Comparison of self-esteem, body satisfaction, and social physique anxiety across males of different exercise frequency and racial background.
Russell, William D.
Self-esteem, body satisfaction and social physique anxiety were
examined in college age males (N=557) of differing self-reported
exercise frequency and racial background Participants were categorized into Caucasian (n=407), African-American, (n=150), three weight training
frequency categories (non-weight training, 1-3 days/week, 4+ days/week),
and three aerobic exercise frequency categories (no aerobic exercise,
1-3 days/week, 4+ days/week). Participants from weight room facilities
and class settings completed Rosenberg's self-esteem Scale (RSE),
the Body Cathexis Scale (BCS), and the Social Physique Anxiety Scale
(SPAS). A 2 x 3 (race x weight training) MANCOVA on SPA, SE, and BCS
indicated a significant overall interaction between race and weight
training on the dependent variables (Wilk's Lambda =.9S, F(1,532) =
7.67, p<. 006). A comparison of SPA across racial background
indicated that African-American males had lower SPA (M=28.92, SD=6.83)
compared to Caucasian males (M =31.35, SD =6.88, t(555) =2.69, p<
.008). Two separate two-step regression analyses separated by race
indicated that for Caucasian males, BMI, body dissatisfaction, and
self-esteem were significant in predicting SPA (F (4,406) =35.96, p<.
0001, R2 =.32). For African-American males, self-esteem and body
dissatisfaction were significant predictors of SPA (F(4,149) =13.74,
p<.0001, R2 =.32). Weight training frequency and aerobic exercise
frequency were not related to SPA, self-esteem, or body dissatisfaction,
nor did exercise frequency enter into the regression models as
significant predictors of SPA. Previous research was supported in that
body dissatisfaction (Eklund & Crawford, 1994) and self-esteem
(Martin, Engels, Wirth, & Smith, 1997) were meaningful predictors of
SPA in males. Results are discussed in terms of potential mediating
variables in males' SPA and the importance that reasons for
exercise may have in determining social physique anxiety.
Recent interest within the exercise psychology literature has
indicated that social physique anxiety (SPA) (Hart, Leary & Rejeski,
1989), or selective presentation anxiety and omission of aspects of one
self to create desired impression (Eklund & Crawford, 1994), may
reduce exercise motivations (Eklund & Crawford, 1994; McAuley, Bane,
& Mihalko, 1995). Specifically, high body dissatisfaction and low
self-esteem have been related to SPA with self-esteem being especially
predictive in females (Martin, Engels, Wirth, & Smith, 1997). Many
people who think others view their bodies favorably or who are
disinterested in other's reactions to their body may rarely
experience SPA. Others, however may be chronically concerned with how
others view their physiques, either because their bodies are objectively
unattractive or because they hold an unrealistically negative perception
of their physiques (Hart et al., 1989).
Although related to global body image and body dissatisfaction, it
has been suggested (Hart et al., 1989; Lantz, Hardy, & Ainsworth,
1991) that SPA is conceptually distinct and useful in understanding
involvement (or lack thereof) in exercise behaviors. Recent research has
indicated exercise preferences are influenced by SPA (McAuley, Bane,
& Mihalko, 1995; Spink, 1992). Specifically, Spink (1992) assigned women to either high or low anxiety groups based on SPA scores and found
women with high SPAS scores reported tendencies to exercise privately
rather than publicly. In addition, McAuley et al., (1995) found that
exercising alone in one s natural environment resulted in lower overall
anxiety compared to laboratory conditions.
SPA is correlated with psychological variables logically associated
with evaluative concerns among young adult women including lower global
self-esteem, fear of negative evaluation, weight dissatisfaction, body
dissatisfaction, body cathexis (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Eklund
& Crawford, 1994; Hart et al. 1989). A variety of physical
characteristics have been found to correlate with SPA including weight,
height, and percent body fat among young adult women (Eklund &
Crawford, 1994; Hart et al., 1989) and a model has recently been
proposed that posits that physiological measures such as body mass
index, influence SPA, which in turn affects protective
self-presentational motives and, ultimately, self-presentational
behaviors in both males and females (Diehl, et al., 1999)
The majority of studies in this area have focused on overall
exercise patterns (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Eklund & Crawford,
1994; Spink, 1992), aerobic exercise patterns (Lantz, et al., 1991;
McAuley et al., 1995) and examination of SPA with respect to young
females, since body dissatisfaction and low self-esteem have shown to be
especially problematic with this population (Silberstein,
Striegel-Moore, Timko, & Rodin, 1988). In fact, it has been argued
that in contemporary society, weight is a domain of central importance
to women (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985), and thus
creates a normative discontent with physical self-esteem. Social
physique anxiety is also predictive of female-specific problematic
behaviors such as disordered eating (Cox, Lantz, & Mayhew, 1997) and
low exercise adherence rates (Eklund & Crawford, 1994).
However, except for notable exceptions (Hart et al., 1989; Martin
& Mack, 1996; McAuley et al., 1995), little research has been done
with self-presentational anxiety in males. In addition, few studies have
examined the relationship between SPA and combined effects strength
training and aerobic exercise. In the construct validation of the SPAS,
Hart et al. (1989) showed that for both females and males, correlations
of the SPA measures of body dissatisfaction and body esteem were
moderate to high. The relationships between body satisfaction,
self-esteem, dieting, and exercise have shown to differ between men and
women. Specifically, Silberstein et al. (1988) found that body
satisfaction was related to self-esteem in men whereas for females
measures of weight dissatisfaction were not associated with self-esteem.
It is clear then that gender differences occur in reasons for exercise
(Silberstein et al., 1988; Vitulli & DePace, 1992) and previous SPA
research has indicated that self-presentational reasons for exer cise
are important for females. It is surprising ,then, that more empirical
work has not investigated the relationship between SPA, body
satisfaction, and self-esteem in males engaged in strength training, an
exercise modality preferred by many males because of males' greater
general interest in building muscle mass to attain what is culturally
regarded as a more socially ideal body image (Silberstein et al., 1988).
Bezner, Adams, and Steinhardt (1997) recently found
exercisers' self-esteem inversely related to body satisfaction. In
their findings, Bezner et al. (1997) hypothesized that individuals with
a less secure sense of self may experience lower self-esteem,
perpetuating a vicious cycle of body dissatisfaction and increased risk
of compulsive behavior. Finally, there has been contradiction in the
literature regarding the relationship between SPA and resultant exercise
behavior. Higher levels of SPA have been shown to be related to lower
exercise participation rates (Hart et al., 1989; Lantz et al., 1997;
McAuley, Bane, Rudolph, & Lox, 1995), yet have also been shown to be
related to emotional profiles similar to addicted exercisers (Frederick
& Morrison, 1996).
It has been noted that male exercisers report body dissatisfaction
(Silberstein et al., 1988), however, it has not generally been
considered as prevalent as among females and is associated with feeling
underweight rather than overweight (Blouin & Goldfield, 1995;
Silberstein et al., 1988). Low self-esteem, depression, and body
dissatisfaction occur in males who are underweight (Davis, Elliott,
Dionne, & Mitchell, 1991) and those who are below self-reported
ideal weight and height report lower self-esteem and greater social
adjustment problems. Concern has also been voiced (Pope, Katz, &
Hudson, 1993) that body dissatisfaction and body image problems,
including muscle dysmorphia, may occur more frequently in males who
weight train in high volumes (Pope et al., 1993) compared to the regular
exercise population and may predispose them to steroid use (Pope,
Gruber, Choi, Olivardia, & Phillips, 1997). Smith et al. (1998)
proposed that when exercisers use weight training to improve
self-esteem, some may come to rel y on this training to feel good about
themselves.
While much of the SPA literature has focused on the relationship
between SPA and exercising females (Martinet al., 1997; McAuley &
Burman, 1993; Thorton & Maurice, 1997), less is known regarding the
relationship regarding males' exercise behavior, and it has been
advocated that this construct be examined in males (Eklund, Mack, &
Hart, 1996). Little research has been done examining the relationship
between self-esteem, body dissatisfaction and SPA in males engaged in
regular weight training and aerobic exercise. Yet, it is generally
regarded that lower physical self-esteem (Martin et al., 1997) and
higher body dissatisfaction (McAuley & Burman, 1993) are related to
greater social physique anxiety. The rationale for the present study was
to extend SPA research to the population of males and various
race/ethnic backgrounds and to an exercise domain (weight training) that
has been less frequently studied.
Finally, it has been suggested that body image should be considered
in a cultural context. Within the eating disorder literature, it has
been noted that African American women develop eating disorders and
experience less body dissatisfaction than Caucasian women (Abood &
Chandler, 1997). However, no studies to date have examined whether
differences exist in self-presentational anxiety as a function of racial
background. Thus, racial background was included as an important
variable to examine whether there were differences in exercising males
on SPA, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem as a function of cultural
and ethnic contextual differences.
One purpose of the current study was to examine the predictors of
SPA in college males. It was hypothesized that strength-training
frequency, aerobic frequency and self-esteem would be negatively related
to SPA, and that body dissatisfaction and BMI would be positively
related to SPA. Self-presentational theory (Leary & Kowalski, 1995)
indicated that people with higher body mass index would be expected to
experience greater SPA. This model was subsequently supported with male
fitness center participants (Diehl et al., 1999). Based on previous
examination of physiological and psychological predictors of SPA (Eklund
& Crawford, 1994; Martin et al., 1997), it was expected that the
psychological predictors and exercise patterns would account for
variance above that attributable to the main physiological measure in
the study (BMI). Since previous studies have shown physical
characteristics to be closely associated with SPA (Eklund &
Crawford, 1994; Hart et al., 1989; McAuley et al., 1995), BMI was
considered a pri mary predictor of SPA in the current study. A second
purpose was to determine whether the variables of racial background and
exercise frequency were related to the psychological variables of SPA,
body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem. Since previous exercise
literature (Abood & Chandler, 1997) has indicated that there are
differences across race in females' body dissatisfaction, it was
hypothesized that similar differences might also exist for males, with
African-American males maintaining lower body dissatisfaction and higher
self-esteem than Caucasian males and males of greater exercise frequency
maintaining lower SPA, body dissatisfaction, and higher self-esteem than
males who exercised less frequently. Since the analysis of racial
background on SPA has not been previously examined, this analysis was
considered exploratory in nature.
Based upon the extant social physique anxiety literature, there
were several specific hypotheses proposed in the current study:
1. Based on previous social physique anxiety research (Crawford
& Eklund, 1994; Martin et al., 1997), individuals who scored higher
on body
dissatisfaction would score higher on SPA.
2. Using body mass index as a covariate, males could be
significantly differentiated on SPA, self-esteem, and body
dissatisfaction based on their strength training frequency and racial
background.
3. Males' SPA, after controlling for the physiological
correlate of body mass index, could be predicted from body
dissatisfaction, self-esteem, weight training frequency, and aerobic
frequency.
4. Males with higher levels of SPA would display lower weekly
strength training and aerobic exercise frequencies.
Method
Subjects
Data were collected from a total of 557 (Caucasian =407, African
American = 150) collegeage males from three different Midwest
universities. The mean age of males was 21.94 years (SD = 3.66), the
mean height of subjects was 1.81 m (SD = .08 in), the mean weight of
subjects was 83.22 kg, (SD = 16.29 kg). Weight training frequency and
aerobic exercise frequency were used as measures of exercise regularity
in the study and were measured through self-reported number of days per
week that participants engaged in exercise.
Measures
Social Physique Anxiety Scale (SPAS). The SPAS developed by Hart et
al. (1989) is used to assess the degree of anxiety from the perception
that others are devaluing one's body. Subjects respond to 12 items
based on the degree to which the items are true for them and values are
summed from a low score of 12 to a high score of 60. Higher scores are
indicative of greater SPA. The 5-point Likert scale is anchored by 5
(extremely) and 1 (not at all). An example of one question from the SPAS
is "In the presence of others, I feel apprehensive about my
physique/figure." Item 2 ("1 would never worry about wearing
clothes that might make me look thin or overweight") was altered to
reflect a positive statement, "I would worry about wearing clothes
that might make me look too thin or overweight." based on previous
recommendations (Lantz et al., 1991; McAuley & Burman, 1993; Petrie
et at., 1996). Adequate internal consistency (r=.90), predictive
validity, and construct validity have been documented (Crawford &
Eklund, 199 4; Hart et at., 1989; McAuley & Burman, 1993).
Self-Esteem (SE). Rosenberg's (1965) self-esteem scale is a
10-item scale that measures global self-esteem with scores ranging from
I (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Total scores range from 10
to 40 with higher scores indicative of more favorable self-esteem. An
example of an item is "On the whole, I am satisfied with
myself." Adequate internal consistency (r =.92), test-retest
reliability (r =.85) and convergent validity and discriminant validity have been demonstrated (Wylie, 1974).
Body Satisfaction (BCS). The Body Cathexis Scale developed by
Secourd and Jourard (1953) is a measure of the degree of feeling of
dissatisfaction with various parts of the body and is a 24-item scale
with scores ranging from I (very satisfied) to 5 (very dissatisfied).
Total scores range from 24 to 120 with higher scores indicative of a
higher level of body dissatisfaction. Adequate internal consistency and
test-retest reliability have also been demonstrated for the BCS (Seggar,
McCammon, & Cannon, 1988).
Procedure
Prior to testing, human subjects approval was obtained from
University institutional review boards. Data was collected by
administration of a questionnaire packet that included Rosenberg's
SE Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), The BCS (Secord & Jourard, 1953) and the
SPAS (Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989) and order of the scales was
counterbalanced. Questionnaire packets were preceded by demographic
information that included age, height, weight, racial background, and
how many days per week, on average, males engaged in strength training
and aerobic exercise. In this study, frequency of aerobic exercise was
the primary interest and type of aerobic exercise was not assessed. In
order to collect data from weight training males, data were collected in
free weight rooms of three separate universities. Males were approached
prior to their workouts and asked to participate in the study. Those who
agreed to participate signed informed consent and were given the
inventory packet to complete prior to beginning their exercise sessi on.
Data was also collected from males enrolled in randomly selected classes
who were not currently involved in regular weight training or aerobic
exercise.
Results
Comparison of Body Dissatisfaction Levels on Social Physique
Anxiety Scores
Table 1 presents descriptive results for study variables divided by
racial background. Median split scores on the BCS were used (Median=62)
to separate males in to high and low body dissatisfaction groups.
Results of an independent t-test comparing high and low body
dissatisfaction groups on SPA revealed that males classified as high in
body dissatisfaction showed significantly higher (M=35.34, SD=6.72) SPA
compared to males classified as low in their body dissatisfaction
(M=29.46, SD=5.99) (t(555)=-9.90, p<.0001,ES=.78).
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Study Variables Combined and Separated
by Racial Background and Weight Training Frequency
Full Sample Training Frequency Age Height Weight SE
Non-weight training (n=129) 22.75 1.80 81.04 34.52
(5.4l) (*) (.07) (14.54) (4.96)
1-3 Days/Week(n=208) 21.96 1.81 82.42 35.15
(3.36) (.08) (14.51) (4.37)
4+ days / Week (n=220) 21.45 1.82 82.54 34.92
(2.37) (.08) (18.53) (4.83)
Full Sample Training Frequency BCS SPAS
Non-weight training (n=129) 61.54 32.66
(17.41) (8.03)
1-3 Days/Week(n=208) 61.31 31.35
(14.12) (6.95)
4+ days / Week (n=220) 59.93 31.09
(16.21) (7.57)
Race Training Frequency Age Height Weight SE
Caucasian
Non-weight training (n=115) 22.79 1.81 81.53 34.33
(5.66) (*) (.07) (14.93) (5.02)
1-3 Days/Week (n=154) 22.03 1.82 82.68 35.10
(3.28) (.08) (13.28) (4.45)
4 + Days/Week (n=138) 21.37 1.82 84.81 34.83
(2.43) (.08) (16.00) (4.80)
African-American
Non-weight training (n=14) 22.42 1.76 77.11 36.14
(2.84) (.08) (10.41) (4.24)
1-3 Days/Week (n=54) 21.76 1.80 81.67 35.30
(3.59) (.08) (17.69) (4.20)
4+Days/Week (n=82) 21.58 1.80 85.95 35.08
(2.27) (.08) (22.24) (4.91)
Race Training Frequency BCS SPAS
Caucasian
Non-weight training (n=115) 61.80 32.71
(16.78) (8.04)
1-3 Days/Week (n=154) 60.64 31.61
(12.81) (6.75)
4 + Days/Week (n=138) 59.91 32.03
(13.89) (7.43)
African-American
Non-weight training (n=14) 59.43 32.21
(22.60) (8.22)
1-3 Days/Week (n=54) 63.20 30.59
(17.33) (7.51)
4+Days/Week (n=82) 59.96 29.51
(19.63) (7.58)
(*) Standard deviations are in parentheses.
Race by Weight Training (2 x 3) MANCOVA on Social Physique Anxiety,
Body Dissatisfaction, and Self-Esteem
The MANCOVA allowed for answering the research question "Are
there certain differences on SPA, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem
for males of different racial background and weight training frequency,
after removing the effects of body mass index?" Weight training was
selected as an independent variable for this analysis since little is
known regarding the relationship between strength training and SPA.
Results from the overall MANCOVA indicated a significant multivariate interaction between racial background and strength training frequency on
the criterion variables of SPA, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem,
using body mass index as the covariate (Wilk's Lambda = .95, F
(3,553) =8.96, p<.001, ES = .05). Controlling for BMI, a comparison
of SPA across racial background indicated that African-American males
had lower SPA (M =28.92, SD =6.88) compared to Caucasian males (M
=31.35, SD = 6.88, F (1,556) 8.61 p<.001, [R.sup.2]=.05), but no
differences in SPA were found across strength training frequency (F
(7,532) =.52, p =ns). Controlling for BMI, body dissatisfaction was not
significant in differentiating racial background (F (1,532) =.06, p=ns),
nor was body dissatisfaction significant in differentiating strength
training frequency (F(7,532) =.88, p=ns). Finally, controlling for BMI,
self-esteem was not significant in differentiating between racial
background (F(l,532) =1.09, p=ns) nor was self-esteem significant in
differentiating between strength training frequency (F (7,532) =1 .38,
p=ns).
Hierarchical Regression Results for Social Physique Anxiety, Body
Dissatisfaction, and Self-Esteem
Table 2 provides the zero-order correlations among the variables
within the study. Two separate two-step hierarchical regression analyses
were performed to determine the predictors of SPA, while controlling for
BMI as a primary physiological correlate within the model. In order to
control for racial background differences in the prediction of the
criterion variable, separate regression analyses across racial
background were conducted on SPA. Table 3 provides a summary table of
the regression analysis for Caucasian males while Table 4 provides a
summary table of the regression analysis for African-American males. In
each analysis, the first model provided information on the effect of BMI
on SPA based on previous findings that BMI is a primary physiological
correlate of SPA (Deihl et al., 1999), while the second model provided
information on the effect of each predictor while controlling for all
other variables. The differences between [R.sup.2] in the first and
second model provided information on whether body dissatisfaction,
self-esteem, strength training frequency, and aerobic exercise frequency
significantly added to the model, after removing the effects of body
mass index.
Table 2
Zero-Order Correlations among Study Variables by Racial Background
Caucasian Males
BMI Strength Aerobic SE BCS SPA
BMI --
Strength .04 --
Aerobic -.04 .32 (**) --
SE -.03 .06 .05 --
BCS .07 -.05 -.14 (*) -.31 (**) --
SPA .20 (*) -.03 -.06 -.37 (**) .47 (**) --
African-American Males
BMI Strength Aerobic SE BCS SPA
BMI --
Strength .06 --
Aerobic .15 .05 --
SE .10 .02 .08 --
BCS .31 (*) -.03 -.17 (*) -.39 (**) --
SPA .10 -.12 -.10 -.47 (**) .47 (**) --
(*)p<.05
(**)p<.001
BMI = Body Mass Index; SE = self-esteem; BCS = body dissatisfaction; SPA
= social physique anxiety
Caucasian Males. For Caucasian males, results from the first model
indicated a significant overall effect for BMI (F(l ,405) = l7.88, p
<.0001, [R.sup.2] = .04, ES = .04), indicating that by itself, there
was a significant relationship between body mass index and SPA.
Examination of the unstandardized beta (b = .38) indicated that as BMI
increased, SPA increased as well. In the second model, SPA was regressed
on BMI along with body dissatisfaction, self-esteem, strength training
frequency, and aerobic exercise frequency. Results of this analysis
indicated that BMI was significant (p<.0001) after accounting for all
other variables. The second overall model was also significant
(F(5,406)=35.96,p<.0001,[R.sup.2]=.31,ES=.45). The test for
significance of R2 increase between model 1 and model 2 was significant
(F(5,406)=69.23,p<.00l) indicating that body dissatisfaction,
self-esteem, and BMI accounted for a significantly greater proportion of
variance than BMI alone. Specifically, within the second model, body dis
satisfaction (t(406)=8.72,p<.0001,b=.20), self-esteem
(t(406)=-5.75,p<.0001,b=-.39) and BMI (t(406)=4.l3,p<.0001,b=.32)
were significant to the model. Together, these results indicate that
BMI, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem were all significant and
accounted for 22% of the unique variance in SPA. Specifically, for
Caucasian males, higher levels of SPA were related to higher BMI, higher
body dissatisfaction, and lower self-esteem.
African-American Males. For African-American males, results of the
first model indicated a nonsignificant overall model (F (1, 149) =
l.39,p =.24, [R.sup.2]=.01), indicating that for African-American males,
BMI itself was not a significant predictor of SPA. The second overall
model was significant (F(4, 149)=13.74,p<.0001,[R.sup.2]=.32,ES=.48)
and indicated a meaningful increase ([R.sup.2] change = .30) in model
prediction. The test for the significance of R2 increase between model 1
and model 2 was significant (F(5,406)=83.33,p<.001) indicating that
self-esteem and body dissatisfaction accounted for a significantly
greater proportion of variance than BMI alone. Specifically, within the
second model, self-esteem (t(149)=-4.40,p<.0001,b=-.57) and body
dissatisfaction (t(149)=3.75,p<.0003,b=.12) were significant to the
model and accounted for 16% of the unique variance in SPA. Lower
self-esteem and higher body dissatisfaction were therefore predictive of
higher SPA in African-American males.
Table 3
Regression Results on Social Physique Anxiety for Caucasian Males
Model Variable beta Beta T p Unique Var.
Model 1 BMI .38 (*) .21 4.23 .0001 .04
Model 2 BMI .32 .17 4.13 .0001 .03
BCS .20 -.38 8.72 .0001 .13
SE -.39 -.25 -5.75 .0001 .06
Strength -.03 -.01 -.18 .86 .000
Aerobic .06 .02 .36 .72 .000
Model Correlations
BMI BCS SE Strength Aerobic
Model 1 --
Model 2 --
.31 --
.10 -.40 --
.06 .03 .02 --
.15 -.18 .08 .05 --
(*)Y intercept=25.57
Table 4
Regression Results on Social Physique Anxiety for African-American Males
Model Variable beta Beta T p Unique Var.
Model 1 BMI .14 (*) .10 1.18 .24 .01
Model 2 BMI .07 .04 .58 .56 .002
BCS .12 .31 3.75 .0003 .07
SE -.57 -.34 -4.40 .0001 .09
Strength -.49 -.11 -1.56 .12 .01
Aerobic -.08 -.02 -.28 .78 .000
Model Correlations
BMI BCS SE Strength Aerobic
Model 1 --
Model 2 --
.31 --
.10 -.40 --
.06 .03 .02 --
.15 -.18 .08 .05 --
(*)Y-intercept=42.63
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to extend SPA research to the
population of males of various race/ethnic backgrounds and to an
exercise domain (weight training) that has been less frequently studied.
The current results support the notion that racial background is a
significant cultural variable capable of influencing SPA. The MANCOVA
results indicated that, after controlling for BMI, racial background was
capable of accounting for differences in SPA, but not global self-esteem
or body dissatisfaction, even under conditions of similar weight
training frequency. Previous literature on SPA with females (McAuley et
al., 1995) has indicated that aerobic exercise frequency was not related
to increased SPA. Current results support this finding in that
males' self-reported frequency of strength training or aerobic
training did not influence SPA. However, the significant main effect for
racial background supports that cultural ideals regarding body-image may
be important in male as well female populations (Abood & Chan dler,
1997). Body mass index means were similar for Caucasians (M= 25.16, SD =
3.94) and African-Americans (M= 25.71, SD 5.12) yet African-American
males were less anxious about their physical appearance. While this
difference was significant, males' SPA levels across both races (M
= 31.35, SD = 7.47) were lower than those previously reported for
females (M = 37.9, SD = 9.78; Eklund & Crawford, 1994), but similar
to previous samples of college age males (M=30.2, SD =7.50; Hart et al.,
1989). Body esteem measures have shown to be a significant influence on
self-esteem, and individuals who have indicated a greater dislike for
their bodies as measured by weight concern (Martin et al., 1997) and
body esteem (Bartlewski, Van Raalte, & Brewer, 1996) have reported
elevated SPA. The multiple regression results extend this relationship
in that, regardless of exercise frequency or race, males with greater
body dissatisfaction had greater SPA. Intercorrelations revealed that
body dissatisfaction was equally correlated ( r =.47, p<.0001) for
Caucasian and African-American males, indicating one's appraisal of
body satisfaction appears to be a primary variable associated with SPA.
While SPA, body dissatisfaction and self-esteem have been shown to
be interrelated (Crawford & Eklund, 1994) MANCOVA results indicated
that only SPA scores differentiated males on racial background, after
controlling for BMI, and that exercise frequency did not differentiate
any of the psychological variables. This is in contrast to previous
results that have concluded that more frequent exercise is associated
with greater SPA (Frederick & Morrison, 1996) and that SPA is a
predictor of exercise behavior (Lantz et al., 1997). There are several
reasons that may exist for the discrepancies in these findings.
First, self-presentational theory (Leary & Kowalski, 1995)
suggests that there are specific relations among anthropometric,
affective, motivational, and behavioral variables. Because exercise has
been shown to reduce factors thought to be associated with SPA such as
body composition (McAuley et al., 1995) and body esteem (Bartlewski et
al., 1996), exercise participation may moderate the effects of changes
in physiological indicators on perceptual variables related to SPA
(McAuley et al. 1996). However, individuals who are uncomfortable with
how their body appears to others may not present themselves in
situations where their bodies can be seen. Thus, it is possible that
individuals with greater SPA may exercise to cope with this anxiety, but
in a private setting. Second, while there are many reasons why males
engage in exercise, one potential reason is to improve physical
appearance. Consistent findings have shown associations between SPA and
reasons for exercise, with positive correlations between SPA and se
lf-presentational reasons for exercise such as body tone, physical
attractiveness and weight control (Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Eklund
& Crawford, 1994) and many males in the current study may have
exercised for improved hhealth or fitness reasons, which may have
accounted for the lack of significance between exercise frequency and
SPA. This conclusion is tenuous since reasons for exercising were not
directly measured. Third, original research examining the SPA construct
(Crawford & Eklund, 1994; Eklund & Crawford, 1994) has indicated
that favoribility of exercise settings emphasizing physique are
negatively related to SPA. Males in the current study were assessed in
free-weight rooms, characterized by full-length mirrors and presence of
other exercisers. Thus, in the present study, subjects with higher SPA
levels may not have been adequately assessed because their
self-presentational anxiety would have prevented them from being
assessed in such a public setting.
Similar to previous research with females (Crawford & Eklund,
1994; Eklund & Crawford, 1994; Hart et al., 1989; McAuley &
Burman, 1993), overall results from this study support that males with
higher body dissatisfaction have higher SPA. This conclusion was
strengthened by the results of regression results indicating that body
dissatisfaction itself accounted for significant unique variance in SPA
for both Caucasian ([R.sup.2]= .13) and African-American males
([R.sup.2] =.07). Body dissatisfaction, therefore, seems to be an
important predictor of SPA in both males and females, regardless of
frequency of exercise patterns. These results support findings with
females (Abood & Chandler, 1997) that indicated differences in body
dissatisfaction across race regardless of physical characteristics.
While BMI was predictive of SPA in Caucasian males, it was not a
significant predictor of SPA in African-American males, indicating that
while there may be differences in body acceptance across race, the
psychological correlates of body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem remain
important predictors of SPA. These findings for body satisfaction
support previous research indicating that SPA in males was significantly
correlated with physical self-perception (Petrie et al., 1996), and
discrepancies between perceived actual and ideal body image may be
equally detrimental for males and females (Bezner et al., 1997).
Contrary, however, to Martin et al. (1997), a much lower percentage of
variance in SPA was accounted for by body dissatisfaction and
self-esteem. Martin et al. (1997) reported that self-esteem accounted
for 45% of the variance in SPA and noted that constructs specific to
body esteem (weight concern and physical condition) may become woven into global self-esteem. Thus, it appears that had physical aspect s of
self-esteem been assessed, a greater percentage of variance in SPA may
have been explained. In addition, the subjects in the Martin et al
(1997) study were elite youth athletes. Therefore, their perceptions
regarding physical appearance were more salient and important in
evaluating reactions from others, thus more central to their
self-esteem.
Another consideration relevant to the current study is the finding
that self-esteem is developed by regular exercise and muscle development
(Fox & Corbin, 1989). Correlational indices indicated that
self-esteem and weight training frequency were virtually unrelated
(r=.06), yet, self-esteem was significantly negatively related to SPA
(r=-.40, p <.0001). The finding that self-esteem was not related to
weight training frequency is not surprising in that previous research
(Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989) has indicated that exercise induced improvements are specific to physical aspects of self-esteem. Future
studies examining SPA in males may need to incorporate a physical
dimension of self-esteem, as body esteem has been shown to be negatively
related to objective body weight and account for additional variance in
SPA beyond that attributable to body fat (Martin et al., 1997).
Specifically, physical aspects of self-esteem may measure self-image
concerns that are more relevant to exercise settings (Franzoi &
Shields, 1984). Exercise experience may also be a mediating variable in
the exercise-SPA relationship. Inexperienced exercisers have shown more
SPA than experienced exercisers (Bartlewski et al., 1996; Hart &
Gill, 1993) and frequent weight trainers in the current study may have
been more desensitized to excessive anxiety associated with exercise in
a public context. This conclusion appears plausible, but tenuous since
subjects were not asked specific questions regarding weight-training
experience.
Cultural ideals have been shown to shape individuals'
body-image experiences and the extent to which one utilizes dieting,
exercising, and other measures to manage body-image experiences. Indeed,
the current finding that African-American males had lower body
dissatisfaction is consistent with the contention that cultural
variables are important in body image experiences. It is not known why
this current difference existed, however, it may be due to a greater
acceptance in body type differences that has been shown to account for
lower prevalence of body dissatisfaction in African American females
compared to Caucasian (Abood & Chandler, 1997).
The current results indicate that weight training frequency and
racial background do not interact to mediate the relationship between
exercise and SPA. However, there are several considerations that should
be pointed out within the context of this study. First, the measures of
body dissatisfaction and self-esteem used in this study were global
measures. Previous study in the area of body image dissatisfaction has
indicated that body esteem (Martin et al., 1997) is also a predictor of
SPA and that body satisfaction indices are different across gender
(Silberstein et al., 1988). Specifically, Silberstein et al. (1988)
indicated that three factors of body esteem that emerged for men
included "physical attractiveness", "upper body
strength", and "physical condition". Therefore, more
accurate relationships between exercise, racial background, and SPA may
need to include gender-specific, multidimensional factors that may be
more relevant in males.
Second, since the current sample of college age males were
non-athletes, their reasons for exercise may have been different than
athletes or high volume exercisers. Women have been shown (Silberstein
et al., 1988; Vitulli & DePace, 1992) to exercise more for weight
control reasons, and while not measured in the current study, a large
number of males may have been exercising for health and fitness reasons
instead of self-presentational motives. In addition, several researchers
(Harter, 1993; Martin et al., 1997) have argued that self-presentational
concerns may be more problematic with training regimes of higher volume
(including athletes) because their negative body image is more closely
associated with their self-esteem. Thus, while the current design
categorized males according to exercise frequency, volume of training
(intensity) was not directly assessed. Thus, for example, in order to
find relationships more indicative of weight training and SPA, it may be
necessary to include an index of current muscula rity (Pope et al.,
1997) and dietary patterns (Blouin & Goldfield, 1995; Pope et al.,
1993), and weight training intensity, as has been done in bodybuilders.
Finally, a greater proportion of variance of SPA in males may be
accounted for by incorporating more specific measures to physical
self-esteem (Fox & Corbin, 1989; Franzoi & Shields, 1984)
Overall, this study extends the SPA literature in that body
dissatisfaction and self-esteem are related to SPA in males. Exercise
frequency and racial background did not interact to differentially
determine body dissatisfaction and SPA. Future studies examining these
particular variables in males may need to incorporate male weight
trainers who engage in higher volumes of training and should assess
reasons for exercise, experience with exercise, and physical aspects of
self-esteem as potential mediating variables.
References
Abood, D.A., & Chandler, S.B. (1997). Race and the role of
weight, weight change, and body dissatisfaction in eating disorders.
American Journal of Health Behavior, 21, 21-25.
Bartlewski, P.P., Van Raalte, J.L., & Brewer, B.W. (1996).
Effects of aerobic exercise on the social physique anxiety and body
esteem of female college students. Women's Sport and Physical
Activity Journal, 5, 49-62.
Bezner, J.R., Adams, T.B., & Steinhardt, M.A. (1997).
Relationship of body dissatisfaction to physical health and wellness.
American Journal of Health Behavior, 21, 147-155.
Blouin, A.G. & Goldfield, G.S. (1995). Body image and steroid
use in male bodybuilders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18,
159-165.
Cox, L.M., Lantz, C.D., & Mayhew, J.L. (1997). The role of
social physique anxiety and other variables in predicting eating
behaviors in college students. International Journal of Sport Nutrition,
7, 310-317.
Crawford, S., & Eklund, R.C. (1994). Social physique anxiety,
reasons for exercise, and attitudes toward exercise settings. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 70-82.
Davis, C., Elliott, S., Dionne, M., & Mitchell, L. (1991). The
relationship of personality factors and physical activity to body
satisfaction in men. Personality and Individual Differences, 12,
689-694.
Diehl, N.S., Brewer, B.W., Cornelius, A.E., Van Raalte, J.L., Shaw,
D., & Fiero, P.L. (1999, September). A preliminary test of a model
of protective self-presentational behavior in exercise. Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of The Association for the Advancement of Applied
Sport Psychology, Banff, Canada.
Eklund, R.C., & Crawford, S. (1994). Active women, social
physique anxiety, and exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 16, 431-448.
Eklund, R.C., Mack, D., & Hart, E. (1996). Factorial validity
of the social physique anxiety scale for females. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 18, 281-295.
Fox, K.R., & Corbin, C.B. (1989). The physical self-perception
profile: Development and preliminary validation. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology 11, 408-430.
Franzoi, S.L., & Shields, S.A. (1984). The body esteem scale:
Multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college population.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 173-178.
Frederick, C.M., & Morrison, C.S. (1996). Social physique
anxiety: Personality constructs, motivations, exercise attitudes, and
behaviors. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 963-972.
Hart, E.A., & Gill, D.L. (1993). Protective self-presentation
and exercise behavior. Unpublished manuscript.
Hart, E.A., Leary, M.R., & Rejeski, W.J. (1989). The
measurement of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology 11, 94-104.
Harter, S. (1993). Causes and consequences of low self-esteem in
children and adolescents. In R.F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The
puzzle of low self-regard (87-116). New York: Plenum.
Lantz, C. D., Hardy, C.J., & Ainsworth, B.E. (1991). Social
physique anxiety and perceived exercise behavior. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 20, 83-93.
Leary, M.R., & Kowalski, R.M. (1995). Social anxiety, New York:
Guilford Press.
Martin, J.J., Engels, H.J., Wirth, J.C., & Smith, K.L. (1997).
Predictors of social physique anxiety in elite female youth athletes.
Women's Sport and Physical Activity Journal, 6, 29-48.
Martin, K.A., & Mack, D. (1996). Relationships between physical
self-presentation and sport competition anxiety: A preliminary study.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 75-82.
McAuley, E., Bane, S.M., & Mihalko, S.L. (1995). Exercise in
middle-aged adults: Self-efficacy and self-presentational outcomes.
Preventive Medicine, 24, 319-328.
McAuley, E., Bane, S.M., Rudolph, D.L., & Lox, C.L. (1995).
Physique anxiety and exercise in middle-aged adults. Journal of
Gerontology 50B, 229-235.
McAuley, E., & Burman, G. (1993). The social physique anxiety
scale: Construct validity in adolescent females. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 25, 1049-1053.
Petrie, T.A., Diehl, N., Rogers, R.L., & Johnson, C.L. (1996).
The social physique anxiety scale: Reliability and construct validity.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 18, 420-425.
Pope, H.G., Gruber, A.J., Choi, P., Olivardia, R., & Phillips,
K.E. (1997). Muscle dysmorphia: An underecognized form of body
dysmorphic disorder. Psychosomatics, 38, 548-557.
Pope, H.G., Katz, D.L., & Hudson, J.I. (1993). Anorexia nervosa and "reverse anorexia among 108 male bodybuilders. Comprehensive
Psychiatry, 34, 406-409.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women
and weight: A normative discontent. In T.B. Sonderegger (Ed.),
Psychology and gender: Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1984. Lincoln,
NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: University Press.
Secord, P.F., & Jourard, S.M. (1953) The appraisal of
body-cathexis: Body cathexis and the self. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 17, 343-347.
Seggar, J.F., McCammon, D.L., & Cannon, L.D. (1988).
Relationships between physical activity, weight discrepancies,
body-cathexis, and psychological well being in college women. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 67, 659-669.
Silberstein, L.R., Striegel-Moore, R.H., Timko, C., & Rodin, J.
(1988). Behavioral and psychological implications of body
dissatisfaction: Do men and women differ? Sex Roles, 19, 219-232.
Smith, D., Hale, B., & Collins, D. (1998). Measurement of
exercise dependence in body-builders. Journal of Sportsmedicine and
Physical Fitness, 38, 66-74.
Sonstroem, R.J., & Morgan, W.P. (1989). Exercise and
self-esteem: Rationale and model. Medicine and Science in Sport and
Exercise, 21, 329-337.
Spink, K.S. (1992). Relation of anxiety about social physique to
location of participation in physical activity. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 74, 1075-1078.
Thorton, B., & Maurice, J. (1997). Physique contrast effect:
Adverse impact of idealized body images for women. Sex Roles, 37,
433-439.
Vitulli, W.F., & DePace, N. (1992). Manifest reasons for
jogging and not jogging. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 111-114.
Wylie, R.C. (1974). The self-esteem concept (Vol. 1): A review of
methodological and measuring instruments. (2nd Ed.). Lincoln, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.