 F.2nd 518, 539 (4th Cir.) (1990).
Ritter, J. L. (2004). Your lips are moving ... but the words
aren't clear: Dissecting the presumption that jurors understand
instructions. Missouri Law Review, 96, 163.
Roberts, A. R., & Brownell, P. (1999). A century of forensic
social work: Bridging the past to the present. Social Work, 44, 359-369.
Sandys, M. (1995). Cross-over-capital jurors who change their minds
about the punishment: A litmus test for sentencing guidelines. Indiana
Law Journal. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from http://www.lawschool.
cornell.edu/library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Schroeder, J. (2003). Social work's role in death penalty
mitigation investigations: Forging a new practice area. Families in
Society, 84, 423-432.
Sherman, S. J. (1995). The role of responsibility and how
psychology can inform the law. Symposium: The Capital Jury Project.
Indiana University Law Journal, 70, 1241. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http:// www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
Slobogin, C. (2003). What Atkins could mean for people with mental
illness. New Mexico Law Review, 33, 293-314.
Steiner, B. D. (1999). Folk knowledge as legal action: Death
penalty judgments and the tenet of early release in a culture of
mistrust and punitiveness. Law and Society Review. Retrieved April 20,
2001, from http://www.lawschool.cornell.edu/library/death/ cjp_publ.htm
Stetler, R. (1999). Why capital cases require mitigation
specialists. The Champion, 1, 35-40.
Sundby, E. (1998). The capital jury and absolution: The
intersection of trial strategy, remorse and the death penalty. Cornell
Law Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http://www.lawschool.cornell.edu/ library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Tiersma, P. M. (1995). Dictionaries and death: Do capital jurors
understand mitigation? Utah Law Review. Retrieved April 20, 2001, from
http://www. lawschool.utah.edu/library/death/cjp_publ.htm
Tomes, J. P. (1997). Damned if you do, damned if you don't:
The use of mitigation experts in death penalty litigation. American
Journal of Criminal Law, 24, 359-398.
Vore, D.A. (1989). Psychological consultation for systematic jury
selection. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 7(4), 21-36.
Werner, H. D. (1986). Cognitive theory. In F.J. Turner (Ed.),
Social work treatment (3rd ed., pp. 91-130). New York: Free Press.
Wiener, R. L., Rogers, M., Winter, R., Hurt, L., Hackney, A.,
Kadela, K., Seib, H., Rauch, S., Warren, L., & Morasco, B. (2004).
Guided jury discretion in capital murder cases: The role of declarative and procedural knowledge. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 10,
516-576.
Julie Schroeder, PhD, is associate professor, Department of
Criminal Justice, Fayetteville State University, 2500 Murchison Road,
Fayetteville, NC 28301; e-mail:
[email protected]. Cecile C. Guin,
PhD, is associate professor, School of Social Work, Louisiana State
University. Rene Pogue, PhD, is a visiting professor, Texas State
University at San Marcos. Danna Bordelon, MSW, is research assistant,
School of Social Work, Louisiana State University.