首页    期刊浏览 2025年01月06日 星期一
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Coming to understanding: developing conservation through incremental learning in the Pacific Northwest.
  • 作者:Turner, Nancy J. ; Berkes, Fikret
  • 期刊名称:Human Ecology
  • 印刷版ISSN:1530-7069
  • 出版年度:2006
  • 期号:August
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Cornell University, Human Ecology
  • 关键词:Conservation of natural resources;Natural resource conservation;Traditional knowledge

Coming to understanding: developing conservation through incremental learning in the Pacific Northwest.


Turner, Nancy J. ; Berkes, Fikret


Published online: 20 July 2006

[c] Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Abstract Lessons in conservation are often seen as resulting from cycles of overexploitation and subsequent depletion of resources, followed by catastrophic consequences of shortage and starvation, and finally, development of various strategies, including privatization of the commons, to conserve remaining resource stocks. While such scenarios have undoubtedly occurred on many occasions, we suggest that they are not the only means by which people develop conservation practices and concepts. There are other pathways leading to ecological understanding and conservation, which act at a range of scales and levels of complexity. These include: lessons from the past and from other places, perpetuated and strengthened through oral history and discourse; lessons from animals, learned through observation of migration and population cycles, predator effects, and social dynamics; monitoring resources and human effects on resources (positive and negative), building on experiences and expectations; observing changes in ecosystem cycles and natural disturbance events; trial and error experimentation and incremental modification of habitats and populations. Humans, we believe, are capable of building a sophisticated conservation ethic that transcends individual species and resources. A combination of conservation knowledge, practices, and beliefs can lead to increasingly greater sophistication of ecological understanding and the continued encoding of such knowledge in social institutions and worldview.

Key words Traditional ecological knowledge * conservation * indigenous peoples * ethnoecology.

Introduction

Resource conserving practices of indigenous and local peoples drawn from their traditional knowledge systems have been described for many parts of the world and for many different cultures and environments (Blackburn and Anderson, 1993; Balee, 1994; Berkes, 1999; Berkes et al., 2000; Minnis and Elisens, 2000; Turner et al., 2000; Alcorn et al., 2003; Hunn et al., 2003). A wide variety of conservation strategies have been documented, ranging from cultural teachings against harvesting specific resources or harvesting at specific times or places, to selective or limited harvesting, to sanctions against waste (Berkes, 1999). In fact, traditional ecological knowledge systems are infused with practices and concepts, and modes of teaching and learning that can be related directly and indirectly to resource stewardship and conservation at various scales. However, despite considerable attention directed towards documentation of these systems and approaches to conservation, we still have a limited understanding about their development, evolution, and transmission over time and space.

It is sometimes assumed that the development of community-based conservation strategies, ethics, and teachings is a result of some realization or recognition of a catastrophic resource depletion situation (e.g., Johannes, 1998, 2002). Many authors have questioned whether such resource management systems can be considered to represent "conservation" at all, and, by extension, whether traditional resource managers can be effective conservationists. In part, the argument goes, a "real" conservationist both acts to prevent or mitigate resource depletion and has the intention to conserve (Smith and Wishnie, 2000). We do not wish to enter here into the debate on "conservation" and whether indigenous conservation is likely or even possible (Hunn et al., 2003). However, we refute the hypothesis that conservation is only authentic if it results from the intention to conserve, as have Wilson et al. (1994) and some others. The conservation biology or evolutionary ecology critique of indigenous conservation has its own logic, based on the notion that evolutionary theory more easily accounts for short-term and self-centered behaviors (Tucker, 2003). We find the assumptions behind the evolutionary ecology critique to be too limiting and western-centric; they are not supported by the realities of the indigenous groups that we deal with in this paper. However, we certainly make no claim for a universal conservationist bent in all indigenous or traditional cultures! The ethnohistorical and archaeological record provides evidence of situations in which people failed to conserve their resources, sometimes with dire consequences (Diamond, 1997; Krech, 1999; Redman, 1999).

Our focus is on learning and knowledge accumulation. Despite the undeniable existence of human-caused resource depletion, it seems unlikely that conservation arising from direct experiences with depletion and the resulting crisis is the only way that humans have learned to manage and conserve their resources. Indeed, the pervasiveness of conservation strategies, philosophies, and teachings that result in regulated resource use, or outright care for and conservation of non-resource species, indicates that people are capable of developing and enacting de facto conservation through other means. As Heiltsuk cultural specialist Pauline Waterfall noted (personal communication to NT, May 2004), "We had a form of regulated use based on the understanding that conservation would result if we regulated our use in a mindful and respectful way." For purposes of discussion, we have developed two models for learning conservation: the depletion crisis model and the ecological understanding model. In this paper, we explore the latter: what are the mechanisms and circumstances, beyond direct response to resource depletion, by which people can come to an understanding of the need to care for their resources and develop ways of promoting conservation?

Based mainly (but not exclusively) on the indigenous peoples of the North American Pacific Northwest, and using a general schematic for describing the diverse components of traditional ecological knowledge (Turner et al., 2000), we consider the development of conservation techniques and prescriptions based on the various components of traditional ecological knowledge systems. We then identify some possible mechanisms for building ecological understanding. We use the Saanich Reefnet fishery as an example of a complex conservation and resource management strategy, combining various elements of traditional ecological knowledge. We conclude by discussing the importance of philosophy and worldview in mediating and directing conservation activities. Given that conventional "western" efforts have generally not been successful in meeting conservation goals and objectives, it is crucial to consider possibilities for integration of indigenous and western perspectives in developing strategies for conservation.

We define traditional ecological knowledge as "a cumulative body of knowledge, practice and belief, evolving by adaptive processes and handed down through generations by cultural transmission" (Berkes, 1999, p. 8). In keeping with the teachings of the indigenous peoples of the Pacific Northwest, we define conservation here as keeping something, especially an important environmental or cultural resource, or an entire habitat, from harm, loss, or change, using a resource sparingly so as not to exhaust supplies, and/or using specific measures to maintain and enhance a resource, a suite of resources or entire habitats (for example, as in landscape burning; Boyd, 1999; Peacock and Turner, 2000). Note that conservation in indigenous thought and practice does not preclude use, as it does in some western conservation traditions (Berkes, 1999). Resource depletion refers to a reduction in the abundance or productivity of plants, animals or substances used or required by humans.

Traditional Ecological Knowledge Systems and Learning of Conservation

In the framework presented here, ecological understanding is the term we use to refer to a suite of attributes embodied within traditional ecological knowledge systems, including:

* Incremental learning of individuals and groups and elaboration of environmental knowledge as a result of detailed observation and experience of variations in nature and leading to a sophisticated understanding of the ecosystem in which they dwell;

* Development of concomitant belief systems that help avert serious resource depletion and promote conserving approaches;

* Creating and perpetuating ways of encoding, communicating and disseminating both the practical aspects of such incremental learning and adaptive response and the ideologies and belief systems associated it; and

* Development of institutions that consolidate environmental knowledge and practice, or development of rules by which members of a society deal with their environment and resources.

Incremental Learning from Observation and Experience

We contend that humans living in close proximity to their environments are capable of observing, identifying, monitoring, and reacting to variations in resource availability, ecological relationships, and biological responses to particular circumstances. Such knowledge can be acquired in the same ways as other important knowledge for survival, such as of food and medicine. Learning about new foods and medicines, and how to prepare and process them safely and effectively, for example, has been in large part incremental and cumulative (Johns, 1996). There would have been calamitous experiences and tragedies along the way, but more often, judicious tasting, sampling, experimentation, and evaluation would have guided the learning experience. Just feeling sick from eating a small amount of a plant, or detecting a bitter taste or some temporary hallucinogenic effect, would have been sufficiently remarkable to engender further experimentation or trial or to ward off more intensive use. This type of experience, Johns maintains, is the very mechanism whereby people learned to differentiate between food and medicine. Furthermore, observations leading to ecological understanding can be positive, just as tasting and learning about certain foods can be a positive experience (imagine a first taste of wild strawberry for someone testing for new foods!). For example, a natural burn attracting browsing deer and increased berry production in subsequent years would provide an incentive for clearing and development of anthropogenic burning (e.g., Boyd, 1999). Pauline Waterfall, Heiltsuk cultural specialist and teacher, writes (personal communication to NT, May 2004), "My grandfather taught me that observing how animals behaved and used certain natural resources was normal process of basis for experimenting, modifying, learning. For example, he told me that he came across many trees that had old scars with dried sap mixed with many varieties of animal fur. Upon observing, he discovered that animals came along and rubbed themselves against the sap and that these animals had injuries."

Plant resource management and conservation practices that could have developed incrementally include burning and clearing, pruning, coppicing, tilling, replanting and transplanting, partial harvesting of individual trees and shrubs, selective harvesting for size and life cycle stage, and rotational harvesting through annual or multiyear cycles, as well as genetic selection for maximum productivity or other desirable traits (Williams and Baines, 1988; Blackburn and Anderson, 1993; Minnis and Elisens, 2000; Deur and Turner, 2005).

For animal populations, including shellfish, fish, terrestrial and marine mammals, birds and bird eggs, and other resources check, there are parallel strategies and specific practices applied to maintaining numbers and representative age categories. Many examples exist of conservation practices including harvest selection by age, sex, size, and reproductive stage and season for various species (Moller et al., 2004), as well as the preparing and maintaining of productive habitats and foods for certain key resource species through the use of fire and other means (Brookfield and Paddoch, 1994; Colfer, 1992; Young et al., 1991).

Pauline Waterfall (personal communication to NT May 2004) described another conservation practice used by Heiltsuk hunters, namely to rotate areas where one hunted. "For example," she said, "If lots of deer were caught at a specific area, that area was left alone for a couple of years and other areas were used." Such rotation of harvesting areas indicates that hunters have an understanding of natural renewal cycles and the length of time that a population would need to replenish itself. Another example is from Daisy Sewid-Smith (personal communication to NT, October 1994), when asked about responses to scarcity:
 It was not the idea of harvesting too much ... sometimes something
 would happen where a certain plant won't grow as much as it did last
 year. And they [her elders] said it's a cycle that happens--fish will
 disappear and there won't be very much fish--and yes, they did have
 people monitoring this. And, when they could see that there was going
 to be scarcity, then they were not allowed to go to that particular
 area. They were told to go to another area, and let that area build
 up. And yes, they did have people within the clan or tribal groups
 that monitored these changes ...


There is evidence that some indigenous groups can manage ecological cycles at multiple scales. For example, Cree hunters of James Bay seem to be managing simultaneously beaver populations on a 4- to 6-year cycle, fish on a 5- to 10-year cycle and caribou on a 80- to 100-year cycle (Berkes et al., 2000). Such management systems can work well only if there are systems of proprietorship in place for these resources; otherwise one group's long-term conservation and management practices could be thwarted by another group's unregulated use (cf. Turner et al., 2005).

Belief Systems that Promote Conservation

Eugene Anderson (1996) suggests that the complex belief systems that comprise human religions and that are reflected in our social institutions, narratives, ceremonies and day-to-day activities are engendered from an ethic of conservation and responsibility to the environment. The question is: how do such complex belief systems develop, particularly in relation to conservation of other lifeforms? We know that attitudes are socially mediated and directed, and that attitudes guide and determine our actions. The notion of "respect" predominates in many traditional belief systems (Callicott, 1994; Turner and Atleo, 1998). As Chief Umeek (Richard Atleo) articulates for the Nuu-Chah-Nulth (Scientific Panel for Sustainable Forest Practices in Clayoquot Sound, 1995, p. 15):
 The Creator made all things one.
 All things are related and interconnected.
 All things are sacred.
 All things are therefore to be respected.


Pauline Waterfall reiterated the same perspective for the Heiltsuk (personal communication to NT May 2004): "All living things have a spirit life. We acknowledged and still do that every life is worthy of being respected."

Related sentiment expressed in indigenous societies is appreciation and thanks. As Tewa elder Vickie Downie explains (Wall, 1993):
 You give thanks before you even receive your gifts from the
 Creator. When you ask, you give thanks. Your prayers are
 thanksgivings for everything--the sun, the moon, the snow, the
 water, the fire, the rocks. You see them as being alive, having a
 life of their own. A tree has its own life ...


Another key concept in indigenous belief systems, arising from the first two, is that waste is deplorable: "Take only what you need; never waste anything!" is an instruction heard over and over again in traditional teachings (Turner and Atleo, 1998; Turner et al., 2000); avoiding waste might not always ensure conservation, but it would certainly help remind people of the limits they must impose on their actions. However, there may be a gap between the ideal and the actual practice. Some authors have also argued that the belief in some indigenous cultures in the unlimited renewability of resources could lead to overexploitation. For example, Brightman (1993 p. 280) uses ethnohistorial evidence that the northern Manitoba Cree conceived animals such as caribou as "infinitely renewable resources whose numbers could neither be reduced by overkilling nor managed by selective hunting." But such potential overexploitation would be reined in by the application of rules of respect.

To what extent do attitudes of "respect," "appreciation" and "taking only what you need" within a belief system promote conservation without the experience of serious resource depletion? It may well be that resource depletion--or a series of resource depletions--somewhere at some time in the past did prompt the development of such belief systems (Berkes, 1999; see also, Berkes and Turner, this volume). However, on a broad scale, and over a long time frame, a belief system, in turn, helps prevent overharvesting or wanton destruction of other lifeforms, whether conservation per se is the intended result or not (Minnis and Elisens, 2000).

One of the major social mechanisms by which societies remember and build upon traditions of resource conservation is the use of stories and myths. Many of the traditional narratives of First Nations that reflect lessons of respect, appreciation, and conservation, describe a time of profound lack of resources, when people, sometimes with animal personas, were deprived of even the basic requirements for life: sun, moon, tides, winds, water, fire, and proper food. The stories do not necessarily depict human negligence or overharvesting as the cause of this deprivation. Consider, for example the Saanich story of the Origin of Salmon:
 Once there were no seals and the people were starving; they lived
 on elk and whatever other game they could kill. Two brave youths
 said to each other, "Let us go and see if we can find any salmon."
 They embarked in their canoe and headed out to sea .... They
 journeyed for three and a half months. Then they came to a strange
 country. When they reached the shore a man came out and welcomed
 them ....

 The youths stayed in the place about a month. Their hosts then said
 to them, "You must go home tomorrow. Everything is arranged for
 you. The salmon that you were looking for will muster at your home
 and start off on their journey. You must follow them." So the two
 youths followed the salmon; for three and a half months they
 travelled, day and night, with the fish. Every night they took
 qexmin (Indian celery, Lomatium nudicaule (Pursh) Coult. & Rose)
 and burned it that the salmon might feed on its smoke and sustain
 themselves. Finally they reached Discovery Island (Ktces), where
 they burned qexmin all along the beach; for their hosts had said to
 them, "Burn qexmin along the beach when you reach land, to feed the
 salmon that travel with you. Then, if you treat the salmon well,
 you will always have them in abundance."

 ... Because their journey took them three and a half months, salmon
 are now absent on the coast for that period. The coho said to the
 other salmon, "You can go ahead of us [on the ocean journey], for
 we have not yet got what we wanted from the lakes." That is why the
 coho is always the last of the salmon.


The story continues, relating how the Salmon people taught the young men how and when to make and use reef nets and how to honor the first salmon with a ceremony and prayer "... that [the salmon] may always be plentiful" (Jenness, 1930).

There are many other stories, such as the Ditidaht story of the origin of the winds, tides, seasons, fire, fish, sun, moon, and daylight (Touchie, 1977), in which the resources that people rely on were provided as gifts from the Creator, or from the powers and generosity of individuals like Raven and other supernatural beings and cultural heroes. Some convey imperatives for conservation directly, but most impart a general sense of the need for appreciating and not wasting these valuable gifts. The end result, however, would be a conserving philosophy.

Perceived kinship with other lifeforms, even trees and other plants, leads to a different way of treating these beings (Salmon, 2000; Turner, 2005). For example, Northwest Coast people had the technology and perhaps even a practical reason to kill cedar trees when they were removing the bark for clothing, mats or baskets. Yet everywhere teachings directed harvesters to be careful and only remove one or two straps from each tree, so as to keep the tree alive (Deur and Turner, 2005). Franz Boas (1921, pp. 616-617) explains this practice for the Kwakwaka'wakw:
 Even when the young cedar-tree is quite smooth, they do not take
 all of the cedar-bark, for the people of the olden times said that
 if they should peel off all the cedar-bark ... the young cedar
 would die, and then another cedar-tree near by would curse the
 bark-peeler so that he would also die. Therefore, the bark-peelers
 never take all of the bark off a young tree.


The ultimate motivation for this practice was evidently to avoid needless killing of another lifeform, rather than conservation per se in the western sense. Pauline Waterfall (personal communication to NT May 2004) explained: "Needless killing is a teaching that is passed along to impress upon us that if we don't respect another lifeform, it won't return or will die out, thereby depriving us of future access and use. This is an explicit teaching of conservation." In any case, the practice of avoiding needless loss of life reflects a widespread belief system, at once respectful and utilitarian, that would engender conservation. Gitga'at elder Helen Clifton (personal communication to NT 2002) said that they were always taught as children that all creatures--mice, wolves, birds--had their own families and their own lives, parallel to and as important as those of people. Children were warned not to harm or needlessly disturb them. Mountains, too, were regarded as living beings, with their own stories and their own families, and requiring of respect. For example, there are certain mountains that one should never point to, or they will cause hard luck at some time in the future, especially bad weather (Roger William, Tsilhqot'in, personal communication to NT 2003; Elsie Claxton, Saanich, personal communication to NT 1998). Children were taught not to pick certain flowers or it might cause rain, or fog, or lightning storms (Turner et al., 1983; Turner, 2004).

There is a well-known narrative theme, recurring in several language communities on the North Coast of British Columbia and coastal Alaska (Haida, Haisla, Tsimshian, Tlingit), about the consequences of harming frogs. In the Haida version, centering in the village of Cumshewa, young boys on a fishing outing were playing with a frog, and threw it into the fire. The frog's mother, a supernatural being named Jilaa quns, or Creek Woman, was so distraught by this cruelty that she predicted the death of the boys, one after another, and finally, the destruction of their entire village (Turner, 2005). How closely is such a story based on an actual event, and, if it is, where and when did the event take place? It would have been many hundreds or thousands of ago, yet it still serves to teach one generation after the next about the dangers of willfully harming other species--in this case, a species that is not eaten by humans. The Nisga'a stories of the lava flows that destroyed several villages, said to be caused by the cruel and disrespectful treatment of salmon by a boy in one of the villages, is similar in the power of its lessons, perhaps even more so because of the obvious presence of the lava beds in the area (BC Parks, 2000). This story, and the lessons it embodies, seems to have its origins not in resource depletion but in disrespectful behavior--in lack of an environmental ethic.

Communication and Dissemination of Conservation Actions and Ethics

Stories and teachings are one example of the ways in which understandings of conservation and environmental ethics can be disseminated over time and space. Just as in learning about edible, poisonous, and medicinal plants without continuous episodes of fatality, it is not necessary for lessons in conservation to be learned by major catastrophic episodes of resource depletion. Rather, observations and experiences and guiding principles can be taught and acquired over generations, and spread through stories, ceremonies, and discourse from one community to another (Turner et al. 2000). Children are often taught by parents and elders about their responsibilities to their family, clan, and the other lifeforms. Community gatherings, such as potlatches and feasts, are occasions for reinforcing these values (George, 2003).

Social relationships, such as the roles and responsibilities of the hereditary chiefs and leaders in relation to their people, resources and territories, would also be reiterated at such times (Turner et al., 2005). Individuals and groups within a community would hold specialized knowledge to be imparted at appropriate times and circumstances (Turner, 2003). Some knowledge, especially relating to specific places, might be held privately, as part of an individual's or family's proprietory rights. Individual words and phrases, as well as stories, and lessons conveyed in art, music, and ceremony, are all part of this knowledge system leading to dissemination of environmental understanding. Children, too, participate on a daily basis in activities that foster conservation.

Secwepemc elder, Dr. Mary Thomas (personal communication to NT 2002), for example, learned lessons about conservation from her two grandmothers. As a child, she watched and helped while her grandmother carefully moved the carcasses of the salmon after they had spawned, from the banks of the Salmon River back into the water. Her grandmother explained that the salmon would nourish the baby fish to come. She also observed her grandmother remove and replant the smaller glacier lily (Erythronium grandiflorum Pursh) and chocolate lily (Fritillaria lanceolata Pursh) bulbs that Mary and her siblings had selected from the overturned turf and put into baskets when they were out harvesting roots with her. Her grandmother told them that they should only take the biggest roots and leave the small ones to grow for the future. In these demonstrations, and in Mary's participation, her grandmother was reinforcing the values of respect for other lifeforms and the practices required to sustain resources, imparting lessons that have endured over the many decades of Mary's life.

Creating and perpetuating ways of encoding, communicating and disseminating both the practical aspects of such incremental learning and adaptive responses and the ideologies and belief systems associated with them is as important today as in the past. In many cases, the opportunities for children to spend time with and learn from parents, grandparents, and others knowledgeable about conservation practices and beliefs, have been diminished, as have their opportunities for direct interaction with habitats and resources. This is a serious issue that needs to be addressed if traditional knowledge is not to be lost. This kind of knowledge cannot be mastered from books.

Institutions for Conservation Practice and Behavior

We have argued that environmental knowledge may be elaborated incrementally, leading to the development of belief systems and ways of transmitting this knowledge, both temporally (to other generations) and geographically (to other groups; cf. Turner et al., 2003). Another component of developing and consolidating conservation is the emergence of institutions that foster the perpetuation of values and knowledge. By institutions, we mean the set of rules actually used or rules-in-use (Ostrom, 1990). Such institutions are socially constructed, with normative and cognitive dimensions, thus they embed values (Jentoft et al., 1998).

Rule sets that define access rights and specify appropriate behaviors are often known as tenure systems. There is a well-developed literature on land and marine tenure systems in the Pacific Northwest (e.g., Williams and Hunn, 1982; Turner et al., 2005) and we need not repeat them here. It is useful, however, to investigate some apparently simple plant use systems to illustrate how such institutions work. The harvesting of devil's club (Oplopanax horridum (Sm.) Torr. and A. Gray ex. Miq.) for medicine is a case in point (Lantz, 2001; Lantz and Antos, 2002). In general, only those who are considered medicine specialists are sanctioned to harvest and administer devil's club medicine. Even then, there are protocols to follow. It is usual to seek devil's club for harvesting in a remote place, removed from human settlement or intrusion. A harvester is taught to be conserving in terms of the way the medicine is harvested and in the quantities taken. Generally, only the branches are taken, not the main "mother" stalk (Captain Gold, Haida, personal communication to NT, 1996, among others). A practice of the Gitga'at (Tsimshian) is for the harvester to take no more than four stalks at any one time (Helen Clifton, Gitga'at, personal communication to NT, 2004). The harvester offers a prayer or words of thanks to the plant, and generally leaves a small gift of tobacco or a coin in the place where the plant was harvested. Some harvesters make a practice of replanting stem sections in the ground whenever they remove part of the plant (Arvid Charlie, personal communication to NT and T. Lantz 2000; Lantz et al., 2004). The "rules" around devil's club harvesting are embedded in a belief system in which devil's club is a living being with a spirit and the capacity to help or harm humans, depending upon how it is approached (Turner, 2004).

Another example is the harvesting and use of highbush cranberries (Viburnum edule (Michx.) Raf.). These berries are considered a valuable food, but the bushes can be quite sporadic. In many places, highbush cranberry patches are specifically owned. The owner, usually a chief or matriarch, may delegate others to pick the berries or may organize and lead a picking expedition of community members, but is always given a portion of the harvest. Then, in turn, the owner of the patch is expected to host a feast at which these berries are served. The patches are carefully monitored, both to determine when the berries are ripe and to ensure that outsiders do not encroach on the harvest. People have been known to transplant highbush cranberries to closer localities, and often these and other berry patches are cared for like orchards (Turner, 2005; Turner and Peacock, 2005; Turner et al., 2005). It is doubtful that such management developed as a result of catastrophic depletion.

Just as there are rules about the appropriate ways to harvest resources, there are also rules about how people make new environmental observations and how these observations eventually become part of the accepted knowledge of that group. Working with the Anishinaabe (Ojibwa) people of northwestern Ontario, Davidson-Hunt and Berkes (2003) argue that such learning requires maintaining a web of relationships of people and places. People literally learn as they travel over the landscape. This knowledge is remembered through social memory (McIntosh, 2000, p. 24) for long-term communal understanding of environmental change and the transmission of pertinent experience. Social memory describes how an individual thought or observation emerging out of a specific experience can become a part of the collective knowledge of a group. But how does individual creativity, emerging in response to a change in the environment, lead to change in social memory?

The rules that govern the evolution of knowledge in a particular group may be called "institutions of knowledge," defined as framing the process of creativity, learning, and remembering (Davidson-Hunt and Berkes, 2003). Institutions of knowledge, in the Anishinaabe case, comprise rules and values about how the process of learning can occur, the culturally correct way in which knowledge can be transmitted, how individual competency develops, and how observations of a specific experience may become part of the accepted, authoritative knowledge of the group.

Learning is a life-long process and legitimate knowledge requires establishing competency over a period of time. Not all observations are equally relevant or equally important. There is a filtering process of learning, within the bounds or frame set by the socially accepted rules that govern the establishment of accepted knowledge. Elders play a key role in this process. Among the Anishinaabe, elder is a social role and designation. Not all old people are elders; conversely some middle-aged people who have developed their competencies relatively quickly may be considered as elders. Institutions of knowledge allow authoritative and legitimate knowledge to be built through experience; this is by and large an incremental process. Understandings of the environment, embedded firmly within belief systems and informing resource use practices, can enable people to live within the constraints of their environment without the necessity of a major resource depletion crisis to force catastrophic learning.

Mechanisms for Building Ecological Understanding

The ability or capacity to learn from small and incremental lessons and from the experiences of others potentially enables people to develop sustainable practices and ecological understandings without always having to respond to and learn from crisis situations. Not only an event itself, but any inferences, extrapolations, or interpretations people draw from it can be enfolded into an enriched, elaborated system of knowledge and practice. Over time, even within one lifetime, experiences of others blend with personal knowledge and observations, compounding and accumulating to bring enhanced knowledge and wisdom. Table I summarizes some of the pathways by which lessons in land and resource management and knowledge of the importance of conservation may have been accrued, using examples from northwestern North American Indigenous peoples.

In reality, the knowledge acquisition and learning leading to an environmental ethic, and ultimately conservation of resources and biological diversity, is highly complex; it is impossible to trace and identify the countless diverse, tangled, and interwoven threads that comprise traditional knowledge systems and their origin and development. Under these circumstances, it is difficult, if not impossible, to identify intention to conserve as separate from a belief system that values and recognizes as kin all lifeforms, from frogs to wolves to cedar trees. Nevertheless, this system, within its cultural contexts, seems to have worked well for people in maintaining their resources over a long period of time. Resource depletion, natural and human-mediated, is a part of the story, but not the whole story.

Developing Conservation: The Reefnet Fishery Example

In reality, people probably combine the lessons and understandings gained from all of the pathways, including experiences of resource depletion, to build up their knowledge, practices, and beliefs into complex systems of land and resource management. These systems ingrain social structures, land and marine tenure systems, and opportunities for adaptation at different scales. For instance, as described by Daisy Sewid-Smith and Chief Adam Dick (personal communication to NT 1997), one time, shortly before Captain George Vancouver arrived on the Northwest Coast (1792), the salmon returning to spawn to the Nimpkish River on northeastern Vancouver Island were very scarce. The Chiefs of the villages situated along the river conversed and decided to place a moratorium on salmon fishing. They moved all the people out to the mouth of the river or to other villages, and they all lived on shellfish and other food until the salmon became more plentiful. (The salmon were described as having their own societies and practices that paralleled those of humans, and it was said that they had gone to attend a wedding of the daughter of the Chief of the Underwater World.) This story reflects some of the complexities around conservation, including social institutions and leadership, resource tenure, belief systems, and adaptive capacity of communities. Although resource depletion was experienced, there were already institutional mechanisms in place to respond to the situation. There are many other treatments of Aboriginal fishing systems and practices relating to fisheries conservation (cf. Swezey and Heizer, 1977; M'Gonigle et al., 1999).

The Saanich reefnet fishery (Claxton and Elliott, 1994; Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2004) serves as a detailed example of how narratives, social rules, ecological knowledge, conservation practices, and technology come together. Reefnet (SXOLE) salmon fishing technology is ancient, and the Straits Salish peoples around Haro and Rosario Straits of southern Vancouver Island, the Gulf Islands, and the American San Juan Islands are well-known for this unique innovation. There are only certain localities where the reefnet can be used, places where the schools of migrating salmon are channeled into a restricted passage, for example between an island and an adjacent reef. These locations fall under the hereditary ownership of individuals and cannot be used by anyone else without permission (Fig. 1).

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Reefnet fishing usually lasts around a month, starting when the oceanspray (Holodiscus discolor (Pursh) Maxim.) starts to bloom and the first berries start to ripen in late June. The reefnet is made of willowbark (Salix lasiandra Benth. and other Salix spp.) and other plant materials and is suspended into the water between two canoes, with a leading edge, or "floor," anchored in place and extending towards the direction from where the salmon are coming (Fig. 2). Sometimes a channel is cleared in an offshore kelp bed leading to the reefnet to help direct the salmon. The leading edge is positioned so that it forms a false bottom sloping downwards to give the approaching salmon the impression that they are swimming into deeper water. Strands of American dunegrass (Elymus mollis Trin.) tied onto the "floor" help in this deception, making the approach resemble the oceanbottom (Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2000). The crews in the two canoes wait until there is evidence that the salmon are approaching--either sighting them underwater, or seeing the advancing fish jumping out of the water. Sometimes, if visibility is poor, they induce the fish to jump by raising some of the floor lines to make the approach more uneven; the lead salmon will jump out of the water to see what the conditions are like (Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2000). The fishers know just how long it will take the salmon to swim into the main part of the net between the canoes. Then, when enough fish are in position, the crew brings the vessels together, pulling up the net at the same time and capturing the fish in the canoes. For conservation purposes, a sizable willow hoop is constructed at head of the net. This is to ensure that a few fish--regarded as all members of the same family--will invariably escape and be able to continue on to their home river to spawn. Earl Claxton Sr. (personal communication to NT 2004) explained that the salmon, like humans, travel in family groups, and it is very important, just as for human families, that some members of the family be allowed to carry on their lineage. Any of the salmon can be caught in the reefnet device, but sockeye and springs are the two most generally sought. The first salmon of the season to be caught is thanked and celebrated through a special ritual. The salmon caught after this are divided up in twos amongst the participating fishers; any odd fish left is replaced in the ocean.

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

Learning how to use the reefnet technology, and regulating its use effectively, required tremendous attention and practice for all those participating and special administrative skills on the part of the owner. Ironically, the Canadian government banned the use of reefnets in the mid 1900s, and only a few members of the Saanich Nation who had an opportunity to fish across the border in the San Juan Islands were able to retain the knowledge and practice of this sustainable fishing method (Claxton and Elliott, 1994; Earl Claxton Sr., personal communication to NT 2004).

The reefnet system, said to be taught to the Saanich people by the Salmon people themselves and by the Creator, XALS, at the same time when the Salmon first offered themselves to the Saanich (as in the story told earlier), represents technological and social knowledge and skills, conserving and respectful attitudes embodied in traditional Saanich philosophies, and mechanisms for perpetuating these practices, technologies, and beliefs from generation to generation (Table II).

Conclusions

The resource harvesting and management systems discussed here, embodying belief systems, narratives, ceremonies, specialized vocabulary, and other means of communicating and acquiring knowledge, institutional structures for regulating resource use, and multifaceted arrangements such as the Saanich reefnet fishery, are too complex and culturally ingrained to have been developed solely in response to experiences of catastrophic resource depletion. Major depletion events may have at some point triggered some of these mechanisms, but they cannot explain or account for the whole range of cultural constructions that lead to sustainable resource use and conservation.

The circumstances for the development of the Saanich reefnet fishery are lost in the mists of time, but the Saanich maintain that they have always been careful not to deplete their stocks, and declare that it is only in the last century, since their reefnets were banned in Canada, that the stocks of sockeye, coho, and other salmon have declined, in some cases to the point of extinction.

The power and potential of such holistic traditional systems that combine harvesting with resource management and conservation is undeniable. It seems that modern industrial society has not been able to match the success of traditional conservation practices, whatever their origins, even with the knowledge of ongoing resource depletion (Pauly et al., 1998). We need something more to help us conserve effectively. Adaptive comanagement, incorporating elements of the complex knowledge system from which the Saanich reefnet was generated, may enable all of us to live more sustainably. Our worldviews and attitudes are critical components of conservation, and may be more important than any other factor in conserving ecological integrity.
Table I Potential Pathways for Accruing Land and Resource Management and
Conservation Lessons with Examples from Indigenous Societies of
Northwestern North America

Mechanisms Explanation References

Lessons from the past Stories of positive George, 2003; Teit,
 and negative 1912; BC Parks,
 experiences, 2000
 remembered by
 individuals,
 recounted within
 families and
 communities, or
 embedded in art,
 place names and
 ceremonies

Language Terms that embody Pauline Waterfall,
 conservation personal
 concepts, communication May
 understandings and 2004; Earl Maquinna
 teachings, e.g., the George, personal
 Heiltsuk word communication to NT
 mnaqels, which refers 1998
 to "selectively
 collecting things
 outside," and
 miaisila, which
 refers to someone
 whose responsibility
 it is to be a
 guardian of certain
 fish-bearing rivers,
 or the Nuu-chah-nulth
 word 7uh-mowashitl,
 "to keep some and not
 take all"

Metaphorical sayings Symbolic and Swanton, 1905;
and narratives metaphorical stories Teit, 1912
 also teach lessons
 about conservation
 (e.g., Nlaka'pamux
 story of Old One and
 the Creation of the
 Earth; Haida Jila
 quuns story)

Lessons from other Technologies, Turner and Loewen,
places products, names, and 1998; Turner et
 ideas relating to al., 2003
 conservation and
 environmental
 stewardship (e.g.,
 use of fire for
 clearing; digging and
 propagation
 techniques; first
 foods ceremonies)
 passed from one
 community to the next
 through
 intermarriage,
 potlatches, trade

Learning from animals Observations of Loewen, 1998; Hunn
 animal foraging et al., 2003;
 strategies, Blackfoot Gallery
 populations, browsing Committee, 2001;
 and predation, Deur and Turner,
 behaviors that might 2005
 engender
 understandings of
 kinship and
 reciprocity (e.g.,
 grizzlies foraging
 for edible roots;
 birds' egg-laying
 habits; pack and
 leadership
 relationships in
 wolves; bears
 "pruning" berry
 bushes)

Monitoring--building on Routine observation Lantz and Turner,
experiences and of seasonal changes, 2003; Davidson-Hunt
expectations animal migrations, and Berkes, 2003
 plant life cycles,
 and berry production
 brings recognition of
 expected patterns and
 ability to detect
 variation from the
 norm

Observing ecosystem Relative abundance Boyd, 1999; George,
cycles and disturbance and productivity of 2003; Thornton,
events plants and animals in 1999
 particular
 circumstances, both
 temporal and spatial,
 can guide peoples'
 land and resource
 management strategies
 (e.g., successional
 stages following
 fire; effects of
 flooding on salmon
 migration patterns;
 relation between
 moisture and berry
 productivity)

Trial and error Observing the Garrick, 1998;
experimentation and results--positive and Boyd, 1999
incremental negative, intentional
modification or incidental, short-
 and long-term--of
 people's own
 activities, such as
 selective harvesting
 (e.g., harvesting
 cedar bark and
 planks), or of
 emulating natural
 disturbance (e.g.,
 use of fire to clear
 patches)

Learning by If a practice works Turner and Loewen,
association, extension, in one place at one 1998
and extrapolation time, it might work
 in another place at
 another time;
 conversely, if a
 practice or activity
 results in negative
 consequences in one
 circumstance, it
 might be avoided at
 another time or place
 (e.g., knowledge
 about harvesting or
 conserving one type
 of shellfish, berry
 or root might be
 extended to other,
 similar types)

Elaborating and Combining the lessons Cyril Carpenter and
building sophistication and understandings Pauline P.
 gained from all of Waterfall, personal
 these pathways, and communication 2002;
 building up Turner, 2005; Deur
 knowledge, practices and Turner, 2005
 and beliefs into
 complex systems of
 land and resource
 management (e.g.,
 Heiltsuk berry
 gardens; Saanich
 reefnet fishery)

Table II The Saanich Reefnet Fishery as a Complex Conservation and
Resource Management Strategy [Based on Claxton and Elliott, 1994; and
Interview with Earl Claxton Sr. (personal communication to NT and
Nicholas Claxton, September, 2004)]

Practical techniques Inferred knowledge and
 understanding

Creation of a false ocean bottom Understanding salmon schooling
leading to reefnet, held in habits and habitat
place by anchor stones preferences; technologies for
 constructing, placing and
 anchoring lines

Cutting swathes through kelp Identifying key salmon habitat
beds to create channels for the and preferences
fish

Paired canoes positioned for Understanding of western
pulling in the reefnet; moved by red-cedar (Thuja plicata Donn
tides and currents ex D. Don), woodworking and
 canoe making technologies;
 tides, weather, currents,
 seasonal changes

Construction of the nets from Understanding of willow
the inner bark of Pacific willow seasonality, prime habitat,
(Salix lasiandra Benth.) and coppicing, regeneration,
other willows harvesting and net making
 techniques

Distinguishing different Pacific Each kind of salmon has its
salmon species [e.g., coho own season, habits, fat
(Oncorhynchus kisutch), Chinook content, etc.; important for
(O. tshawytscha, also known as timing of fishing and
spring) and sockeye (O. nerka)] processing methods; provides
 diversity and flexibility in
 fishery

Use of ecological indicators, Familiarity of salmon
like time of blooming of lifecycles in context of
oceanspray (Holodiscus discolor overall ecosystem cycles and
(Pursh) Maxim.), berries weather patterns (cf. Lantz
ripening and summer and Turner, 2003);
thunderstorms, to judge the understanding of the dynamics
timing of the fishing activity of salmon runs

Belief system Result in terms of
 Conservation

Salmon and other resources, as Constraints against waste;
well as techniques for take only as many as needed,
harvesting and processing them, and as many as can be properly
as gifts of the Creator, XALS processed

Salmon viewed as members of Need to always allow
families and lineages, akin to escapement of a portion of the
human families catch (a built in "escape"
 hole at the end of the net),
 to perpetuate individual
 Salmon families

Humans as responsible to the Respect and careful use
Salmon and other resources

Attachment to territory and Attention to specific places
place, as in traditional tenure and changes over many
systems generations

Communication and Dissemination Perpetuating Knowledge and
of Practice and Belief Beliefs

Original absence of salmon Primary resource depletion,
 followed by the "gift" of
 salmon

Ritual and spiritual recognition First Salmon ceremony (cf.
of the gift of salmon Swezey and Heizer 1977)

Recognition of different kinds Use of names; taxonomies for
of salmon distinguishing different
 species and stocks

All the reefnet locations have Allows proprietory recognition
names and monitoring of specific
 sites; enhances communication

Stories, narratives, language Reinforcing and communicating
and names about the reefnet knowledge and beliefs
fishery and the salmon

Institutions: the tenure system Rules that govern access and
 fisher behavior

Social organization of fishers, Confirming, teaching and
families and leaders enforcing the cultural
 constraints against waste and
 disrespect

Fishing areas (and other Proprietary rights to located
resource harvesting sites) owned resources, enables
by the Saanich people, and often coordination of fishing effort
particular families and conservation efforts as
 well

Systematic sharing of catch Salmon catch from reefnet haul
amongst participating fishers divided by twos; any "odd"
and their families fish remaining are returned to
 the water, another form of
 conservation

Reciprocal fishing access rights Builds resilience in the face
are granted to neighboring of uncertainty; opportunities
groups (e.g., Halkomelem weir to access a wider range of
fishing on the Cowichan River) resources

Hereditary rights of individuals Allows learning and
and families to reefnet sustainable use of resources
locations as well as to other, across multiple generations;
associated property, names, links property, place, actions
ceremonial dances, fresh water and beliefs with social
sources, and house sites organization


Ackowledgments We are indebted to the many knowledgeable elders and cultural specialists who contributed to the development of this paper, especially: Cyril Carpenter (Heiltsuk), Arvid Charlie (Hul'qumin'um Coast Salish), Earl Claxton, Sr. (Saanich, Coast Salish), Chief Johnny and Helen Clifton (Gitga'at, Tsimshian), Chief Adam Dick (Kwakwaka'wakw), John Elliott Jr. (Saanich, Coast Salish), Chief Earl Maquinna George (Nuu-Chah-Nulth), Captain Gold (Haida), Dr. Daisy Sewid-Smith (Kwakwaka'wakw), Kim Recalma-Clutesi (Kwakwaka'wakw), Dr. Mary Thomas (Secwepemc), Pauline Waterfall (Heiltsuk), Chief Roger William (Tsilhqot'in). A special Giaxsixa to Pauline Waterfall for her careful reading of this article and her contributions and to Nicholas Claxton for his interest in the Saanich Reefnet fishery. We also thank our colleagues, Dr. Iain Davidson-Hunt, Dr. Eugene Hunn, Dr. Eugene Anderson, and Nigel Haggan for their insights. We also acknowledge with appreciation the contributions of the three anonymous reviewers of this paper. Research was funded in part by Coasts Under Stress major collaborative research initiative (Dr. Rosemary Ommer, P.I.) and through a grant from Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (General research grant #410-2000-1166 to NT). Berkes' work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) and the Canada Research Chairs program.

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the IASCP Conference in Oaxaca, Mexico (August 2004) for a panel organized by Berkes and Turner, "How does resource management knowledge develop?"

References

Alcorn, J. B., Bamba, J., Masiun, S., Natalia, I., and Royo, A. (2003). Keeping ecological resilience afloat in cross-scale turbulence: an indigenous social movement navigates change in Indonesia. In Berkes, F., Colding, J., and Folke, C. (eds.), Navigating the Dynamics of Social-Ecological Systems, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 299-327.

Anderson, E. N. (1996). Ecologies of the Heart. Emotion, Belief, and the Environment, Oxford University Press, UK.

Balee, W. (1994). Footprints of the Forest. Ka'apor Ethnobotany--The Historical Ecology of Plant Utilization by an Amazonian People, Columbia University Press, New York.

BC Parks (2000). Nisga'a Memorial Lava Bed Park. Anhluut' ukwsim Laxmihl Angwinga' asanskwhl Nisga'a. Pamphlet. Nisga'a Tribal Council, and BC Parks, Terrace, BC.

Berkes, F. (1999). Sacred Ecology. Traditional Ecological Knowledge and Resource Management, Taylor & Francis, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.

Berkes, F., Colding, J., and Floke, C. (2000). Rediscovery of Traditional Ecological Knowledge as Adaptive Management. Ecological Applications 10: 1251-1262.

Blackburn, T.C., and Anderson, K. (eds.) (1993). Before the Wilderness: Environmental Management by Native Californians, Anthropological Papers No. 40, Ballena Press, Menlo Park, California.

Blackfoot Gallery Committee (2001). Nisitapiisini. The Story of the Blackfoot People. Exhibit Guide for The Glenbow Museum, Key Porter Books, Toronto, ON.

Boas, F. (1921). Ethnology of the Kwakiutl. Bureau of American Ethnology 35th annual Report, Parts 1 and 2. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, District of Columbia.

Boyd, R. T. (ed.) (1999). Indians, Fire and the Land in the Pacific Northwest, Oregon State University Press, Corvallis.

Brightman, R. A. (1993). Grateful Prey. Rock Cree Human-Animal Relationships, University of California, Berkeley.

Brookfield, H. C., and Paddoch, C. (1994). Appreciating Agrodiversity. A Look at the Dynamism and Diversity of Indigenous Farming Practices. Environment 38(5):7-11, 37-45.

Callicott, J. B. (1994). Earth's Insights. A Multicultural Survey of Ecological Ethics from the Mediterranean Basin to the Australian Outback, University of California, Berkeley.

Claxton, E., Sr., and Elliott, J., Sr. (1994). Reef Net Technology of the Saltwater People, Saanich Indian School Board, Brentwood Bay, BC.

Colfer, C. J. P. (1992). Beyond Slash and Burn. Building on Indigenous Management of Borneo's Tropical Rain Forests. The New York Botanical Garden, Advances in Economic Botany volume II (C. M. Peters, Series Editor), Bronx, New York.

Crosby, A. W. (1986). Ecological Imperialism. The Biological Expansion of Europe, 900-1900, Cambridge University Press, UK.

Davidson-Hunt, I., and Berkes, F. (2003). Learning as you journey: Anishinaabe perception of social-ecological environments and adaptive learning. Conservation Ecology 8 (1):5. [online] URL: http://www.consecol.org/vo18/iss1/art5

Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.) (2005). "Keeping it Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation on the Northwest Coast of North America, University of Washington Press, Seattle, and UBC Press, Vancouver.

Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies, Norton, New York.

Garrick, D. (1998). Shaped Cedars and Cedar Shaping (Hanson Island, B.C.), Western Canada Wilderness Committee, Vancouver, BC.

George, E. Maquinna (2003). Living on the Edge. Nuu-Chah-Nulth History from an Ahousaht Chief's Perspective, Sono Nis, Winlaw, BC.

Hunn, E. S., Johnson, D., Russell, P., and Thornton, T. F. (2003). Huna Tlingit Traditional Environmental Knowledge, Conservation, and the Management of a "Wilderness" Park. Current Anthropology 44: S79-S103.

Jenness, D. (ca. 1930). "The Saanich Indians of Vancouver Island." Unpublished manuscript, Royal British Columbia Museum. Victoria, No date, pp. 1-10.

Jentoft, S., McCay, B. J., and Wilson, D. C. (1998). Social Theory and Fisheries Co-Management. Marine Policy 22: 423-436.

Johns, T. (1996). The Origins of Human Diet and Medicine: Chemical Ecology, University of Arizona, Tucson.

Johannes, R. E. (1998). The Case for Data-Less Marine Resource Management: Examples from Tropical Nearshore Fisheries. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13: 243-246.

Johannes, R. E. (2002). Did Indigenous Conservation Ethics Exist? Traditional Marine Resource Management and Knowledge Information Bulletin 14: 3-6.

Krech, S., III (1999). The Ecological Indian: Myth and History, Norton, New York.

Lantz, T. C., (2001). Population Ecology and Ethnobotany of Devil's Club (Oplopanax horridus (Sm.) Torr. and A. Gray. ex. Miq.). MS thesis, Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC.

Lantz, T. C., and Antos, J. A. (2002). Clonal Expansion in the Deciduous Understory Shrub, Devil's Club (Oplopanax horridus (Sm.) Torr. and A. Gray ex. Miq.). Canadian Journal of Botany 80: 1052-1062.

Lantz, T. C., and Turner, N. J. (2003). Traditional Phenological Knowledge (TPK) of Aboriginal Peoples in British Columbia. Journal of Ethnobiology 23(2): 263-286.

Lantz, T.C., Swerhun, K., and Turner, N. J. (2004). Devil's Club (Oplopanax horridus): An Ethnobotanical Review. Herbalgram 62: 33-48.

Loewen, D. (1998). Ecological, Ethnobotanical, and Nutritional Aspects of Yellow Glacier Lily, Erythronium grandiflorum Pursh (Liliaceae) in Western Canada, MS thesis, Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC.

McIntosh, R. J. (2000). Climate, history and human action. In McIntosh, R. J., Tainter, J. A., and McIntosh, S. K. (eds.), The Way the Wind Blows: Climate, History and Human Action, Columbia University Press, New York, pp. 1-42.

M'Gonigle, M., Walter, E., and McKay, C. (1999). Fishing Around the Law: The Pacific Salmon Management System as a Structural Infringement of Aboriginal Rights. Eco-Research Report 99-1, University of Victoria, Victoria, B.C. McGill Law Journal 45: 263-314.

Minnis, P., and Elisens, W. (2000). Biodiversity and Native America, University of Oklahoma, Norman.

Moller, H., Berkes, F. Lyver, P. O., and Kislalioglu, M. (2004). Combining Science and Traditional Ecological Knowledge: Monitoring Populations for Co-management. Ecology and Society 9(3): 2. [online] URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vo19/iss3/art2

Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action, Cambridge University Press, UK.

Pauly, D., Christensen, V., Dalsgaard, J., Froese, R., and Torres, F. (1998). Fishing Down Marine Food Webs. Science 279: 860-863.

Peacock, S., and Turner, N. J. (2000). "Just like a garden": Traditional plant resource management and biodiversity conservation on the British Columbia Plateau. In Minnis, P., and Elisens, W. (eds.), Biodiversity and Native North America, University of Oklahoma, Norman, pp. 133-179.

Redman, C. L. (1999). Human Impact on Ancient Environments, University of Arizona, Tucson.

Salmon, E. (2000). Kincentric Ecology: Indigenous Perceptions of the Human-Nature Relationship. Ecological Applications 10(5): 1327-1332.

Scientific Panel for Sustainable Forest Practices in Clayoquot Sound (1995). First Nations' Perspectives on Forest Practices in Clayoquot Sound, Cortex Consulting, Victoria, BC.

Smith, E. A., and Wishnie, M. (2000). Conservation and Subsistence in Small-Scale Societies. Annual Review of Anthropology 29: 493-524.

Swanton, J. R. (1905). Haida Texts and Myths: Skidegate Dialect. Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 29, Government Printing Office, Washington, District of Columbia.

Swezey, S., and Heizer, R. F. (1977). Ritual Management of Salmonid Fish Resources in California. Journal of California Anthropology 4: 6-29.

Teit, J. A. (1912). Mythology of the Thompson Indians. Vol. VII, Part II. The Jesup North Pacific Expedition. In Boas, F. (ed.), Memoirs of the American Museum of Natural History, G.E. Stechert, New York.

Thornton, T. F. (1999). Tleikwaani, The "Berried" Landscape: The Structure of Tlingit Edible Fruit Resources at Glacier Bay, Alaska. Journal of Ethnobiology 19(1): 27-48.

Touchie, B. (1977). Stealing Daylight. Nitinaht. International Journal of American Linguistics, Native American Text Series 2(3): 69-97.

Tucker, B. (2003). Comment on Hunn et al., Huna Tlingit Traditional Environmental Knowledge, Conservation, and the Management of a "Wilderness" Park. Current Anthropology 44:S98.

Turner, N. J. (2003). "Passing on the News": Women's Work, Traditional Knowledge and Plant Resource Management in Indigenous Societies of NW N. America. In Howard, P. (ed.), Women and Plants: Case Studies on Gender Relations in Local Plant Genetic Resource Management, Zed Books, UK, pp. 133-149.

Turner, N. J. (2004). Plants of Haida Gwaii. xaadlaa gwaay guud gina k'aws (Skidegate), xaadlaa gwaayee guud ginn k'aws (Massett). Sono Nis, Winlaw, BC.

Turner, N. J. (2005). The Earth's Blanket. Traditional Teachings for Sustainable Living, Douglas & McIntyre, Vancouver, BC and University of Washington, Seattle.

Turner, N. J., and Atleo, E. R. (Chief Umeek). (1998). Pacific North American First Peoples and the Environment. In Coward, Harold (ed.), Traditional and Modern Approaches to the Environment on the Pacific Rim, Tensions and Values, Centre for Studies in Religion and Society, State University of New York, Albany, pp. 105-124.

Turner, N. J., and Loewen, D. C. (1998). The Original "Free Trade": Exchange of Botanical Products and Associated Plant Knowledge in Northwestern North America. Anthropologica XL (1998): 49-70.

Turner, N. J., and Peacock, S. (2005). Solving the Perennial Paradox: Ethnobotanical Evidence for Plant Resource Management on the Northwest Coast. In Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.), "Keeping it Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation on the Northwest Coast of North America, University of Washington Press, Seattle and UBC Press, Vancouver, pp. 101-150.

Turner, N. J., Ignace, M. B., and Ignace, R. (2000). Traditional Ecological Knowledge and Wisdom of Aboriginal Peoples in British Columbia. Ecological Applications 10(5): 1275-1287.

Turner, N. J., Davidson-Hunt, I. J., and O'Flaherty, M. (2003). Living on the Edge: Ecological and Cultural Edges as Sources of Diversity for Social-Ecological Resilience. Human Ecology 31(3): 439-463.

Turner, N. J., Smith, R. Y., and Jones, J. T. (2005). "A fine line between two nations": Ownership Patterns for Plant Resources among Northwest Coast Indigenous Peoples--Implications for Plant Conservation and Management. In Deur, D., and Turner, N. J. (eds.), "Keeping it Living": Traditions of Plant Use and Cultivation on the Northwest Coast of North America, University of Washington Press, Seattle and UBC Press, Vancouver, pp. 151-180.

Turner, N. J., Thomas, J., Carlson, B. F., and Ogilvie, R. T. (1983). Ethnobotany of the Nitinaht Indians of Vancouver Island. British Columbia Provincial Museum Occasional Paper No. 24, Victoria.

Wall, S. (1993). Vickie Downey, Tewa, Tesuque Pueblo, quoted in: Wisdom's Daughters. Conversations with Women Elders of Native America, Harper Collins, New York.

Williams, N. M., and Baines, G. (1988). Traditional Ecological Knowledge. Wisdom for Sustainable Development, Centre for Resource and Environmental Studies, Australian National University, Canberra.

Williams, N. M., and Hunn, E. S. (eds.) (1982). Resource Managers: North American and Australian Hunter-Gatherers, American Association for the Advancement of Science, Washington, District of Columbia.

Wilson, J. A., Acheson, J. M., Metcalfe, M., and Kleban, P. (1994). Chaos, Complexity and Communal Management of Fisheries. Marine Policy 18: 291-305.

Young, E., Ross, H., Johnson, J., and Kesteven, J. (1991). Caring for Country. Aborigines and Land Management, Australian National Parks and Wildlife Service, Canberra.

N. J. Turner

School of Environmental Studies, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia V8W 2Y2, Canada e-mail: [email protected]

F. Berkes

Natural Resources Institute, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba R3T 2N2, Canada
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有