Coming to understanding: developing conservation through incremental learning in the Pacific Northwest.
Turner, Nancy J. ; Berkes, Fikret
Published online: 20 July 2006
[c] Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006
Abstract Lessons in conservation are often seen as resulting from
cycles of overexploitation and subsequent depletion of resources,
followed by catastrophic consequences of shortage and starvation, and
finally, development of various strategies, including privatization of
the commons, to conserve remaining resource stocks. While such scenarios
have undoubtedly occurred on many occasions, we suggest that they are
not the only means by which people develop conservation practices and
concepts. There are other pathways leading to ecological understanding
and conservation, which act at a range of scales and levels of
complexity. These include: lessons from the past and from other places,
perpetuated and strengthened through oral history and discourse; lessons
from animals, learned through observation of migration and population
cycles, predator effects, and social dynamics; monitoring resources and
human effects on resources (positive and negative), building on
experiences and expectations; observing changes in ecosystem cycles and
natural disturbance events; trial and error experimentation and
incremental modification of habitats and populations. Humans, we
believe, are capable of building a sophisticated conservation ethic that
transcends individual species and resources. A combination of
conservation knowledge, practices, and beliefs can lead to increasingly
greater sophistication of ecological understanding and the continued
encoding of such knowledge in social institutions and worldview.
Key words Traditional ecological knowledge * conservation *
indigenous peoples * ethnoecology.
Introduction
Resource conserving practices of indigenous and local peoples drawn
from their traditional knowledge systems have been described for many
parts of the world and for many different cultures and environments
(Blackburn and Anderson, 1993; Balee, 1994; Berkes, 1999; Berkes et al.,
2000; Minnis and Elisens, 2000; Turner et al., 2000; Alcorn et al.,
2003; Hunn et al., 2003). A wide variety of conservation strategies have
been documented, ranging from cultural teachings against harvesting
specific resources or harvesting at specific times or places, to
selective or limited harvesting, to sanctions against waste (Berkes,
1999). In fact, traditional ecological knowledge systems are infused
with practices and concepts, and modes of teaching and learning that can
be related directly and indirectly to resource stewardship and
conservation at various scales. However, despite considerable attention
directed towards documentation of these systems and approaches to
conservation, we still have a limited understanding about their
development, evolution, and transmission over time and space.
It is sometimes assumed that the development of community-based
conservation strategies, ethics, and teachings is a result of some
realization or recognition of a catastrophic resource depletion situation (e.g., Johannes, 1998, 2002). Many authors have questioned
whether such resource management systems can be considered to represent
"conservation" at all, and, by extension, whether traditional
resource managers can be effective conservationists. In part, the
argument goes, a "real" conservationist both acts to prevent
or mitigate resource depletion and has the intention to conserve (Smith
and Wishnie, 2000). We do not wish to enter here into the debate on
"conservation" and whether indigenous conservation is likely
or even possible (Hunn et al., 2003). However, we refute the hypothesis
that conservation is only authentic if it results from the intention to
conserve, as have Wilson et al. (1994) and some others. The conservation
biology or evolutionary ecology critique of indigenous conservation has
its own logic, based on the notion that evolutionary theory more easily
accounts for short-term and self-centered behaviors (Tucker, 2003). We
find the assumptions behind the evolutionary ecology critique to be too
limiting and western-centric; they are not supported by the realities of
the indigenous groups that we deal with in this paper. However, we
certainly make no claim for a universal conservationist bent in all
indigenous or traditional cultures! The ethnohistorical and
archaeological record provides evidence of situations in which people
failed to conserve their resources, sometimes with dire consequences
(Diamond, 1997; Krech, 1999; Redman, 1999).
Our focus is on learning and knowledge accumulation. Despite the
undeniable existence of human-caused resource depletion, it seems
unlikely that conservation arising from direct experiences with
depletion and the resulting crisis is the only way that humans have
learned to manage and conserve their resources. Indeed, the
pervasiveness of conservation strategies, philosophies, and teachings
that result in regulated resource use, or outright care for and
conservation of non-resource species, indicates that people are capable
of developing and enacting de facto conservation through other means. As
Heiltsuk cultural specialist Pauline Waterfall noted (personal
communication to NT, May 2004), "We had a form of regulated use
based on the understanding that conservation would result if we
regulated our use in a mindful and respectful way." For purposes of
discussion, we have developed two models for learning conservation: the
depletion crisis model and the ecological understanding model. In this
paper, we explore the latter: what are the mechanisms and circumstances,
beyond direct response to resource depletion, by which people can come
to an understanding of the need to care for their resources and develop
ways of promoting conservation?
Based mainly (but not exclusively) on the indigenous peoples of the
North American Pacific Northwest, and using a general schematic for
describing the diverse components of traditional ecological knowledge
(Turner et al., 2000), we consider the development of conservation
techniques and prescriptions based on the various components of
traditional ecological knowledge systems. We then identify some possible
mechanisms for building ecological understanding. We use the Saanich
Reefnet fishery as an example of a complex conservation and resource
management strategy, combining various elements of traditional
ecological knowledge. We conclude by discussing the importance of
philosophy and worldview in mediating and directing conservation
activities. Given that conventional "western" efforts have
generally not been successful in meeting conservation goals and
objectives, it is crucial to consider possibilities for integration of
indigenous and western perspectives in developing strategies for
conservation.
We define traditional ecological knowledge as "a cumulative
body of knowledge, practice and belief, evolving by adaptive processes
and handed down through generations by cultural transmission"
(Berkes, 1999, p. 8). In keeping with the teachings of the indigenous
peoples of the Pacific Northwest, we define conservation here as keeping
something, especially an important environmental or cultural resource,
or an entire habitat, from harm, loss, or change, using a resource
sparingly so as not to exhaust supplies, and/or using specific measures
to maintain and enhance a resource, a suite of resources or entire
habitats (for example, as in landscape burning; Boyd, 1999; Peacock and
Turner, 2000). Note that conservation in indigenous thought and practice
does not preclude use, as it does in some western conservation
traditions (Berkes, 1999). Resource depletion refers to a reduction in
the abundance or productivity of plants, animals or substances used or
required by humans.
Traditional Ecological Knowledge Systems and Learning of
Conservation
In the framework presented here, ecological understanding is the
term we use to refer to a suite of attributes embodied within
traditional ecological knowledge systems, including:
* Incremental learning of individuals and groups and elaboration of
environmental knowledge as a result of detailed observation and
experience of variations in nature and leading to a sophisticated
understanding of the ecosystem in which they dwell;
* Development of concomitant belief systems that help avert serious
resource depletion and promote conserving approaches;
* Creating and perpetuating ways of encoding, communicating and
disseminating both the practical aspects of such incremental learning
and adaptive response and the ideologies and belief systems associated
it; and
* Development of institutions that consolidate environmental
knowledge and practice, or development of rules by which members of a
society deal with their environment and resources.
Incremental Learning from Observation and Experience
We contend that humans living in close proximity to their
environments are capable of observing, identifying, monitoring, and
reacting to variations in resource availability, ecological
relationships, and biological responses to particular circumstances.
Such knowledge can be acquired in the same ways as other important
knowledge for survival, such as of food and medicine. Learning about new
foods and medicines, and how to prepare and process them safely and
effectively, for example, has been in large part incremental and
cumulative (Johns, 1996). There would have been calamitous experiences
and tragedies along the way, but more often, judicious tasting,
sampling, experimentation, and evaluation would have guided the learning
experience. Just feeling sick from eating a small amount of a plant, or
detecting a bitter taste or some temporary hallucinogenic effect, would
have been sufficiently remarkable to engender further experimentation or
trial or to ward off more intensive use. This type of experience, Johns
maintains, is the very mechanism whereby people learned to differentiate
between food and medicine. Furthermore, observations leading to
ecological understanding can be positive, just as tasting and learning
about certain foods can be a positive experience (imagine a first taste
of wild strawberry for someone testing for new foods!). For example, a
natural burn attracting browsing deer and increased berry production in
subsequent years would provide an incentive for clearing and development
of anthropogenic burning (e.g., Boyd, 1999). Pauline Waterfall, Heiltsuk
cultural specialist and teacher, writes (personal communication to NT,
May 2004), "My grandfather taught me that observing how animals
behaved and used certain natural resources was normal process of basis
for experimenting, modifying, learning. For example, he told me that he
came across many trees that had old scars with dried sap mixed with many
varieties of animal fur. Upon observing, he discovered that animals came
along and rubbed themselves against the sap and that these animals had
injuries."
Plant resource management and conservation practices that could
have developed incrementally include burning and clearing, pruning,
coppicing, tilling, replanting and transplanting, partial harvesting of
individual trees and shrubs, selective harvesting for size and life
cycle stage, and rotational harvesting through annual or multiyear
cycles, as well as genetic selection for maximum productivity or other
desirable traits (Williams and Baines, 1988; Blackburn and Anderson,
1993; Minnis and Elisens, 2000; Deur and Turner, 2005).
For animal populations, including shellfish, fish, terrestrial and
marine mammals, birds and bird eggs, and other resources check, there
are parallel strategies and specific practices applied to maintaining
numbers and representative age categories. Many examples exist of
conservation practices including harvest selection by age, sex, size,
and reproductive stage and season for various species (Moller et al.,
2004), as well as the preparing and maintaining of productive habitats
and foods for certain key resource species through the use of fire and
other means (Brookfield and Paddoch, 1994; Colfer, 1992; Young et al.,
1991).
Pauline Waterfall (personal communication to NT May 2004) described
another conservation practice used by Heiltsuk hunters, namely to rotate
areas where one hunted. "For example," she said, "If lots
of deer were caught at a specific area, that area was left alone for a
couple of years and other areas were used." Such rotation of
harvesting areas indicates that hunters have an understanding of natural
renewal cycles and the length of time that a population would need to
replenish itself. Another example is from Daisy Sewid-Smith (personal
communication to NT, October 1994), when asked about responses to
scarcity:
It was not the idea of harvesting too much ... sometimes something
would happen where a certain plant won't grow as much as it did last
year. And they [her elders] said it's a cycle that happens--fish will
disappear and there won't be very much fish--and yes, they did have
people monitoring this. And, when they could see that there was going
to be scarcity, then they were not allowed to go to that particular
area. They were told to go to another area, and let that area build
up. And yes, they did have people within the clan or tribal groups
that monitored these changes ...
There is evidence that some indigenous groups can manage ecological
cycles at multiple scales. For example, Cree hunters of James Bay seem
to be managing simultaneously beaver populations on a 4- to 6-year
cycle, fish on a 5- to 10-year cycle and caribou on a 80- to 100-year
cycle (Berkes et al., 2000). Such management systems can work well only
if there are systems of proprietorship in place for these resources;
otherwise one group's long-term conservation and management
practices could be thwarted by another group's unregulated use (cf.
Turner et al., 2005).
Belief Systems that Promote Conservation
Eugene Anderson (1996) suggests that the complex belief systems
that comprise human religions and that are reflected in our social
institutions, narratives, ceremonies and day-to-day activities are
engendered from an ethic of conservation and responsibility to the
environment. The question is: how do such complex belief systems
develop, particularly in relation to conservation of other lifeforms? We
know that attitudes are socially mediated and directed, and that
attitudes guide and determine our actions. The notion of
"respect" predominates in many traditional belief systems
(Callicott, 1994; Turner and Atleo, 1998). As Chief Umeek (Richard
Atleo) articulates for the Nuu-Chah-Nulth (Scientific Panel for
Sustainable Forest Practices in Clayoquot Sound, 1995, p. 15):
The Creator made all things one.
All things are related and interconnected.
All things are sacred.
All things are therefore to be respected.
Pauline Waterfall reiterated the same perspective for the Heiltsuk
(personal communication to NT May 2004): "All living things have a
spirit life. We acknowledged and still do that every life is worthy of
being respected."
Related sentiment expressed in indigenous societies is appreciation
and thanks. As Tewa elder Vickie Downie explains (Wall, 1993):
You give thanks before you even receive your gifts from the
Creator. When you ask, you give thanks. Your prayers are
thanksgivings for everything--the sun, the moon, the snow, the
water, the fire, the rocks. You see them as being alive, having a
life of their own. A tree has its own life ...
Another key concept in indigenous belief systems, arising from the
first two, is that waste is deplorable: "Take only what you need;
never waste anything!" is an instruction heard over and over again
in traditional teachings (Turner and Atleo, 1998; Turner et al., 2000);
avoiding waste might not always ensure conservation, but it would
certainly help remind people of the limits they must impose on their
actions. However, there may be a gap between the ideal and the actual
practice. Some authors have also argued that the belief in some
indigenous cultures in the unlimited renewability of resources could
lead to overexploitation. For example, Brightman (1993 p. 280) uses
ethnohistorial evidence that the northern Manitoba Cree conceived
animals such as caribou as "infinitely renewable resources whose
numbers could neither be reduced by overkilling nor managed by selective
hunting." But such potential overexploitation would be reined in by
the application of rules of respect.
To what extent do attitudes of "respect,"
"appreciation" and "taking only what you need"
within a belief system promote conservation without the experience of
serious resource depletion? It may well be that resource depletion--or a
series of resource depletions--somewhere at some time in the past did
prompt the development of such belief systems (Berkes, 1999; see also,
Berkes and Turner, this volume). However, on a broad scale, and over a
long time frame, a belief system, in turn, helps prevent overharvesting
or wanton destruction of other lifeforms, whether conservation per se is
the intended result or not (Minnis and Elisens, 2000).
One of the major social mechanisms by which societies remember and
build upon traditions of resource conservation is the use of stories and
myths. Many of the traditional narratives of First Nations that reflect
lessons of respect, appreciation, and conservation, describe a time of
profound lack of resources, when people, sometimes with animal personas,
were deprived of even the basic requirements for life: sun, moon, tides,
winds, water, fire, and proper food. The stories do not necessarily
depict human negligence or overharvesting as the cause of this
deprivation. Consider, for example the Saanich story of the Origin of
Salmon:
Once there were no seals and the people were starving; they lived
on elk and whatever other game they could kill. Two brave youths
said to each other, "Let us go and see if we can find any salmon."
They embarked in their canoe and headed out to sea .... They
journeyed for three and a half months. Then they came to a strange
country. When they reached the shore a man came out and welcomed
them ....
The youths stayed in the place about a month. Their hosts then said
to them, "You must go home tomorrow. Everything is arranged for
you. The salmon that you were looking for will muster at your home
and start off on their journey. You must follow them." So the two
youths followed the salmon; for three and a half months they
travelled, day and night, with the fish. Every night they took
qexmin (Indian celery, Lomatium nudicaule (Pursh) Coult. & Rose)
and burned it that the salmon might feed on its smoke and sustain
themselves. Finally they reached Discovery Island (Ktces), where
they burned qexmin all along the beach; for their hosts had said to
them, "Burn qexmin along the beach when you reach land, to feed the
salmon that travel with you. Then, if you treat the salmon well,
you will always have them in abundance."
... Because their journey took them three and a half months, salmon
are now absent on the coast for that period. The coho said to the
other salmon, "You can go ahead of us [on the ocean journey], for
we have not yet got what we wanted from the lakes." That is why the
coho is always the last of the salmon.
The story continues, relating how the Salmon people taught the
young men how and when to make and use reef nets and how to honor the
first salmon with a ceremony and prayer "... that [the salmon] may
always be plentiful" (Jenness, 1930).
There are many other stories, such as the Ditidaht story of the
origin of the winds, tides, seasons, fire, fish, sun, moon, and daylight
(Touchie, 1977), in which the resources that people rely on were
provided as gifts from the Creator, or from the powers and generosity of
individuals like Raven and other supernatural beings and cultural
heroes. Some convey imperatives for conservation directly, but most
impart a general sense of the need for appreciating and not wasting
these valuable gifts. The end result, however, would be a conserving
philosophy.
Perceived kinship with other lifeforms, even trees and other
plants, leads to a different way of treating these beings (Salmon, 2000;
Turner, 2005). For example, Northwest Coast people had the technology
and perhaps even a practical reason to kill cedar trees when they were
removing the bark for clothing, mats or baskets. Yet everywhere
teachings directed harvesters to be careful and only remove one or two
straps from each tree, so as to keep the tree alive (Deur and Turner,
2005). Franz Boas (1921, pp. 616-617) explains this practice for the
Kwakwaka'wakw:
Even when the young cedar-tree is quite smooth, they do not take
all of the cedar-bark, for the people of the olden times said that
if they should peel off all the cedar-bark ... the young cedar
would die, and then another cedar-tree near by would curse the
bark-peeler so that he would also die. Therefore, the bark-peelers
never take all of the bark off a young tree.
The ultimate motivation for this practice was evidently to avoid
needless killing of another lifeform, rather than conservation per se in
the western sense. Pauline Waterfall (personal communication to NT May
2004) explained: "Needless killing is a teaching that is passed
along to impress upon us that if we don't respect another lifeform,
it won't return or will die out, thereby depriving us of future
access and use. This is an explicit teaching of conservation." In
any case, the practice of avoiding needless loss of life reflects a
widespread belief system, at once respectful and utilitarian, that would
engender conservation. Gitga'at elder Helen Clifton (personal
communication to NT 2002) said that they were always taught as children
that all creatures--mice, wolves, birds--had their own families and
their own lives, parallel to and as important as those of people.
Children were warned not to harm or needlessly disturb them. Mountains,
too, were regarded as living beings, with their own stories and their
own families, and requiring of respect. For example, there are certain
mountains that one should never point to, or they will cause hard luck
at some time in the future, especially bad weather (Roger William,
Tsilhqot'in, personal communication to NT 2003; Elsie Claxton,
Saanich, personal communication to NT 1998). Children were taught not to
pick certain flowers or it might cause rain, or fog, or lightning storms
(Turner et al., 1983; Turner, 2004).
There is a well-known narrative theme, recurring in several
language communities on the North Coast of British Columbia and coastal
Alaska (Haida, Haisla, Tsimshian, Tlingit), about the consequences of
harming frogs. In the Haida version, centering in the village of
Cumshewa, young boys on a fishing outing were playing with a frog, and
threw it into the fire. The frog's mother, a supernatural being
named Jilaa quns, or Creek Woman, was so distraught by this cruelty that
she predicted the death of the boys, one after another, and finally, the
destruction of their entire village (Turner, 2005). How closely is such
a story based on an actual event, and, if it is, where and when did the
event take place? It would have been many hundreds or thousands of ago,
yet it still serves to teach one generation after the next about the
dangers of willfully harming other species--in this case, a species that
is not eaten by humans. The Nisga'a stories of the lava flows that
destroyed several villages, said to be caused by the cruel and
disrespectful treatment of salmon by a boy in one of the villages, is
similar in the power of its lessons, perhaps even more so because of the
obvious presence of the lava beds in the area (BC Parks, 2000). This
story, and the lessons it embodies, seems to have its origins not in
resource depletion but in disrespectful behavior--in lack of an
environmental ethic.
Communication and Dissemination of Conservation Actions and Ethics
Stories and teachings are one example of the ways in which
understandings of conservation and environmental ethics can be
disseminated over time and space. Just as in learning about edible,
poisonous, and medicinal plants without continuous episodes of fatality,
it is not necessary for lessons in conservation to be learned by major
catastrophic episodes of resource depletion. Rather, observations and
experiences and guiding principles can be taught and acquired over
generations, and spread through stories, ceremonies, and discourse from
one community to another (Turner et al. 2000). Children are often taught
by parents and elders about their responsibilities to their family,
clan, and the other lifeforms. Community gatherings, such as potlatches
and feasts, are occasions for reinforcing these values (George, 2003).
Social relationships, such as the roles and responsibilities of the
hereditary chiefs and leaders in relation to their people, resources and
territories, would also be reiterated at such times (Turner et al.,
2005). Individuals and groups within a community would hold specialized
knowledge to be imparted at appropriate times and circumstances (Turner,
2003). Some knowledge, especially relating to specific places, might be
held privately, as part of an individual's or family's
proprietory rights. Individual words and phrases, as well as stories,
and lessons conveyed in art, music, and ceremony, are all part of this
knowledge system leading to dissemination of environmental
understanding. Children, too, participate on a daily basis in activities
that foster conservation.
Secwepemc elder, Dr. Mary Thomas (personal communication to NT
2002), for example, learned lessons about conservation from her two
grandmothers. As a child, she watched and helped while her grandmother
carefully moved the carcasses of the salmon after they had spawned, from
the banks of the Salmon River back into the water. Her grandmother
explained that the salmon would nourish the baby fish to come. She also
observed her grandmother remove and replant the smaller glacier lily
(Erythronium grandiflorum Pursh) and chocolate lily (Fritillaria
lanceolata Pursh) bulbs that Mary and her siblings had selected from the
overturned turf and put into baskets when they were out harvesting roots
with her. Her grandmother told them that they should only take the
biggest roots and leave the small ones to grow for the future. In these
demonstrations, and in Mary's participation, her grandmother was
reinforcing the values of respect for other lifeforms and the practices
required to sustain resources, imparting lessons that have endured over
the many decades of Mary's life.
Creating and perpetuating ways of encoding, communicating and
disseminating both the practical aspects of such incremental learning
and adaptive responses and the ideologies and belief systems associated
with them is as important today as in the past. In many cases, the
opportunities for children to spend time with and learn from parents,
grandparents, and others knowledgeable about conservation practices and
beliefs, have been diminished, as have their opportunities for direct
interaction with habitats and resources. This is a serious issue that
needs to be addressed if traditional knowledge is not to be lost. This
kind of knowledge cannot be mastered from books.
Institutions for Conservation Practice and Behavior
We have argued that environmental knowledge may be elaborated
incrementally, leading to the development of belief systems and ways of
transmitting this knowledge, both temporally (to other generations) and
geographically (to other groups; cf. Turner et al., 2003). Another
component of developing and consolidating conservation is the emergence
of institutions that foster the perpetuation of values and knowledge. By
institutions, we mean the set of rules actually used or rules-in-use
(Ostrom, 1990). Such institutions are socially constructed, with
normative and cognitive dimensions, thus they embed values (Jentoft et
al., 1998).
Rule sets that define access rights and specify appropriate
behaviors are often known as tenure systems. There is a well-developed
literature on land and marine tenure systems in the Pacific Northwest
(e.g., Williams and Hunn, 1982; Turner et al., 2005) and we need not
repeat them here. It is useful, however, to investigate some apparently
simple plant use systems to illustrate how such institutions work. The
harvesting of devil's club (Oplopanax horridum (Sm.) Torr. and A.
Gray ex. Miq.) for medicine is a case in point (Lantz, 2001; Lantz and
Antos, 2002). In general, only those who are considered medicine
specialists are sanctioned to harvest and administer devil's club
medicine. Even then, there are protocols to follow. It is usual to seek
devil's club for harvesting in a remote place, removed from human
settlement or intrusion. A harvester is taught to be conserving in terms
of the way the medicine is harvested and in the quantities taken.
Generally, only the branches are taken, not the main "mother"
stalk (Captain Gold, Haida, personal communication to NT, 1996, among
others). A practice of the Gitga'at (Tsimshian) is for the
harvester to take no more than four stalks at any one time (Helen
Clifton, Gitga'at, personal communication to NT, 2004). The
harvester offers a prayer or words of thanks to the plant, and generally
leaves a small gift of tobacco or a coin in the place where the plant
was harvested. Some harvesters make a practice of replanting stem
sections in the ground whenever they remove part of the plant (Arvid
Charlie, personal communication to NT and T. Lantz 2000; Lantz et al.,
2004). The "rules" around devil's club harvesting are
embedded in a belief system in which devil's club is a living being
with a spirit and the capacity to help or harm humans, depending upon
how it is approached (Turner, 2004).
Another example is the harvesting and use of highbush cranberries
(Viburnum edule (Michx.) Raf.). These berries are considered a valuable
food, but the bushes can be quite sporadic. In many places, highbush
cranberry patches are specifically owned. The owner, usually a chief or
matriarch, may delegate others to pick the berries or may organize and
lead a picking expedition of community members, but is always given a
portion of the harvest. Then, in turn, the owner of the patch is
expected to host a feast at which these berries are served. The patches
are carefully monitored, both to determine when the berries are ripe and
to ensure that outsiders do not encroach on the harvest. People have
been known to transplant highbush cranberries to closer localities, and
often these and other berry patches are cared for like orchards (Turner,
2005; Turner and Peacock, 2005; Turner et al., 2005). It is doubtful
that such management developed as a result of catastrophic depletion.
Just as there are rules about the appropriate ways to harvest
resources, there are also rules about how people make new environmental
observations and how these observations eventually become part of the
accepted knowledge of that group. Working with the Anishinaabe (Ojibwa)
people of northwestern Ontario, Davidson-Hunt and Berkes (2003) argue
that such learning requires maintaining a web of relationships of people
and places. People literally learn as they travel over the landscape.
This knowledge is remembered through social memory (McIntosh, 2000, p.
24) for long-term communal understanding of environmental change and the
transmission of pertinent experience. Social memory describes how an
individual thought or observation emerging out of a specific experience
can become a part of the collective knowledge of a group. But how does
individual creativity, emerging in response to a change in the
environment, lead to change in social memory?
The rules that govern the evolution of knowledge in a particular
group may be called "institutions of knowledge," defined as
framing the process of creativity, learning, and remembering
(Davidson-Hunt and Berkes, 2003). Institutions of knowledge, in the
Anishinaabe case, comprise rules and values about how the process of
learning can occur, the culturally correct way in which knowledge can be
transmitted, how individual competency develops, and how observations of
a specific experience may become part of the accepted, authoritative
knowledge of the group.
Learning is a life-long process and legitimate knowledge requires
establishing competency over a period of time. Not all observations are
equally relevant or equally important. There is a filtering process of
learning, within the bounds or frame set by the socially accepted rules
that govern the establishment of accepted knowledge. Elders play a key
role in this process. Among the Anishinaabe, elder is a social role and
designation. Not all old people are elders; conversely some middle-aged
people who have developed their competencies relatively quickly may be
considered as elders. Institutions of knowledge allow authoritative and
legitimate knowledge to be built through experience; this is by and
large an incremental process. Understandings of the environment,
embedded firmly within belief systems and informing resource use
practices, can enable people to live within the constraints of their
environment without the necessity of a major resource depletion crisis
to force catastrophic learning.
Mechanisms for Building Ecological Understanding
The ability or capacity to learn from small and incremental lessons
and from the experiences of others potentially enables people to develop
sustainable practices and ecological understandings without always
having to respond to and learn from crisis situations. Not only an event
itself, but any inferences, extrapolations, or interpretations people
draw from it can be enfolded into an enriched, elaborated system of
knowledge and practice. Over time, even within one lifetime, experiences
of others blend with personal knowledge and observations, compounding
and accumulating to bring enhanced knowledge and wisdom. Table I
summarizes some of the pathways by which lessons in land and resource
management and knowledge of the importance of conservation may have been
accrued, using examples from northwestern North American Indigenous
peoples.
In reality, the knowledge acquisition and learning leading to an
environmental ethic, and ultimately conservation of resources and
biological diversity, is highly complex; it is impossible to trace and
identify the countless diverse, tangled, and interwoven threads that
comprise traditional knowledge systems and their origin and development.
Under these circumstances, it is difficult, if not impossible, to
identify intention to conserve as separate from a belief system that
values and recognizes as kin all lifeforms, from frogs to wolves to
cedar trees. Nevertheless, this system, within its cultural contexts,
seems to have worked well for people in maintaining their resources over
a long period of time. Resource depletion, natural and human-mediated,
is a part of the story, but not the whole story.
Developing Conservation: The Reefnet Fishery Example
In reality, people probably combine the lessons and understandings
gained from all of the pathways, including experiences of resource
depletion, to build up their knowledge, practices, and beliefs into
complex systems of land and resource management. These systems ingrain social structures, land and marine tenure systems, and opportunities for
adaptation at different scales. For instance, as described by Daisy
Sewid-Smith and Chief Adam Dick (personal communication to NT 1997), one
time, shortly before Captain George Vancouver arrived on the Northwest
Coast (1792), the salmon returning to spawn to the Nimpkish River on
northeastern Vancouver Island were very scarce. The Chiefs of the
villages situated along the river conversed and decided to place a
moratorium on salmon fishing. They moved all the people out to the mouth
of the river or to other villages, and they all lived on shellfish and
other food until the salmon became more plentiful. (The salmon were
described as having their own societies and practices that paralleled
those of humans, and it was said that they had gone to attend a wedding
of the daughter of the Chief of the Underwater World.) This story
reflects some of the complexities around conservation, including social
institutions and leadership, resource tenure, belief systems, and
adaptive capacity of communities. Although resource depletion was
experienced, there were already institutional mechanisms in place to
respond to the situation. There are many other treatments of Aboriginal
fishing systems and practices relating to fisheries conservation (cf.
Swezey and Heizer, 1977; M'Gonigle et al., 1999).
The Saanich reefnet fishery (Claxton and Elliott, 1994; Earl
Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2004) serves as a detailed
example of how narratives, social rules, ecological knowledge,
conservation practices, and technology come together. Reefnet (SXOLE)
salmon fishing technology is ancient, and the Straits Salish peoples
around Haro and Rosario Straits of southern Vancouver Island, the Gulf
Islands, and the American San Juan Islands are well-known for this
unique innovation. There are only certain localities where the reefnet
can be used, places where the schools of migrating salmon are channeled
into a restricted passage, for example between an island and an adjacent
reef. These locations fall under the hereditary ownership of individuals
and cannot be used by anyone else without permission (Fig. 1).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Reefnet fishing usually lasts around a month, starting when the
oceanspray (Holodiscus discolor (Pursh) Maxim.) starts to bloom and the
first berries start to ripen in late June. The reefnet is made of
willowbark (Salix lasiandra Benth. and other Salix spp.) and other plant
materials and is suspended into the water between two canoes, with a
leading edge, or "floor," anchored in place and extending
towards the direction from where the salmon are coming (Fig. 2).
Sometimes a channel is cleared in an offshore kelp bed leading to the
reefnet to help direct the salmon. The leading edge is positioned so
that it forms a false bottom sloping downwards to give the approaching
salmon the impression that they are swimming into deeper water. Strands
of American dunegrass (Elymus mollis Trin.) tied onto the
"floor" help in this deception, making the approach resemble
the oceanbottom (Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2000).
The crews in the two canoes wait until there is evidence that the salmon
are approaching--either sighting them underwater, or seeing the
advancing fish jumping out of the water. Sometimes, if visibility is
poor, they induce the fish to jump by raising some of the floor lines to
make the approach more uneven; the lead salmon will jump out of the
water to see what the conditions are like (Earl Claxton Sr., personal
communication to NT 2000). The fishers know just how long it will take
the salmon to swim into the main part of the net between the canoes.
Then, when enough fish are in position, the crew brings the vessels
together, pulling up the net at the same time and capturing the fish in
the canoes. For conservation purposes, a sizable willow hoop is
constructed at head of the net. This is to ensure that a few
fish--regarded as all members of the same family--will invariably escape
and be able to continue on to their home river to spawn. Earl Claxton
Sr. (personal communication to NT 2004) explained that the salmon, like
humans, travel in family groups, and it is very important, just as for
human families, that some members of the family be allowed to carry on
their lineage. Any of the salmon can be caught in the reefnet device,
but sockeye and springs are the two most generally sought. The first
salmon of the season to be caught is thanked and celebrated through a
special ritual. The salmon caught after this are divided up in twos
amongst the participating fishers; any odd fish left is replaced in the
ocean.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Learning how to use the reefnet technology, and regulating its use
effectively, required tremendous attention and practice for all those
participating and special administrative skills on the part of the
owner. Ironically, the Canadian government banned the use of reefnets in
the mid 1900s, and only a few members of the Saanich Nation who had an
opportunity to fish across the border in the San Juan Islands were able
to retain the knowledge and practice of this sustainable fishing method
(Claxton and Elliott, 1994; Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to
NT 2004).
The reefnet system, said to be taught to the Saanich people by the
Salmon people themselves and by the Creator, XALS, at the same time when
the Salmon first offered themselves to the Saanich (as in the story told
earlier), represents technological and social knowledge and skills,
conserving and respectful attitudes embodied in traditional Saanich
philosophies, and mechanisms for perpetuating these practices,
technologies, and beliefs from generation to generation (Table II).
Conclusions
The resource harvesting and management systems discussed here,
embodying belief systems, narratives, ceremonies, specialized
vocabulary, and other means of communicating and acquiring knowledge,
institutional structures for regulating resource use, and multifaceted
arrangements such as the Saanich reefnet fishery, are too complex and
culturally ingrained to have been developed solely in response to
experiences of catastrophic resource depletion. Major depletion events
may have at some point triggered some of these mechanisms, but they
cannot explain or account for the whole range of cultural constructions
that lead to sustainable resource use and conservation.
The circumstances for the development of the Saanich reefnet
fishery are lost in the mists of time, but the Saanich maintain that
they have always been careful not to deplete their stocks, and declare
that it is only in the last century, since their reefnets were banned in
Canada, that the stocks of sockeye, coho, and other salmon have
declined, in some cases to the point of extinction.
The power and potential of such holistic traditional systems that
combine harvesting with resource management and conservation is
undeniable. It seems that modern industrial society has not been able to
match the success of traditional conservation practices, whatever their
origins, even with the knowledge of ongoing resource depletion (Pauly et
al., 1998). We need something more to help us conserve effectively.
Adaptive comanagement, incorporating elements of the complex knowledge
system from which the Saanich reefnet was generated, may enable all of
us to live more sustainably. Our worldviews and attitudes are critical
components of conservation, and may be more important than any other
factor in conserving ecological integrity.
Table I Potential Pathways for Accruing Land and Resource Management and
Conservation Lessons with Examples from Indigenous Societies of
Northwestern North America
Mechanisms Explanation References
Lessons from the past Stories of positive George, 2003; Teit,
and negative 1912; BC Parks,
experiences, 2000
remembered by
individuals,
recounted within
families and
communities, or
embedded in art,
place names and
ceremonies
Language Terms that embody Pauline Waterfall,
conservation personal
concepts, communication May
understandings and 2004; Earl Maquinna
teachings, e.g., the George, personal
Heiltsuk word communication to NT
mnaqels, which refers 1998
to "selectively
collecting things
outside," and
miaisila, which
refers to someone
whose responsibility
it is to be a
guardian of certain
fish-bearing rivers,
or the Nuu-chah-nulth
word 7uh-mowashitl,
"to keep some and not
take all"
Metaphorical sayings Symbolic and Swanton, 1905;
and narratives metaphorical stories Teit, 1912
also teach lessons
about conservation
(e.g., Nlaka'pamux
story of Old One and
the Creation of the
Earth; Haida Jila
quuns story)
Lessons from other Technologies, Turner and Loewen,
places products, names, and 1998; Turner et
ideas relating to al., 2003
conservation and
environmental
stewardship (e.g.,
use of fire for
clearing; digging and
propagation
techniques; first
foods ceremonies)
passed from one
community to the next
through
intermarriage,
potlatches, trade
Learning from animals Observations of Loewen, 1998; Hunn
animal foraging et al., 2003;
strategies, Blackfoot Gallery
populations, browsing Committee, 2001;
and predation, Deur and Turner,
behaviors that might 2005
engender
understandings of
kinship and
reciprocity (e.g.,
grizzlies foraging
for edible roots;
birds' egg-laying
habits; pack and
leadership
relationships in
wolves; bears
"pruning" berry
bushes)
Monitoring--building on Routine observation Lantz and Turner,
experiences and of seasonal changes, 2003; Davidson-Hunt
expectations animal migrations, and Berkes, 2003
plant life cycles,
and berry production
brings recognition of
expected patterns and
ability to detect
variation from the
norm
Observing ecosystem Relative abundance Boyd, 1999; George,
cycles and disturbance and productivity of 2003; Thornton,
events plants and animals in 1999
particular
circumstances, both
temporal and spatial,
can guide peoples'
land and resource
management strategies
(e.g., successional
stages following
fire; effects of
flooding on salmon
migration patterns;
relation between
moisture and berry
productivity)
Trial and error Observing the Garrick, 1998;
experimentation and results--positive and Boyd, 1999
incremental negative, intentional
modification or incidental, short-
and long-term--of
people's own
activities, such as
selective harvesting
(e.g., harvesting
cedar bark and
planks), or of
emulating natural
disturbance (e.g.,
use of fire to clear
patches)
Learning by If a practice works Turner and Loewen,
association, extension, in one place at one 1998
and extrapolation time, it might work
in another place at
another time;
conversely, if a
practice or activity
results in negative
consequences in one
circumstance, it
might be avoided at
another time or place
(e.g., knowledge
about harvesting or
conserving one type
of shellfish, berry
or root might be
extended to other,
similar types)
Elaborating and Combining the lessons Cyril Carpenter and
building sophistication and understandings Pauline P.
gained from all of Waterfall, personal
these pathways, and communication 2002;
building up Turner, 2005; Deur
knowledge, practices and Turner, 2005
and beliefs into
complex systems of
land and resource
management (e.g.,
Heiltsuk berry
gardens; Saanich
reefnet fishery)
Table II The Saanich Reefnet Fishery as a Complex Conservation and
Resource Management Strategy [Based on Claxton and Elliott, 1994; and
Interview with Earl Claxton Sr. (personal communication to NT and
Nicholas Claxton, September, 2004)]
Practical techniques Inferred knowledge and
understanding
Creation of a false ocean bottom Understanding salmon schooling
leading to reefnet, held in habits and habitat
place by anchor stones preferences; technologies for
constructing, placing and
anchoring lines
Cutting swathes through kelp Identifying key salmon habitat
beds to create channels for the and preferences
fish
Paired canoes positioned for Understanding of western
pulling in the reefnet; moved by red-cedar (Thuja plicata Donn
tides and currents ex D. Don), woodworking and
canoe making technologies;
tides, weather, currents,
seasonal changes
Construction of the nets from Understanding of willow
the inner bark of Pacific willow seasonality, prime habitat,
(Salix lasiandra Benth.) and coppicing, regeneration,
other willows harvesting and net making
techniques
Distinguishing different Pacific Each kind of salmon has its
salmon species [e.g., coho own season, habits, fat
(Oncorhynchus kisutch), Chinook content, etc.; important for
(O. tshawytscha, also known as timing of fishing and
spring) and sockeye (O. nerka)] processing methods; provides
diversity and flexibility in
fishery
Use of ecological indicators, Familiarity of salmon
like time of blooming of lifecycles in context of
oceanspray (Holodiscus discolor overall ecosystem cycles and
(Pursh) Maxim.), berries weather patterns (cf. Lantz
ripening and summer and Turner, 2003);
thunderstorms, to judge the understanding of the dynamics
timing of the fishing activity of salmon runs
Belief system Result in terms of
Conservation
Salmon and other resources, as Constraints against waste;
well as techniques for take only as many as needed,
harvesting and processing them, and as many as can be properly
as gifts of the Creator, XALS processed
Salmon viewed as members of Need to always allow
families and lineages, akin to escapement of a portion of the
human families catch (a built in "escape"
hole at the end of the net),
to perpetuate individual
Salmon families
Humans as responsible to the Respect and careful use
Salmon and other resources
Attachment to territory and Attention to specific places
place, as in traditional tenure and changes over many
systems generations
Communication and Dissemination Perpetuating Knowledge and
of Practice and Belief Beliefs
Original absence of salmon Primary resource depletion,
followed by the "gift" of
salmon
Ritual and spiritual recognition First Salmon ceremony (cf.
of the gift of salmon Swezey and Heizer 1977)
Recognition of different kinds Use of names; taxonomies for
of salmon distinguishing different
species and stocks
All the reefnet locations have Allows proprietory recognition
names and monitoring of specific
sites; enhances communication
Stories, narratives, language Reinforcing and communicating
and names about the reefnet knowledge and beliefs
fishery and the salmon
Institutions: the tenure system Rules that govern access and
fisher behavior
Social organization of fishers, Confirming, teaching and
families and leaders enforcing the cultural
constraints against waste and
disrespect
Fishing areas (and other Proprietary rights to located
resource harvesting sites) owned resources, enables
by the Saanich people, and often coordination of fishing effort
particular families and conservation efforts as
well
Systematic sharing of catch Salmon catch from reefnet haul
amongst participating fishers divided by twos; any "odd"
and their families fish remaining are returned to
the water, another form of
conservation
Reciprocal fishing access rights Builds resilience in the face
are granted to neighboring of uncertainty; opportunities
groups (e.g., Halkomelem weir to access a wider range of
fishing on the Cowichan River) resources
Hereditary rights of individuals Allows learning and
and families to reefnet sustainable use of resources
locations as well as to other, across multiple generations;
associated property, names, links property, place, actions
ceremonial dances, fresh water and beliefs with social
sources, and house sites organization
Ackowledgments We are indebted to the many knowledgeable elders and
cultural specialists who contributed to the development of this paper,
especially: Cyril Carpenter (Heiltsuk), Arvid Charlie
(Hul'qumin'um Coast Salish), Earl Claxton, Sr. (Saanich, Coast
Salish), Chief Johnny and Helen Clifton (Gitga'at, Tsimshian),
Chief Adam Dick (Kwakwaka'wakw), John Elliott Jr. (Saanich, Coast
Salish), Chief Earl Maquinna George (Nuu-Chah-Nulth), Captain Gold
(Haida), Dr. Daisy Sewid-Smith (Kwakwaka'wakw), Kim Recalma-Clutesi
(Kwakwaka'wakw), Dr. Mary Thomas (Secwepemc), Pauline Waterfall
(Heiltsuk), Chief Roger William (Tsilhqot'in). A special Giaxsixa
to Pauline Waterfall for her careful reading of this article and her
contributions and to Nicholas Claxton for his interest in the Saanich
Reefnet fishery. We also thank our colleagues, Dr. Iain Davidson-Hunt,
Dr. Eugene Hunn, Dr. Eugene Anderson, and Nigel Haggan for their
insights. We also acknowledge with appreciation the contributions of the
three anonymous reviewers of this paper. Research was funded in part by
Coasts Under Stress major collaborative research initiative (Dr.
Rosemary Ommer, P.I.) and through a grant from Social Sciences and
Humanities Research Council of Canada (General research grant
#410-2000-1166 to NT). Berkes' work was supported by the Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) and the Canada Research
Chairs program.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the IASCP Conference in Oaxaca, Mexico (August 2004) for a panel organized by
Berkes and Turner, "How does resource management knowledge
develop?"
References
Alcorn, J. B., Bamba, J., Masiun, S., Natalia, I., and Royo, A.
(2003). Keeping ecological resilience afloat in cross-scale turbulence:
an indigenous social movement navigates change in Indonesia. In Berkes,
F., Colding, J., and Folke, C. (eds.), Navigating the Dynamics of
Social-Ecological Systems, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK,
pp. 299-327.
Anderson, E. N. (1996). Ecologies of the Heart. Emotion, Belief,
and the Environment, Oxford University Press, UK.
Balee, W. (1994). Footprints of the Forest. Ka'apor
Ethnobotany--The Historical Ecology of Plant Utilization by an Amazonian
People, Columbia University Press, New York.
BC Parks (2000). Nisga'a Memorial Lava Bed Park. Anhluut'
ukwsim Laxmihl Angwinga' asanskwhl Nisga'a. Pamphlet.
Nisga'a Tribal Council, and BC Parks, Terrace, BC.
Berkes, F. (1999). Sacred Ecology. Traditional Ecological Knowledge
and Resource Management, Taylor & Francis, Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania.
Berkes, F., Colding, J., and Floke, C. (2000). Rediscovery of
Traditional Ecological Knowledge as Adaptive Management. Ecological
Applications 10: 1251-1262.
Blackburn, T.C., and Anderson, K. (eds.) (1993). Before the
Wilderness: Environmental Management by Native Californians,
Anthropological Papers No. 40, Ballena Press, Menlo Park, California.
Blackfoot Gallery Committee (2001). Nisitapiisini. The Story of the
Blackfoot People. Exhibit Guide for The Glenbow Museum, Key Porter
Books, Toronto, ON.
Boas, F. (1921). Ethnology of the Kwakiutl. Bureau of American
Ethnology 35th annual Report, Parts 1 and 2. Smithsonian Institution,
Washington, District of Columbia.
Boyd, R. T. (ed.) (1999). Indians, Fire and the Land in the Pacific
Northwest, Oregon State University Press, Corvallis.
Brightman, R. A. (1993). Grateful Prey. Rock Cree Human-Animal
Relationships, University of California, Berkeley.
Brookfield, H. C., and Paddoch, C. (1994). Appreciating
Agrodiversity. A Look at the Dynamism and Diversity of Indigenous
Farming Practices. Environment 38(5):7-11, 37-45.
Callicott, J. B. (1994). Earth's Insights. A Multicultural
Survey of Ecological Ethics from the Mediterranean Basin to the
Australian Outback, University of California, Berkeley.
Claxton, E., Sr., and Elliott, J., Sr. (1994). Reef Net Technology
of the Saltwater People, Saanich Indian School Board, Brentwood Bay, BC.
Colfer, C. J. P. (1992). Beyond Slash and Burn. Building on
Indigenous Management of Borneo's Tropical Rain Forests. The New
York Botanical Garden, Advances in Economic Botany volume II (C. M.
Peters, Series Editor), Bronx, New York.
Crosby, A. W. (1986). Ecological Imperialism. The Biological
Expansion of Europe, 900-1900, Cambridge University Press, UK.
Davidson-Hunt, I., and Berkes, F. (2003). Learning as you journey:
Anishinaabe perception of social-ecological environments and adaptive
learning. Conservation Ecology 8 (1):5. [online] URL:
http://www.consecol.org/vo18/iss1/art5
Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.) (2005). "Keeping it
Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation on the Northwest
Coast of North America, University of Washington Press, Seattle, and UBC
Press, Vancouver.
Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human
Societies, Norton, New York.
Garrick, D. (1998). Shaped Cedars and Cedar Shaping (Hanson Island,
B.C.), Western Canada Wilderness Committee, Vancouver, BC.
George, E. Maquinna (2003). Living on the Edge. Nuu-Chah-Nulth
History from an Ahousaht Chief's Perspective, Sono Nis, Winlaw, BC.
Hunn, E. S., Johnson, D., Russell, P., and Thornton, T. F. (2003).
Huna Tlingit Traditional Environmental Knowledge, Conservation, and the
Management of a "Wilderness" Park. Current Anthropology 44:
S79-S103.
Jenness, D. (ca. 1930). "The Saanich Indians of Vancouver
Island." Unpublished manuscript, Royal British Columbia Museum.
Victoria, No date, pp. 1-10.
Jentoft, S., McCay, B. J., and Wilson, D. C. (1998). Social Theory
and Fisheries Co-Management. Marine Policy 22: 423-436.
Johns, T. (1996). The Origins of Human Diet and Medicine: Chemical
Ecology, University of Arizona, Tucson.
Johannes, R. E. (1998). The Case for Data-Less Marine Resource
Management: Examples from Tropical Nearshore Fisheries. Trends in
Ecology and Evolution 13: 243-246.
Johannes, R. E. (2002). Did Indigenous Conservation Ethics Exist?
Traditional Marine Resource Management and Knowledge Information
Bulletin 14: 3-6.
Krech, S., III (1999). The Ecological Indian: Myth and History,
Norton, New York.
Lantz, T. C., (2001). Population Ecology and Ethnobotany of
Devil's Club (Oplopanax horridus (Sm.) Torr. and A. Gray. ex.
Miq.). MS thesis, Department of Biology, University of Victoria,
Victoria, BC.
Lantz, T. C., and Antos, J. A. (2002). Clonal Expansion in the
Deciduous Understory Shrub, Devil's Club (Oplopanax horridus (Sm.)
Torr. and A. Gray ex. Miq.). Canadian Journal of Botany 80: 1052-1062.
Lantz, T. C., and Turner, N. J. (2003). Traditional Phenological
Knowledge (TPK) of Aboriginal Peoples in British Columbia. Journal of
Ethnobiology 23(2): 263-286.
Lantz, T.C., Swerhun, K., and Turner, N. J. (2004). Devil's
Club (Oplopanax horridus): An Ethnobotanical Review. Herbalgram 62:
33-48.
Loewen, D. (1998). Ecological, Ethnobotanical, and Nutritional
Aspects of Yellow Glacier Lily, Erythronium grandiflorum Pursh
(Liliaceae) in Western Canada, MS thesis, Department of Biology,
University of Victoria, Victoria, BC.
McIntosh, R. J. (2000). Climate, history and human action. In
McIntosh, R. J., Tainter, J. A., and McIntosh, S. K. (eds.), The Way the
Wind Blows: Climate, History and Human Action, Columbia University
Press, New York, pp. 1-42.
M'Gonigle, M., Walter, E., and McKay, C. (1999). Fishing
Around the Law: The Pacific Salmon Management System as a Structural
Infringement of Aboriginal Rights. Eco-Research Report 99-1, University
of Victoria, Victoria, B.C. McGill Law Journal 45: 263-314.
Minnis, P., and Elisens, W. (2000). Biodiversity and Native
America, University of Oklahoma, Norman.
Moller, H., Berkes, F. Lyver, P. O., and Kislalioglu, M. (2004).
Combining Science and Traditional Ecological Knowledge: Monitoring
Populations for Co-management. Ecology and Society 9(3): 2. [online]
URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vo19/iss3/art2
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the Commons: The Evolution of
Institutions for Collective Action, Cambridge University Press, UK.
Pauly, D., Christensen, V., Dalsgaard, J., Froese, R., and Torres,
F. (1998). Fishing Down Marine Food Webs. Science 279: 860-863.
Peacock, S., and Turner, N. J. (2000). "Just like a
garden": Traditional plant resource management and biodiversity
conservation on the British Columbia Plateau. In Minnis, P., and
Elisens, W. (eds.), Biodiversity and Native North America, University of
Oklahoma, Norman, pp. 133-179.
Redman, C. L. (1999). Human Impact on Ancient Environments,
University of Arizona, Tucson.
Salmon, E. (2000). Kincentric Ecology: Indigenous Perceptions of
the Human-Nature Relationship. Ecological Applications 10(5): 1327-1332.
Scientific Panel for Sustainable Forest Practices in Clayoquot
Sound (1995). First Nations' Perspectives on Forest Practices in
Clayoquot Sound, Cortex Consulting, Victoria, BC.
Smith, E. A., and Wishnie, M. (2000). Conservation and Subsistence
in Small-Scale Societies. Annual Review of Anthropology 29: 493-524.
Swanton, J. R. (1905). Haida Texts and Myths: Skidegate Dialect.
Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 29,
Government Printing Office, Washington, District of Columbia.
Swezey, S., and Heizer, R. F. (1977). Ritual Management of Salmonid
Fish Resources in California. Journal of California Anthropology 4:
6-29.
Teit, J. A. (1912). Mythology of the Thompson Indians. Vol. VII,
Part II. The Jesup North Pacific Expedition. In Boas, F. (ed.), Memoirs
of the American Museum of Natural History, G.E. Stechert, New York.
Thornton, T. F. (1999). Tleikwaani, The "Berried"
Landscape: The Structure of Tlingit Edible Fruit Resources at Glacier
Bay, Alaska. Journal of Ethnobiology 19(1): 27-48.
Touchie, B. (1977). Stealing Daylight. Nitinaht. International
Journal of American Linguistics, Native American Text Series 2(3):
69-97.
Tucker, B. (2003). Comment on Hunn et al., Huna Tlingit Traditional
Environmental Knowledge, Conservation, and the Management of a
"Wilderness" Park. Current Anthropology 44:S98.
Turner, N. J. (2003). "Passing on the News": Women's
Work, Traditional Knowledge and Plant Resource Management in Indigenous
Societies of NW N. America. In Howard, P. (ed.), Women and Plants: Case
Studies on Gender Relations in Local Plant Genetic Resource Management,
Zed Books, UK, pp. 133-149.
Turner, N. J. (2004). Plants of Haida Gwaii. xaadlaa gwaay guud
gina k'aws (Skidegate), xaadlaa gwaayee guud ginn k'aws
(Massett). Sono Nis, Winlaw, BC.
Turner, N. J. (2005). The Earth's Blanket. Traditional
Teachings for Sustainable Living, Douglas & McIntyre, Vancouver, BC
and University of Washington, Seattle.
Turner, N. J., and Atleo, E. R. (Chief Umeek). (1998). Pacific
North American First Peoples and the Environment. In Coward, Harold
(ed.), Traditional and Modern Approaches to the Environment on the
Pacific Rim, Tensions and Values, Centre for Studies in Religion and
Society, State University of New York, Albany, pp. 105-124.
Turner, N. J., and Loewen, D. C. (1998). The Original "Free
Trade": Exchange of Botanical Products and Associated Plant
Knowledge in Northwestern North America. Anthropologica XL (1998):
49-70.
Turner, N. J., and Peacock, S. (2005). Solving the Perennial
Paradox: Ethnobotanical Evidence for Plant Resource Management on the
Northwest Coast. In Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.), "Keeping it
Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation on the Northwest
Coast of North America, University of Washington Press, Seattle and UBC
Press, Vancouver, pp. 101-150.
Turner, N. J., Ignace, M. B., and Ignace, R. (2000). Traditional
Ecological Knowledge and Wisdom of Aboriginal Peoples in British
Columbia. Ecological Applications 10(5): 1275-1287.
Turner, N. J., Davidson-Hunt, I. J., and O'Flaherty, M.
(2003). Living on the Edge: Ecological and Cultural Edges as Sources of
Diversity for Social-Ecological Resilience. Human Ecology 31(3):
439-463.
Turner, N. J., Smith, R. Y., and Jones, J. T. (2005). "A fine
line between two nations": Ownership Patterns for Plant Resources
among Northwest Coast Indigenous Peoples--Implications for Plant
Conservation and Management. In Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.),
"Keeping it Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation
on the Northwest Coast of North America, University of Washington Press,
Seattle and UBC Press, Vancouver, pp. 151-180.
Turner, N. J., Thomas, J., Carlson, B. F., and Ogilvie, R. T.
(1983). Ethnobotany of the Nitinaht Indians of Vancouver Island. British
Columbia Provincial Museum Occasional Paper No. 24, Victoria.
Wall, S. (1993). Vickie Downey, Tewa, Tesuque Pueblo, quoted in:
Wisdom's Daughters. Conversations with Women Elders of Native
America, Harper Collins, New York.
Williams, N. M., and Baines, G. (1988). Traditional Ecological
Knowledge. Wisdom for Sustainable Development, Centre for Resource and
Environmental Studies, Australian National University, Canberra.
Williams, N. M., and Hunn, E. S. (eds.) (1982). Resource Managers:
North American and Australian Hunter-Gatherers, American Association for
the Advancement of Science, Washington, District of Columbia.
Wilson, J. A., Acheson, J. M., Metcalfe, M., and Kleban, P. (1994).
Chaos, Complexity and Communal Management of Fisheries. Marine Policy
18: 291-305.
Young, E., Ross, H., Johnson, J., and Kesteven, J. (1991). Caring
for Country. Aborigines and Land Management, Australian National Parks
and Wildlife Service, Canberra.
N. J. Turner
School of Environmental Studies, University of Victoria, Victoria,
British Columbia V8W 2Y2, Canada e-mail:
[email protected]
F. Berkes
Natural Resources Institute, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg,
Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada