Cork! The effects of positive and negative self-talk on dart throwing performance.
Van Raalte, Judy L. ; Brewer, Britton W. ; Lewis, Brian P. 等
In the applied sport psychology literature, much attention has been
focused on the benefits of positive self-talk arid the deleterious effects of negative self-talk (Gould, Hodge, Peterson, & Giannini,
1989; Mahoney & Avener, 1977). Proponents of positive self-talk have
suggested that positive self-talk can reduce anxiety, increase effort,
and enhance self-confidence (Finn, 1985; Weinberg, 1988). Concerns about
the harmful effects of negative self-talk have led to the implementation
of techniques such as cognitive restructuring, thought stopping, and
countering to reduce the occurrence of negative self-talk (Bunker,
Williams, & Zinsser, 1993; Weinberg, 1988). Psychological skills
training packages that include positive self-talk as an important
component have been developed (Kendall, Hrycaiko, Martin, & Kendall,
1990; Weinberg, 1988). However, direct experimental evidence for the
role of self-talk in performance is limited.
Indirect assessments of positive and negative self-talk in
conjunction with other strategies have resulted in equivocal findings in
the sport literature. Rotella, Gansneder, Oljala, and Billing (1980)
found that more successful elite skiers did not differ from less
successful skiers in terms of the content of self-talk; Mahoney and
Avener (1977) found that elite gymnasts who qualified for the Olympic
team reported that they used self-talk significantly more in training
and competition than nonqualifiers. Highlen and Bennett (1983) explored
the self-talk of competitive wrestlers and divers and found that
wrestlers who qualified for the Pan American Games used critical
self-talk during competition more often than athletes who did not
qualify. Divers who qualified for the Pan American Games reported that
they used more content based self-instructions during competition and
less positive self-talk than did nonqualifiers. Surprisingly, these
results suggest that negative self-talk is associated with better, or at
least no worse, performance than positive self-talk.
Most sport researchers conducting experimental research on the
effects of self-talk on sport performance have compared positive
self-talk to other cognitive strategies. For example, Weinberg, Smith,
Jackson, and Gould (1984) compared the effects of positive self-talk,
associative, and dissociative strategies on two endurance activities, a
30-minute endurance run and a novel leg lift task. Results indicated no
significant performance differences between groups on the endurance run.
On the leg lift task, however, subjects in the positive self-talk and
dissociation conditions held their legs out significantly longer than
the subjects in the association condition. Weinberg (1985) used the same
leg lift task to examine the relationship between self-efficacy and
various cognitive strategies. As in the Weinberg et al. (1984) study,
Weinberg (1985) found no significant performance differences between
positive self-talk and dissociation conditions.
In the only published study to examine experimentally the effects of
both positive and negative self-talk on sport performance, Dagrou,
Gauvin, and Halliwell (1992) had undergraduate students complete
baseline dart throws, randomly assigned them to self-talk conditions,
then had them complete another group of dart throws. Results indicated
that subjects who were asked to verbalize positive self-talk performed
significantly better on the next group of dart throws than control
condition subjects, who performed significantly better than subjects who
were asked to verbalize negative self-talk. Dagrou et al. (1992)
concluded that self-talk influences sport performance such that positive
self-talk is associated with better performance than negative self-talk.
Replication of these results is warranted particularly because the
results of this experimental research differ from the results of
self-report studies.
Outside the sport literature the effects of positive and negative
self-talk on task performance have been examined in several studies.
Goodhart (1986) found that negative self-talk led to better performance
than positive self-talk on an anagram task. She suggested that the
effects of self-talk, in this case, may be primarily motivational.
Subjects who used negative self-talk were stimulated to avoid a negative
outcome and so tried harder than subjects who were using positive
self-talk. Schillf, Monroe, Evanst and Ramanaiah (1978) randomly
assigned male subjects to positive, negative or control self-talk
conditions. Subjects using positive self-talk made significantly fewer
errors on a mirror tracing task and completed the task significantly
faster than subjects in the control and negative self-talk conditions.
Sport research on the effects of self-talk has suggested that
positive self-talk leads to better performances than negative self-talk
(Dagrou et al., 1992). We attempted to replicate and extend the findings
of Dagrou et al. (1992) and therefore hypothesized that positive
self-talk condition subjects would perform significantly better on a
dart throwing task than negative self-talk condition subjects. Due to
the potential motivating effect of negative self-talk (Goodhart, 1986),
it was also hypothesized that negative self-talk condition subjects
would expect to do better on a future dart throwing task than positive
self-talk condition subjects.
Method
Subjects
Male undergraduates (N = 60) ranging in age from 18 to 22 years
volunteered to participate in this research. Male subjects were used to
allow comparisons with previous research that used primarily male
subjects (Dagrou et al., 1992).
Materials
Darts were thrown at a regulation board (35.5 cm diameter) located
seven feet from the target line at a height of six feet. The dad board
was covered with newspaper, with a round hole indicating the center of
the target. Following completion of each subject's throws, the
newspaper was removed and the distance of each dart throw hole from the
bull's-eye was measured. Throws that missed the dart board
completely were counted as 17.75 cm, the maximum distance from the
bull's-eye to the edge of the dart board.
The Post Dart Questionnaire (PDQ) consisted of a manipulation check
item, "What were you thinking about (or saying to yourself) while
you were throwing darts?", and an item designed to assess
subjects' expectations of future performance on a similar task,
"If you were to try this dart throwing task again, how would you
expect to do?" (1 = much worse, 11 = much better).
Procedure
Upon their arrival in the laboratory, subjects were randomly assigned
to control, positive, or negative self-talk conditions with the
constraint that an equal number of subjects was assigned to each
condition (Aronson, Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Gonzalez, 1990).
Subjects were instructed to try to hit the bull's-eye in 15
practice and 15 experimental dart throws (pilot testing indicated that
15 practice throws allowed subjects to gain experience without becoming
bored with the task). Prior to the dart throws, control condition
subjects were given no particular self-talk instructions, positive
self-talk condition subjects were asked to say, "you can do
it" before each throw, and negative self-talk condition subjects
were asked to say, "you cannot do it" before each throw. These
phrases were chosen because the former phrase was the one used most
frequently by pilot subjects and the latter phrase represented its
negation. After the dart throwing was completed, subjects completed the
PDQ and were thoroughly debriefed as to the nature of the experiment
Results
Manipulation Check
Subjects' responses to the question "What were you thinking
about (or saying to yourself) while you were throwing darts?" were
coded by three independent raters on a scale of 1 to 3 (1 = negative
self-talk, 2 = neutral self-talk, 3 = positive self-talk). The three
raters agreed on 94% of subjects' responses. For the remaining 6%
of the responses, the majority opinion (two out of three raters agreed)
was used to code the data. ANOVA indicated that there were significant
differences among conditions in terms of self-reported thoughts, F(2,58)
= 28.64, p [less than] .001. Planned contrasts indicated that the
positive self-talk condition subjects (M = 2.79) reported significantly
more positive self-talk than the control condition subjects (M = 2.10),
t(56) = 3.38, p [less than] .01, and negative self-talk condition
subjects (M = 1.25), t(56) = 7.55, p [less than] .01. Control condition
subjects reported significantly more positive self-talk than the
negative self-talk condition subjects, t(56) = 4.22, p [less than] .01.
Dart Throwing Accuracy
ANOVA indicated differences in dart throwing accuracy (cm from the
bull's-eye) among conditions, F(2,59) = 4.73, p [less than] .02.
Planned contrasts (means and standard deviations of dart throwing
performance are presented in Table 1) showed that the positive self-talk
condition subjects performed significantly better on the dad throwing
task than did negative self-talk condition subjects, t(57) = 3.03, p
[less than] .005, and control condition subjects, t(57) = -1.98, p =
.05. Control condition subjects and negative self-talk condition
subjects did not differ in terms of dart throwing accuracy, t(57) = 1.05
p[greater than].29.
Expectations for Future Dart Throwing Performance
In terms of expectations for future dart throwing performance, group
differences were found using ANOVA, F(2,58) = 3.35, p [less than] .05.
Means and standard deviations for expectations for future performance
are presented in Table 1. As revealed by planned contrasts, the negative
self-talk group reported that they expected to improve significantly
more on a future dart throwing task than did control condition subjects,
t(56) = 2.33, p [less than] .03, and positive self-talk condition
subjects, t(56) = 2.13, p [less than] .04.
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviation of Performance and Expectations for
Future Dart Throwing Performance
Condition Dart Future
Performance Expectations
Positive Self-Talk
M 69.75 6.68
SD (22.58) (1.16)
ControI
M 84.75 6.60
SD (24.48) (1.54)
Negative Self-Talk
M 92.75 7.75
SD (25.88) (1.89)
Note: Lower performance scores indicate greater throwing accuracy
(shorter distance in cm from the bull's-eye).
Discussion
The results of this research replicate those of Dagrou et al. (1992)
and suggest that self-talk is an important variable in sport
performance. Subjects who used positive self-talk performed
significantly better on the dart-throwing task than did subjects who
used negative self-talk. The results for future performance expectation
indicate that negative self-talk can serve as a motivating factor in
performance. Nevertheless, further research in this area is needed
before conclusions can be drawn. For example, it would be important to
determine if subjects who use negative self-talk actually work harder on
a task in the long run or if they just report that they are motivated to
perform much better on a similar task in the future.
There are threats to the generalizability of these results to actual
sport tasks. The subjects in this investigation may have had some
experience throwing darts, but were performing a specific task with
novel requirements. Responses may be different on more familiar tasks.
It is possible that self-talk has less of an impact on performance of a
task when the parameters of the task are well known. It should also be
noted that the subjects in this investigation were all male. Dagrou et
al. (1992) used predominantly male subjects, and they did not make
gender comparisons. Spink (1990) found no gender differences in the
self-reported use of self-talk. Further research exploring the effects
of both naturally occurring and randomly assigned self-talk for male and
female subjects is needed.
It would also be interesting to determine the prevalence of overt positive and negative self-talk in actual sporting events. Through
behavioral observation, it would be possible to assess how much overt
positive and negative self-talk actually occurs. During a tennis
tournament, for example, one could assess the relationship between
self-talk and tennis performance.
The findings of this study and those of Dagrou et al. (1992) support
the utility of positive self-talk interventions. Given the responses of
subjects to the future performance item, further attention to the
potential motivating effects of negative self-talk is warranted.
References
Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Gonzalez, M.
H. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology (2nd ed.). New York:
McGraw Hill.
Barren, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual,
strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Bunker, L., Williams, J. M., & Zinsser, N. (1993). Cognitive
techniques for improving performance and building confidence. In J. M.
Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak
performance (2nd ed.) (pp. 225-242). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Dagrou, E., Gauvin, L., & Halliwell, W. (1992). Effets du langage
positif, negatif, et neutre sur la performance motrice. Canadian Journal
of Sport Sciences, 17, 145-147.
Finn, J. A. (1985). Competitive excellence: It's a matter of
mind and body. The Physician and Sportsmedicine, 13, 61-75.
Goodhart, D. E. (1986). The effects of positive and negative thinking
on performance in an achievement situation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 117-124.
Gould, D., Hodge, K., Peterson, K., & Giannini, J. (1989). An
exploratory examination of strategies used by elite coaches to enhance
self-efficacy in athletes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
11, 128-140.
Highlen, P. S., & Bennett, B. B. (1983). Elite divers and
wrestlers: A comparison between open- and closed-skill athletes. Journal
of Sport Psychology, 5, 390-409.
Kendall, G., Hrycaiko, D., Martin, G. L., & Kendall, T. (1990).
The effects of an imagery rehearsal, relaxation, and self-talk package
on basketball game performance. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 12, 157-166.
Mahoney, M. J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology or the elite
athlete: An exploratory study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1,
135-141.
Rotella, R. J., Gansneder, B., Oljala, D., & Billings, J. (1980).
Cognitions and coping strategies of elite skiers: An exploratory study
of young developing athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 350-354.
Schill, T., Monroe, S., Evans, R., & Ramanaiah, N. (1978). The
effects of self-verbalizations on performance: A test of the
rational-emotive position. Psychotherapy: Theory. Research and Practice,
15, 2-7.
Spink, K. (1990). Gender differences in psychological strategies
among talented swimmers: An exploratory study. Australian Journal of
Science and Medicine in Sport, 22, 68-70.
Weinberg, R. S. (1985). Relationship between self-efficacy and
cognitive strategies in enhancing endurance performance. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 17, 280-292.
Weinberg, R. S. (1988). The mental advantage: Developing your
psychological skills in tennis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Weinberg, R. S., Smith, J., Jackson, A., & Gould, D. (1984).
Effect of association, dissociation, and positive self-talk strategies
on endurance performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 9,
25-32.
These data were presented at the annual meeting of the American
Psychological Association, August 1992, Washington DC. We would like to
thank Amanda Daley and Jill Philips for their assistance in data coding,
LeThuy Hoang for her abstract translation, and Jennifer Rojohn for her
inspiration.