Concurrent Validity of the Revised Anxiety Rating Scale.
Cox, Richard H. ; Robb, Marshall ; Russell, William D. 等
The Anxiety Rating Scale (ARS) is a shortened version of the
Competitive State Anxiety Inventory - 2 (CSAI-2). Subsequent use of the
ARS led to the suggestion that minor modifications in the ARS might
enhance the concurrent validity of the instrument. It was the purpose of
this investigation to compare the concurrent validity of the original
ARS with two revisions of the ARS. Participants in the investigation
were 180 college age intramural volleyball participants. Results
revealed that for females, revision two yielded the largest correlations
with CSAI-2 subscales; for males, revision two again yielded the largest
correlations relative to cognitive and somatic anxiety, but not for
self-confidence. Collapsing across gender revision two yielded the
highest correlations for cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and
self-confidence. These correlation coefficients were .67, .69 and .75
respectively. It was concluded that revision two exhibited the highest
level of concurrent validity when research participants wer e intramural
volleyball players.
The Anxiety Rating Scale (ARS) was developed by Cox, Russell and
Robb (1998, 1999) as a short rating scale version of the Competitive
State Anxiety Inventory - 2 (CSAI-2) (Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump,
& Smith, 1990). The ARS was not merely conceptualized as a short
version of the CSAI-2 with fewer items devoted to each anxiety
construct, but as a single statement rating scale that would allow an
athlete to quickly rate how they felt in terms of precompetitive
cognitive state anxiety, somatic state anxiety, and self-confidence. In
this regard, it was patterned after the Borg Scale (1973) designed to
estimate perceived exertion during exercise. Evidence of concurrent
validity of the ARS with the CSAI-2 has been provided with team sport
intramural athletes (Cox et al., 1998, 1999) and with individual sport
intramural athletes (Cox, Reed, & Robb, 1997). Results of these
investigations have shown the ARS to be moderately correlated with
anxiety and self-confidence subcomponents of Martens' CSAI-2 (.60
to .70).
The ARS has been compared favorably with Krane's (1994)
modification of the Mental Readiness Form (MRF-L). The MRF-L is a short
version of the CSAI-2 originally developed by Murphy, Greenspan, Jowdy,
and Tammen (1989). Comparative correlations between the anxiety
components of the ARS and MRF-L with the cognitive and somatic state
anxiety subscales of the CSAI-2 have consistently been higher for the
ARS (Cox, Reed, & Robb, 1997; Cox et al., 1999). As originally
developed (Cox et al., 1998), the ARS measured competitive somatic state
anxiety and competitive cognitive state anxiety, but not state
self-confidence. State self-confidence was later added to the ARS (Cox
et al., 1999).
In crafting the statements associated with cognitive state anxiety,
somatic state anxiety, and state self-confidence, the developers of the
ARS used stepwise multiple regression procedures to identify the
"best" three items from the CASI-2 to represent each
psychological state. These CSAI-2 items were then crafted into aggregate
statements that represent cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and
self-confidence. The three aggregate statements were then set to a
7-point scale ranging from I (not at all) to 7 (intensely so), which
allowed an athlete to rate how she felt immediately prior to
competition. Respectively, the three statements for cognitive state
anxiety, somatic state anxiety, and self-confidence read as follows:
1. I feel concerned about performing poorly and that others will be
disappointed with my performance.
2. I feel nervous, my body feels tight and/or my stomach tense.
3. I feel secure, mentally relaxed, and confident of coming through
under pressure.
Anonymous critics have argued that each of these statements contain
more than one psychological construct or idea, and therefore the athlete
might be confused as to how to respond. For example, the third statment
contains the psychological constructs of "secure,"
"relaxed" and "confident." As the arguement goes,
the athlete might feel secure and confident, but not relaxed, and
therefore would not know how to respond to this statement on a 7-point
scale. Evidence suggests, however, that a respondent would simply
provide a "global" response as to how they felt relative to
feeling secure, relaxed, and confident (Cox, Russell & Robb, 1996).
In the developmental phase of the ARS, Cox et al., (1996) reported data
that showed that an aggregate statement that included three
psychological constructs in a single statement had a higher concurrent
correlation with the parent CSAI-2 subscale than a statement that
included only the element that had the highest simple correlation with
the CSAI-2 subscale. Specifically, the correlations between ARS measured
cognitive and somatic state anxiety and the corresponding CSAI-2
measured subscales were reported to be .717 and .761. The correlations
between the single cognitive anxiety CSAI-2 item "I'm concerned about performing poorly" and the single somatic anxiety
CSAI-2 item "I feel nervous" with the cognitive and somatic
subscales of the CSAI-2 were .497 and .630 respectively. This was taken
as evidence that an aggregated statement was superior to a single
unambiguous statement in predicting cognitive and somatic state anxiety
as measured by the CSAI-2.
A second criticism of the ARS is that some of the psychological
constructs that are included in each statement seem to cause some
confusion in the mind of the responder relative to whether it is a
cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, or self-confidence statement. For
example, the word "nervous" which appears in the somatic
anxiety statment might be interpreted by the respondent as a cognitive
state anxiety construct. And again, the phrase "mentally
relaxed" which appears in the self-confidence statement might be
interpreted by the respondent as a somatic anxiety statement. This
confusion is possible, despite the fact that the phrase "I feel
nervous" is scored as a somatic anxiety element in the CSAI-2
parent inventory, and the phrase "I feel mentally relaxed" is
scored as a self-confidence element in the CSAI-2 parent test.
It was the purpose of this investigation to develop revised
versions of the Anxiety Rating Scale, and to compare them with the
parent CSAI-2 for concurrent validity, in much the same way that the ARS
was compared with the MRF-L for concurrent validity (Cox, et al., 1999).
It was hypothesized that revised versions of the ARS would exhibit
larger concurrent validity coefficients with the CSAI-2 than the
original Anxiety Rating Scale (ARS).
Method
Participants
Participants for this research were 90 male and 90 female (N 180)
intramural athletes (largely undergraduates) competing in volleyball at
a large Midwest University. The average age of the male participants was
19.8 years (SD = 1.6) and the average of female participants was 19.3
years (SD = 1.4). Use of human subjects for the purpose of research was
approved by the appropriate University human subject's committee
and anonymity assured.
Instruments
The instruments used for collecting anxiety data were the
Competitive State Anxiety lnventory-2 (CSAI-2), the Anxiety Rating Scale
(ARS), and two revised versions of the ARS (ARS-l, ARS-2). The CSAI-2 is
composed of 27-items that measure cognitive state anxiety, somatic state
anxiety, and self-confidence. Details associated with the development
and testing of the CSAI-2 are reported by Martens, Vealey, and Burton
(1990). The Anxiety Rating Scale (ARS) was previously described in some
detail in the introduction of this article.
In addition to the CSAI-2 and the ARS, two revised versions of the
ARS were developed for testing. Details associated with the development
of the two revised versions of the ARS are described in the following
sections.
ARS revision one. The primary difference between the original ARS
and revision one (ARS-l) was in the replacement of the word
"nervous" with the word "jittery" in the somatic
state anxiety statement, and the replacement of the phrase
"mentally relaxed" with the phrase "I feel
comfortable" in the self-confidence statement. The word
"nervous" and the phrase "mentally relaxed" were
replaced because of their potential ambiguous meaning in the minds of
some respondents. Some respondents have reported that the word
"nervous" brings to mind thoughts that are more mental than
somatic. Similarly, some respondents have reported that the phrase
"mentally relaxed" brings to mind thoughts that are more
related to bodily tension/anxiety than to self-confidence.
Focusing upon these two words/phrases, we went back into the
original data set from which the ARS was developed (Cox et al., 1998) to
identify logical replacements. In the original data set the correlation
between the CSAI-2 subscale for somatic anxiety and the phrase "I
feel nervous" item was .778, while the same relationship for the
phrase "I feel jittery" was .777. Additionally, the
intercorrelation between these two phrases was relatively high, .69,
providing a possible explanation why both were not included in the best
three variable prediction model. Based upon this information, we
replaced the word "nervous" in the somatic state anxiety
statement of the ARS with the word "jittery." Similarly, we
went back into the original data set to find a replacement for the
phrase "mentally relaxed." In the original data set, the
correlation between the CSAI-2 subscale for self-confidence and the
"I feel mentally relaxed" item was .68, while the same
correlation with the "I feel comfortable" item was .71. The
two items were likely not included together in the best three variable
model because of the relatively high correlation between the two items
(r = .50) and their intercorrelations with other self-confidence items.
With these modifications, the statements that appear in ARS revision one
(ARS-1), with respect to cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and
self-confidence, are as follows:
1. I feel concerned about performing poorly and that others will be
disappointed with my performance.
2. I feel jittery, my body feels tight and/or my stomach tense.
3. I feel comfortable, secure, and confident of coming through
under pressure.
All three statements were anchored with a 7-point rating scale
ranging from I (not at all) to 7 (intensely so).
ARS revision two. Revision two differed significantly from the
original and the first revision. The ARS and ARS-l were both developed
and based on the data from which the ARS was originally developed. ARS
revision two (ARS-2), however, was developed with an entirely different
frame of reference. We looked at the nine items for each subcomponent of
the GSA I2 and selected three items that seemed to most logically be
related to cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, or self-confidence. Thus,
the ARS-2 was based on the collective judgement of the researchers. We
selected CSAI-2 items 13, 16 and 22 to form the basis of the single
aggregate statement for cognitive state anxiety; items 5,8 and 17 to
form the basis of the somatic state anxiety statement; and items 6, 12,
and 18 to form the basis of the self-confidence statement. The aggregate
statements that appear in ARS revision two (ARS-2), with respect to
cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence, were as
follows:
1. I feel concerned about performing poorly, choking under
pressure, and that others will be disappointed with my performance.
2. I feel jittery, my body feels tense, and my heart is racing.
3. I feel comfortable, secure, and confident about performing well.
As with the ARS and ARS-1, the ARS-2 was achored with a 7-point
rating scale.
Procedures
Starting members of intramural volleyball teams were approached
approximately 15 mm prior to a round-robin competition and asked if they
would participate in the study. Volunteers were given a pencil and an
inventory packet that included instructions, consent form, an ARS
version, and the CSAI-2, in that order. In completing the ARS and the
CSAI-2, they were asked to respond relative to how they felt at that
moment. The inventories were distributed to the athletes in random
order, so that each athlete was as likely to receive a packet containing
the ARS, ARS-l or ARS-2. Inventories were distributed in such a fashion,
that the ARS was administered to 30 men and 30 women, and the same for
the ARS- 1 and ARS-2. Teams were not approached for their participation
more than one time during round-robin competition.
Results
Correlation procedures were used to compare the concurrent validity
of participants who used the three different versions of the ARS. Each
reported correlation coefficient was based upon 30 participants, unless
gender was pooled, in which case each correlation was based upon 60
participants.
Correlation coefficients between CSAI-2 subscales and the three
versions of the Anxiety Rating Scale are illustrated in Table 1. In this
Table, data are displayed separately by gender, but also by collapsing
across gender. As can be observed in this Table, the highest concurrent
validity coefficients (correlations) for the females were obtained using
the ARS-2, for cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, as well as
self-confidence. The original version of the ARS produced the lowest
correlation coefficients among the three measures of anxiety and
confidence. Based upon this result, we might conclude that the ARS-2 is
the preferred version of the scale.
Focusing upon the male participants, however, we get a slightly
different result. Relative to cognitive and somatic state anxiety, the
ARS-2 still exhibits the highest correlations with CSAI-2 subscales, but
in the case of self-confidence, the original version of the ARS yielded
the highest correlation coefficient. Collapsing across gender, however,
we get a result that suggests that the ARS-2 is the preferred version
for predicting cognitive anxiety, somatic anxiety, and self-confidence
as measured by the CSAI-2.
Discussion
Three different versions of the Anxiety Rating Scale were compared
for concurrent validity with the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory -2
(CSAI-2). For the female participants, the results seem to demonstrate
the superiority of the ARS-2 over the ARS and the ARS-l for cognitive
state anxiety, somatic state anxiety and self-confidence. Although there
were some differences, this pattern of superiority remained generally
consistent for the male participants. For cognitive state anxiety, the
ARS-2 continued to exhibit a clear superiority over the ARS and ARS-1.
For somatic state anxiety, the ARS-2 exhibited a correlation coefficient
that was slightly larger than the correlation for the ARS, but clearly
larger than for the ARS-1. In the case of self-confidence, however, the
pattern changes suggesting that the ARS remains the best predictor of
this subscale of the CSAI-2. Collapsing the two samples together (males
and females), however, we again have a pattern of results that suggests
that the ARS-2 is the best p redictor of the CSAI-2 subscales of
cognitive state anxiety, somatic state anxiety, and self-confidence.
In this data set, using volleyball participants, the evidence seems
to suggest that the second revised version of the ARS (ARS-2) is the
preferred version. In terms of cognitive and somatic state anxiety, its
superiority over the original ARS and the ARS-1 is fairly consistent,
regardless of gender. In terms of self-confidence, the evidence favoring the ARS-2 is less convincing, although when the male and female samples
are pooled, it does exhibit the largest correlation coefficient. Based
upon these observations, it is concluded that the concurrent validity of
the Anxiety Rating Scale can be improved by modifying it consistent with
revision two (ARS-2).
In future investigations, it is recommended that the construct
validity of the ARS-2 be tested. Construct validity of the ARS-2 could
be established by demonstrating that groups of participants who
theoretically should differ on state anxiety and confidence, do in fact
differ on these psychological constructs as measured by the Anxiety
Rating Scale (Thomas & Nelson, 1996).
References
Borg, G.A.V. (1973). Perceived exertion: A note on
"history" and methods. Medicine and Science in Sports, 5,
90-93.
Cox, R.H., Reed, C., & Robb, M. (1997). Comparative validity of
the MRF-L and ARS competitive state anxiety rating scales for intramural
athletes competing in six individual sports. Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 68(1), Supplement, A-101.
Cox, R.H., Russell, W.D., & Robb, M. (1996). Five phases in the
development of an instrument for assessing competitive state anxiety
during and prior to competition. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Missouri - Columbia.
Cox, R.H., Russell, W.D., & Robb, M. (1998). Development of a
CSAI-2 Short form for assessing competitive state anxiety during and
immediately prior to competition. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, 31-40.
Cox, R.H., Russell, W.D., & Robb, M. (1999). Comparative
concurrent validity of the MRF-L. and ARS competitive state anxiety
rating scales for volleyball and basketball. Journal of Sport Behavior,
22, 1 - 11.
Krane, V. (1994). The mental readiness form as a measure of
competitive state anxiety. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 189-202.
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R.S., Bump, L.A., & Smith
(1990). Development and validation of the competitive state anxiety
inventory-2. In R. Martens, R.S. Vealey, & D. Burton (Eds.),
competitive anxiety in sport (pp. 117-190). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics Books.
Murphy, S.M., Greenspan, M., Jowdy, D., & Tammen, V. (1989).
Development of a brief rating instrument of competitive anxiety:
Comparisons with the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. Proceedings
of the Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology
(p.82). Seattle, WA.
Thomas, J.R., & Nelson, J.K. (1996). Research methods in
physical activity (3rd edition). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.