Comparative concurrent validity of the MRF-L and ARS competitive state Anxiety Rating Scales for volleyball and basketball.
Cox, Richard H. ; Russell, William D. ; Robb, Marshall 等
Measurement of competitive state anxiety during and before
competition has suffered from a number of measurement-related
limitations. Martens (1982) and Landers (1980) disagreed on the best way
to measure competitive state anxiety. Martens argued that a pencil and
paper assessment of state anxiety was as accurate and reliable as a
physiological assessment. Landers, however, argued that both
physiological and a psychological assessment is necessary. These two
divergent opinions are complicated by the fact that low correlations
exist among physiological measures of state anxiety and between
psychological and physiological measures (Karteroliotis & Gill,
1987; Landers, 1980). These early observations of Martens and Landers
remain as viable today as they were in the early 1980's. Further
compounding this complex problem is the observation that measurement of
state anxiety prior to competition may be different than state anxiety
measured during competition. It is very difficult, however, to get
accurate estimates of competitive state anxiety during competition. An
exception to this general observation may include individual sports such
as golf, diving, and bowling where delays of various lengths occur
between strokes, dives, and rolls. It is more difficult to imagine
assessing state anxiety during competition involving a team sport such
as volleyball or basketball, but if the measurement instrument was
unobtrusive, it would be possible.
Spielberger's State Anxiety Inventory (SAI) has been utilized
by many researchers to obtain an estimate of precompetitive state
anxiety in athletes (Spielberger, 1983; Spielberger, Gorsuch, &
Lushene, 1970). In an effort to shorten the time necessary to administer
the SAI, Martens (1977) developed the Competitive State Anxiety
Inventory (CSAI), which is a shortened sport specific version of the
SAI. Use of the CSAI was short lived, however, due to the development of
multidimensional tests of state anxiety such as the Competitive State
Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2).
The CSAI-2 provided a measurement of both cognitive and somatic competitive state anxiety (as well as self-confidence). This distinction
between cognitive and somatic state anxiety has been pivotal in helping
sport psychologists better understand the relationship between anxiety
and performance (Cox, 1998). Specifically, this distinction is based on
a multidimensional theory of competitive state anxiety from which the
CSAI-2 was developed. Multidimensional theory posits that a distinction
needs to be made between cognitive state anxiety and somatic state
anxiety, and that their relative independence warrants separate
measurement (Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump, & Smith, 1990). While
psychometrically sound, the CSAI-2 is 27 items in length and does not
lend itself to measurement of anxiety immediately prior to an at bat in
softball or a stroke in golf. The typical administration period for the
CSAI-2 is 15-30 minutes before competition and the estimated test
completion time is approximately 5 minutes. While this time frame is
sufficient for precompetitive measures, it is rare that the athlete has
this time available during competition for assessment of state anxiety.
Hanin (1986) proposed a retrospective method of determining state
anxiety. Using retrospective techniques athletes are asked to recall
their level of state anxiety several days after a specific event. If
reliable, this technique would allow researchers to unobtrusively obtain
precompetitive state anxiety. Harger and Raglin (1994) reported that
collegiate track and field athletes can accurately recall precompetitive
state anxiety two days after the initial event. It is not known if the
retrospective technique could be utilized to recall state anxiety during
competition. To clarify the relationship between state anxiety and
performance, McAuley (1985) made an early recommendation that future
researchers attempt to assess state anxiety during competition. In an
effort to address this recommendation, Murphy, Greenspan, Jowdy, and
Tammen (1989) developed the Mental Readiness Form (MRF). As an
alternative to the CSAI-2, the MRF was designed to assess precompetitive
state anxiety through a 3-item instrument employing a visual analog
scale. The three parts of the MRF correspond to cognitive anxiety,
somatic anxiety, and self-confidence subscales of the CSAI-2. Krane
(1994) replaced the visual analog scale in the original MRF with an
11-point Likert scale and called it the MRF-Likert (MRF-L). Krane also
developed the MRF-3 which used the bipolar opposites of worried-not
worried, tense-not tense, and confident-not confident to represent
cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence. Krane concluded
that all three versions of the MRF exhibit concurrent validity when
compared with the CSAI-2, but that the MRF-L and MRF-3 versions were
preferred to the original MRF because of clarity of response gained when
using a Likert scale. Her data also revealed higher correlations with
CSAI-2 subscales when the MRF-L was used as opposed to either the MRF or
the MRF-3. She reported a simple correlation of .76 between the MRF-L
and corresponding CSAI-2 cognitive subscale and .69 between the MRFL and
the corresponding CSAI-2 somatic subscale.
While the Mental Readiness Scale has emerged as a viable
alternative to the CSAI-2 when an unobtrusive measurement tool is
desired, Krane (1994) cautions that it should not be considered a viable
replacement for the more psychometrically sound CSAI-2. It should also
be pointed out that the MRF was not developed in any psychometric sense
from the CSAI-2 and cannot be technically considered a short form of the
CSAI-2. The anchors, however, as utilized in both the MRF-L and MRF-3
appear to be theoretically selected to represent the measurement of
cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence.
Somewhat concurrent with the development of the MRF-L and the
MRF-3, Cox, Russell, and Robb (1998) reported the development of the
Anxiety Rating Scale (ARS). The ARS was developed directly from the
CSAI-2 and utilizes two brief statements from the parent test to measure
cognitive and somatic anxiety. The ARS was developed using stepwise multiple regression techniques and follow-up concurrent validity checks
with the parent CSAI-2 test. Both the MRF-L and ARS independently
exhibit concurrent validity coefficients with the CSAI-2 in the
magnitude of .60 to .84. It is not known, however, which of the two
rating scales would be most predictive of CSAI-2 subscales in the same
competitive environment with the same subjects. The purpose of the
present investigation, therefore, was to compare the concurrent validity
of the MRF-L and ARS in the same competitive environment with the same
participants. Because the ARS was originally developed and tested
utilizing a sample of 771 volleyball and basketball intramural athletes,
it was hypothesized that the current independent sample of volleyball
and basketball athletes would yield validity coefficients favoring the
ARS over the MRF-L.
Method
Participants
Participants for this research were 418 male and female intramural
athletes competing in volleyball or basketball at a large Midwest
University. Two-hundred and twenty-four of the subjects competed in
volleyball while 194 competed in basketball. The average age of the 211
male athletes was 20.3 yr (SD = 1.96), while the average age of the 207
female athletes was 19.2 yr (SD = 1.11). Use of human subjects for the
purpose of research was approved by the appropriate University human
subject's committee and anonymity assured.
Instruments
The instruments used for collecting anxiety data were the
Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2), the Anxiety Rating Scale
(ARS), and the Mental Readiness Form (MRFL). The CSAI-2 is composed of
27-items that measure cognitive state anxiety, somatic state anxiety,
and self-confidence. Details associated with the development and testing
of the CSAI-2 are reported by Martens, Vealey, and Burton (1990).
The MRF-L (Krane, 1994) is composed of three brief statements
associated with an 11 point Likert scale. The three brief statements,
associated with cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence
respectively, are "my thoughts are", my body feels", and
"I am feeling." The anchors for the "my thoughts
are" cognitive anxiety statement were calm and worried. The anchors
for the "my body feels" somatic anxiety statement were relaxed
and tense. Finally, the anchors for the "I am feeling"
self-confidence statement were confident and scared. A high score for
self-confidence in the CSAI-2 indicates high self-confidence, whereas a
high score for self-confidence on the MRF-L indicates low
self-confidence. Thus, the two scales will be negatively correlated.
The ARS (Cox et al., 1998) is composed of three brief statements
associated with a seven point Likert scale. The three brief statements
in the ARS are as follows:
Cognitive Anxiety - I feel concerned about performing poorly and
that others will be disappointed with my performance.
Somatic Anxiety - I feel nervous, my body feels tight and/or my
stomach tense.
Self-Confidence(1) - I feel secure, mentally relaxed, and confident
of coming through under pressure.
The Likert scale associated with each of these statements runs from
1 (not at all) to 7 (intensely so). Each statement represents an
aggregate of three items from the CSAI-2. The three items that make up
each statement were selected on the basis of maximum-[R.sup.2] (SAS)
stepwise multiple regression procedures. Thus, the three selected items
(out of nine) were those items that together contributed the most to
predicting the CSAI-2 subscale for cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety,
or self-confidence. In each case, the three selected items represented
the best three variable model in terms of maximizing the squared
multiple correlation. Intercorrelations among the three selected somatic
anxiety items ranged from .441 to .545, while for cognitive anxiety and
self-confidence the range was .215 to .517 and .409 to .505
respectively. While issues related to collinearity are of concern when
analyzing effects, they pose little problem when the researcher's
sole purpose is in the determination and interpretation of the squared
multiple correlation (Pedhazur, 1997). While the concurrent validity of
the ARS is being addressed in the current investigation, the test-retest
reliability of the ARS was reported to be .97 for cognitive state
anxiety, .90 for somatic state anxiety, and .95 for self-confidence
(Cox, Davis, & Robb, 1998).
Procedures
During round-robin and play-off volleyball and basketball
competition, starting members of selected teams completed the MRF-L,
ARS, and CSAI-2 approximately 15 minutes before match/game. During
round-robin competition, records were kept so that selected teams were
tested only once. During the play-off games, it was not possible to
avoid a repeated measurement situation, in which an athlete was measured
more that once. Measurements were considered to be independent of each
other, however, because of the varying conditions that prevail prior to
each game (Turner & Raglin, 1996). The order in which the MRF-L and
ARS were administered to athlete's was counterbalanced to eliminate
an order bias. For example, of the I 13 male volleyball players tested,
approximately half of them completed the MRF-L before the ARS and the
other half completed the ARS before the MRF-L. The CSAI-2 was always
administered last. Participants were briefed regarding the nature of
inventories, asked to read questions carefully and respond in like
manner. Observations were obtained from 117 males prior to round-robin
games, 110 females prior to round-robin games, 94 males prior to
play-off games, and 97 females prior to play-off games.
Statistical Analyses
Correlation techniques were utilized to determine the relative
concurrent validity of the ARS and MRF-L with the CSAI-2. A t-test for
comparing two dependent correlations was utilized to compare r's
associated with the ARS and the MRF-L (Bruning & Kintz, 1987). In
order to ascertain the influence of gender, situation (round-robin,
play-off), and sport upon the ability of the ARS or MRF-L to predict
subscales associated with the CSAI-2, they were included in multiple
regression analyses.
Results
Correlational relationships between MRF-L and CSAI-2 subscales, and
between ARS and CSAI-2 subscales are illustrated in Table 1. As can be
observed in Table 1, correlation results are compared for all 418
subjects combined, but also for sub-groups as a function of gender,
situation (round-robin vs. play-off games), and sport. Sub-group
breakdowns provide insight into comparative relationships. In the case
of cognitive state anxiety, the ARS consistently exhibits higher
correlations with the CSAI-2 subscale for cognitive anxiety than does
the MRF-L. The single exception to this observation is in the play-off
game situation. In the case of somatic state anxiety, the ARS again
consistently exhibits higher correlations with the CSAI-2 subscale for
somatic anxiety than does the MRF-L. The single exception to this
observation was in the case of the female participants. In the case of
self-confidence, however, the MRF-L exhibits higher correlations with
the CSAI-2 subscale for self-confidence than does the ARS. The single
exception to this observation was in the case of male intramural
athletes.
Also noted in Table 1 are the results of dependent t-tests between
ARS and MRF-L correlations with the CSAI-2. The results of these
comparisons revealed five significant differences at the .05 level and
one at the .01 level. All but one of these differences favored the ARS
over the MRF-L. The differences for cognitive and somatic anxiety
favored the ARS, while the single difference for self-confidence favored
the MRF-L.
In order to ascertain the influence of gender, situation
(round-robin, play-off), and sport upon the ability of the MRF-L or ARS
to predict competitive state anxiety as measured by the CSAI-2, they
were included in a multiple regression analysis. Utilizing either the
MRF-L or the ARS as the single continuous property predictor variable,
and gender, situation, and sport as categorical predictor variables, six
separate regression models were tested as illustrated in [TABULAR DATA
FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] Table 2. These regression analyses provide insight
as to how gender, situation, and sport interact with the MRF-L or ARS in
predicting subscales of the CSAI-2. As can be observed in Table 2, the
MRF-L or ARS scores were significant predictors of subscales of the
CSAI-2 in all six models. Gender was revealed to be a significant
parameter for predicting somatic state anxiety for both the ARS and the
MRF-L. As gender switches from male to female, either a 1.57 (MRF-L) or
1.12 (ARS) point reduction in somatic anxiety is predicted (as measured
by the CSAI-2).
Discussion
As previously stated, the purpose of this investigation was to
compare the concurrent validity of the MRF-L and ARS in the same
competitive environment with the same participants. Because the ARS was
originally developed and tested utilizing a sample of volleyball and
basketball intramural athletes, it was hypothesized that the current
independent sample of volleyball and basketball athletes would yield
validity coefficients favoring the ARS over the MRF-L.
Results revealed that for all participants combined, the ARS is a
stronger predictor of cognitive and somatic state anxiety as measured by
the CSAI-2, while for confidence, the MRF-L is a stronger predictor.
based upon the results of this study it may be concluded that the ARS
provides a more accurate assessment of state anxiety in basketball and
volleyball players than does the MRF-L. Use of the ARS will make it
possible to ascertain an athlete's precompetitive state anxiety at
a point in time closer to the actual event, and to measure state anxiety
during actual competition. Krane, Joyce, and Rafeld(1994) and Durr
(1996) provide illustrations of how this can be accomplished in research
involving softball batters and 3-meter board divers.
Development and testing of short inventories capable of accurately
measuring anxiety and self-confidence at a point in time contiguous with
actual performance seems critical in the ultimate understanding of the
effects of affect on performance. Much work still remains to be done,
however. The current investigation suggests that the ARS is more
accurate than the MRF-L relative to the measurement of somatic and
cognitive state anxiety in selected team sport athletes (concurrent
validity). Results of research reported by Cox, Reed, and Robb (1996)
suggest that the same is true for team sport athletes.
The construct validity of the ARS must also be addressed. Athletes,
for example, who have been exposed to psychological skills training
should be better able to control their state anxiety prior to an
important competition. The ARS, or for that matter the MRF-L, should be
able to distinguish between a group of athletes who have experienced
psychological skills training and a group who has not.
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 2 OMITTED]
Note
1 The Cox et al. (1998) article chronicling the development of the
ARS did not include measurement of self-confidence, although data were
collected on this variable. Results of a stepwise regression procedure
identified three CSAI-2 self-confidence items that together accounted
for 86% of the variability of the CSAI-2 self-confidence subscale. These
three were items 12 (I feel secure), 21 (I feel mentally relaxed), and
27 (I'm confident of coming through under pressure). As with
cognitive and somatic anxiety, these three items were condensed into a
single aggregated statement and set to a 7-point Likert scale in the
current research.
References
Bruning, J.L., & Kintz, B.L. (1987). Computational handbook of
statistics (3rd ed.). Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.
Cox, R.H. (1998). Sport psychology: Concepts and applications (4th
ed.). Dubuque, IA: WCB/McGraw-Hill.
Cox, R.H., Russell, W.D., & Robb, M. (1998). Development of a
CSAI-2 Short form for assessing competitive state anxiety during and
immediately prior to competition. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, 30-40.
Cox, R.H., Reed, C., & Robb, M. (1996). Comparative concurrent
validity of the MRF-L and ARS competitive state anxiety rating scales
for individual sport athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 8,
Supplement, S23.
Cox, R.H., Davis, J.E., & Robb, M. (1998). Test-retest
reliability of the anxiety rating scale. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 69, Supplement, Abstract 111.
Durr, K.R. (1996). Relationship between state anxiety and
performance in high school divers. Unpublished master's thesis,
University of Missouri, Columbia.
Hanin, Y.L. (1986). State - trait anxiety research on sports in the
U.S.S.R. In C.D. Spielberger, & R. Dias-Guerrero (Editors), Cross -
cultural anxiety (pp. 45 - 64). Washington D.C.: Hemisphere
Publications.
Harger, G.J., & Raglin, J.S. (1994). Correspondence between
actual and recalled precompetition anxiety in collegiate track and field
athletes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 16, 206-211.
Karteroliotis, C., & Gill, D.L. (1987). Temporal changes in
psychological and physiological components of state anxiety. Journal of
Sport Psychology, 9, 261-274.
Krane, V. (1994). The mental readiness form as a measure of
competitive state anxiety. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 189-202.
Krane, V., Joyce, D., & Rafeld, J. (1994). Competitive anxiety,
situation criticality, and softball performance. The Sport Psychologist,
8, 58-72.
Landers, D.M. (1980). The arousal - performance relationship
revisited. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 51, 77-90.
Martens, R. (1977). Sport competition anxiety test. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics Publishers.
Martens, R. (1982). Sport competition anxiety test(2nd ed.).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R.S., Bump, L.A., & Smith, D.
(1990). Development and validation of the competitive state anxiety
inventory-2. In R. Martens, R.S. Vealey, & D. Burton (Eds.),
Competitive anxiety in sport (pp. 117-190). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics Books.
Martens, R., Vealey, R.S., & Burton, D. (1990). Competitive
anxiety in sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.
McAuley, E. (1985). State - anxiety: Antecedent or result of sport
performance. Journal of Sport Behavior, 8, 71 - 77.
Murphy, S.M., Greenspan, M., Jowdy, D., & Tammen, V. (1989).
Development of a brief rating instrument of competitive anxiety:
Comparisons with the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. Proceedings
of the Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology
(p.82). Seattle, WA.
Pedhazur, E.J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research
(3rd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace.
Spielberger, C.D. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety
inventory (Form-Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.F. (1970).
Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychological Press.
Turner, P.E., & Raglin, J.S. (1996). Variability in
precompetition anxiety and performance in college track and field
athletes. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 28, 378-385.