A comparison of post-exercise mood enhancement across common exercise distraction activities.
Russell, William ; Pritschet, Brian ; Frost, Beth 等
There is a strong consensus within the exercise psychology
literature that mood enhancement is a primary benefit of physical
activity (Berger, 1996; Berger & McInnman, 1993; International
Society of Sport Psychology, 1992; Morgan, 1997). The International
Society for Sport Psychology (1992) recently described psychological
benefits of physical activity and concluded that exercise has been
related to desirable changes in mood. Based on a systematic analysis of
self-regulation techniques, Thayer, Newman, & McClain (1994)
reported that exercise was the most successful technique at changing a
negative mood. Recently, the United States Department of Health and
Human Services (1996) also indicated that physical activity is
associated with improvements in mood states such as anxiety and
depression.
Mood enhancement has resulted from both chronic (Brown, Wang,
Ebberling, Fortlace, Puleo, Benson, & Rippe, 1994) and acute
exercise bouts (McGowan, et al., 1996; Pierce & Pate, 1994; Roth,
1989; Steptoe & Cox, 1988), yet there is no current consensus for
explanations for potential mechanisms in short or long-term exercise
effects on mood state and both physiological and psychological
mechanisms have been proposed (Berger & Motl, 2000). Despite common
perceptions, there is little research evidence to support any individual
or group factors as consistently producing mood benefits as measured by
the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr, & Dropplemann,
1971). Research on physiological mechanisms includes the endorphin,
cortisol, monoamine, and thermogenic hypotheses (Berger & Motl,
2000). However, support for hypotheses such as the endorphin hypothesis
is equivocal because the blood brain barrier is impermeable to peptides
in the blood stream and endorphin levels have not been related to mood
changes on the POMS (Farrell et al., 1986; Farrell, Gustafson, Morgan,
& Pert, 1987). Psychological mechanisms also may influence the
relationship between exercise and mood alteration. Potential
psychological mechanisms include; improved self-concept, feelings of
self-efficacy, enjoyment, expectancy of psychological benefits,
increased sense of control, and "time-out" from daily hassles
(Berger, 1996; Berger, Owen, & Man, 1993; Morgan, 1985). In
particular, acute exercise has been purported to produce short-term mood
improvements due to its distraction characteristics and it was recently
proposed that continued research of the POMS necessitated determining
whether the "time-out" hypothesis is a viable explanation for
mood alteration (Berger & Motl, 2000).
Studies examining acute mood changes after a single bout of
exercise have found fairly consistent beneficial changes in mood as
measured by the POMS. Steptoe and Cox (1988) compared females
participating in single bouts of high and low intensity exercise
accompanied by either music or a metronome and found that while music
did not influence post-exercise mood, beneficial mood changes were
observed following low-intensity exercise. Single session activity bouts
have shown exercise participants to exhibit beneficial POMS susbscale
changes compared to controls during exercise compared to post-exercise
(Ewing, Scott, Mandez, & McBride, 1984), in response to aerobic
exercise in college students (Roth, 1989) and older women (Peirce and
Pate, 1994) and for submaximal exercise (Steptoe, Kearsely, &
Waiters, 1993).
Berger and Owen (1988) proposed a tentative exercise taxonomy for
mood enhancement with exercise which includes an overall requirement for
exercise to be (1) enjoyable, (2) aerobic, (3) noncompetitive, (4)
temporally and spatially certain, and (5) repetitive and rhythmical.
Suggested training requirements have included (6) 2-3 times per week,
(7) moderate intensity, and (8) 20-30 minutes in duration. However,
activities that are nonaerobic, but meet the other taxonomy requirements
have also shown to improve mood (Berger & Owen, 1992; McGowan et
al., 1996). Among additional concerns regarding short-term effects of
exercise on mood are that the mechanisms for short-term change remain
unclear and that expectancy effects of mood improvement, not exercise
itself, may be responsible for mood improvement. Recently, Berger, Owen,
Motl, and Parks (1998) examined whether expectancy of psychological
benefits was related to mood changes after jogging. Despite significant
acute mood benefits, expectancy of psychological benefits was not
conclusively related to significant changes on the POMS subscales of
tension, depression, anger, and confusion (Berger et al., 1998).
It appears then that a viable explanation for short-term mood
enhancement from various exercise activities may come from the
distraction characteristics of the activity. A new direction for mood
research that has recently been advocated (Berger & Motl, 2000) is
on environmental influences and their capacity to increase activity
enjoyment and stimulate greater distractibility during exercise.
Directors of many gymnasia, health clubs, university recreation centers,
and other workout sites are now providing music and television for
motivational reasons including the attraction of exercisers, to
encourage pulsating workouts, and to distract people from the
potentially negative sensations of effortful exercise. In addition,
exercise modes such as elliptical machines, cycle ergometers, and stair
climbing machines are often popular because they allow exercisers to
engage in concomitant distraction activities during exercise.
Unfortunately, the effects of distraction activities on mood change
are unclear. Steptoe and Cox (1988) examined whether lively stimulating
music moderated mood changes as measured by the POMS after participation
in high- and low-intensity bicycle ergometry and found no evidence that
music was differentially associated with mood changes at either
intensity. The effects of two other commonly engaged activities during
certain physical activity, reading and television watching, and their
distraction effects on mood, are unknown. The authors are not aware of
any research examining the influence of exercising while either watching
television or reading on the relationship between exercise and mood
alteration. The potential influence of these distracting activities on
the exercise -mood relationship is a needed area of investigation.
Thus, the central purpose of this study was to compare the mood
alteration and perceptual appraisal of effort during exercise under
standardized distraction activities (television watching and reading)
with exercise in a non-distraction control condition. Subsequently,
according to the "time-out" hypothesis as an underlying
mechanism for mood benefits from exercise, it was hypothesized that:
1. Participants exercising in the distraction activity conditions
would report greater post-exercise mood improvement compared to the
exercise control condition as measured by the POMS TMD scores and
individual POMS subscale scores.
2. Participants in the distraction condition would subsequently
appraise exercise of a similar intensity as less effortful compared to
participants in the exercise control condition.
Method
Subjects
Fifty-three (32 males, 21 females) college-age undergraduate
physical education majors from a Midwest university volunteered their
participation for this study. All participants reported they engaged in
regular aerobic activity at least three times a week, meeting ACSM frequency requirements for regularly active adults (ACSM, 1998).
Subjects were treated in accordance with the human subjects protocol and
were randomly assigned to treatment conditions.
Measures
The Profile of Mood States (McNair, Lorr, & Dropplemenn, 1971)
is comprised of 65 adjectives that are reflected on a 5-point scale to
measure six identifiable mood or affective states and a total mood
disturbance score. Scores on the six mood subscales include;
tension-anxiety, depression-dejection, anger-hostility, vigor-activity,
fatigue-inertia, and confusion-bewilderment. The profile has been shown
to be suitable for individuals from 18 years to adulthood and internal
consistency reliability has ranged from .84 to .95 (McNair & Lorr,
1964). Test-retest reliability has ranged from .65 to .74. The immediate
"how do you feel right now" response was used for the current
study, in order to measure psycho-physiological appraisal of effort
sense throughout the ride conditions, Borg's (1982) Rating of
Perceived Exertion Scale was used to evaluate subjects' perceptions
of exercise.
Procedures
The experimental protocol consisted of two separate laboratory
sessions. The first session was a habituation session during which
subjects were explained the exercise protocol, completed informed
consent, and had their resting heart rate assessed using a heart rate
monitor (Polar Electro Model). The purpose of the this session was to
obtain resting heart rates so experimental exercise heart rates could be
obtained and to familiarize subjects with the experimental protocol to
obtain more accurate pre-exercise mood. After resting heart rate was
obtained, subjects were taken to a private room where they practiced
riding an electronically braked bicycle ergometer at a work rate which
corresponded to 60-75% of individual heart rate reserve. After obtaining
individuals' work rates, subjects were scheduled for their
experimental ride session.
Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three exercise
conditions; (1) a video condition in which the subject was required to
watch a standardized health video, (2) a reading condition in which
subjects were required to read standardized health material similar in
content to the video, or (3) an exercise control condition, in which
subjects were not provided any external distraction. All exercise
sessions consisted of a 25-minute exercise period at 60-75% of the
individual's heart rate reserve and were preceded by a 5-minute
warm-up and 5-minute cool-down period. Heart rate data was not available
to subjects during their ride but was actively monitored by
experimenters so that exercise intensity could be held constant across
all conditions. Since previous research has indicated that psychological
responses to exercise may vary according to circadian rhythms (Hill,
Cureton, & Collins, 1988; Koltyn, Lynch, & Hill, 1998), all
subjects were tested in the morning to control for time of day effects.
Prior to each experimental session, subjects entered the laboratory and
were fitted with a heart rate monitor and were seated in an enclosed
room for a 5-minute quiet rest period. Following this rest period,
subjects completed the pre-exercise POMS. During the experimental rides,
participants were required to maintain a work rate corresponding with
60-75% of their heart rate reserve, and starting at five minutes into
the ride, were asked to rate their perceived exertion at five minute
intervals. Following the post-exercise cool-down period, participants
were again instructed to rest quietly in an enclosed room and the
post-exercise POMS was administered at five minutes after the cool down
period ended. As an experimental manipulation check, participants in the
distraction condition were asked a series of content questions regarding
either the video or reading to verify that they were, in fact, attending
to the distraction material during their exercise bout. Prior to
experimental rides, participants were told they would be asked a series
of questions about the material they were to attend during the ride. No
statistical hypothesis was tested for recall response differences across
distraction conditions, but participants in the distraction conditions
correctly recalled all responses to post-exercise content questions
asked by the investigator.
Data Analysis
In order to determine if participants in the distraction condition
would report greater post-exercise mood improvement compared to exercise
controls, a one-way ANOVA (exercise condition) was performed on TMD
changes (pre-post exercise) scores as the dependent variable. In
addition, a one-way MANOVA (exercise condition) was performed on POMS
subscale change scores across exercise conditions to determine POMS
subscale differences across exercise conditions. Finally, to determine
whether participants' effort sense appraisal of exercise intensity
was different across exercise conditions, a 3 x 5 (exercise condition x
RPE assessment) repeated measures ANOVA was performed with exercise
condition as the between-subjects variable and RPE as the
within-subjects variable.
Results
Results of an initial dependent t-test indicated that participants
had significantly improved post-exercise mood (M= 102.28, SD = 14.34)
compared to pre-exercise mood (M = 110.58, SD = 26.07, t(53) =2. 71,
p<.001) as measured by total mood disturbance scores. Pre-and
post-exercise mood means and standard deviations across the three
experimental conditions are displayed in Table 1. In order to determine
pre-post exercise differences collapsed across conditions, six separate
t-tests were performed for the separate subscale change scores. Results
indicated significant pre-post exercise mood state decreases for tension
(t(53) =2.45, p<.02) and depression (t(53)=3.09, p<.03), as well
as a significant pre-post exercise increase for vigor, supporting
similar pre-post exercise mood-change from previous research on affect
and exercise (McGowan, Talton, & Thompson, 1996).
Results of the one-way ANOVA on TMD change scores were
nonsignificant (F(2,52) =.63,p=.53) indicating that participants'
TMD change scores did not differ across conditions. Results of the
one-way MANOVA comparing exercise conditions on POMS subscale change
scores were also nonsignificant (Wilk's Lambda =.76, F(12,40)
=1.09, p=.37) indicating no significant mood change for POMS subscales
across conditions. Since participants' pre-exercise mood
differences may have accounted for nonsignificant MANOVA results, a
oneway MANCOVA was performed on post-exercise POMS subscales, using
pre-exercise POMS subscale scores as covariates. Results indicated a
nonsignificant exercise effect (Wilk's Lambda =.74, F (14,74) =.87,
p =.59). However, MANCOVA results did indicate an effect size that was
moderate ([R.sup.2] = .27) and that there may not have been sufficient
power to show a significant effect with highly correlated variables.
Correlations for pre- and post-exercise mood states are shown in Table
2. The lack of power was also supported in that follow-up ANCOVAs
indicated significant reductions in post-exercise TMD (F(9,52)
=2.39,p<.03), depression (F(9,52) =5.12, p<.0001), anger (F(9,52)
= 3.47, p<.003), and fatigue (F(9,52)=2.73, p<.009) and a
significant increase in vigor (F(9,52) =2.19, p<.05), after
controlling for pre-exercise mood states.
In order to examine differences across pre-and post-exercise POMS
scores, separate MANOVAs were run on pre-exercise POMS scores and
post-exercise POMS scores. Results from a one-way MANOVA on pre-exercise
POMS scores were nonsignificant (Wilk's Lambda =.65, F(14,88)
=1.57, p=.12), indicating that pre-exercise scores were equal. This
allowed for a second MANOVA examining differences on post-exercise POMS
scores. Results indicated no significant differences (Wilk's Lambda
=.81, F(14,88) =.72, p=.75) across exercise condition on post-exercise
mood. Results of a 3 x 5 (exercise condition x RPE assessment) repeated
measures ANOVA resulted in a nonsignificant time x condition interaction
(F(8,200) =1.56, p=. 14) as well as a nonsignificant main effect for
exercise condition (F(2,50) =.20,p=.82). There was a significant main
effect for time (F(4,200) =36.28,p<.0001, ES =.58), indicating
participants' RPE increased significantly across time.
Discussion
The major purpose of this study was to examine whether a single
bout of aerobic exercise combined with distraction activities improved
post-exercise mood state, compared to an exercise bout without any
external distraction activity. Results from the ore-post exercise mood
state comparison indicated that a single bout of activity improved
overall mood, tension, depression, and vigor, supporting previous
studies that have also found acute mood benefits from single exercise
bouts (Berger & Motl, 2000; Roth, 1989; Pierce & Pate, 1994).
This result also supports previous literature showing acute mood changes
in cycling exercise of similar duration and exercise intensities
(Farrell et al., 1986; Steptoe & Cox, 1988; Steptoe et al., 1993) in
members of normal populations.
The hypothesis that participants who exercised in the distraction
activity condition would report greater post-exercise mood improvement
compared to exercise controls was not supported. The intent of the
experimental distraction was to standardize the exercise mode and
content of the distraction material so that any motivational or
subjective nature of the distraction content would not mask or confound the effects of mood improvements from exercise per se. However, Berger
and Motl (2000) have indicated that personal enjoyment of the activity
is an important criterion for encouraging short-term improvements in
mood associated with exercise. The current distracting conditions, while
successful in directing exercisers' attention to the content, may
not have been personally enjoyable or sufficiently motivating to improve
post-exercise mood. In addition, nonsignificant pre-post exercise mood
differences across conditions may also have been due to ceiling effects
in regard to participants' pre-exercise mood. MANCOVA result
indicated that ore-exercise mood was not significantly different across
conditions, yet, as Table 1 indicates, pre-exercise mood states were
relatively low for these college students (Control TMD M = 119.53;
Reading TMD M = 107.63; Video TMD M = 105.35) compared to previous
exercise-mood research (i.e. Berger & Owen, 1992). Therefore,
according to a dose-response paradigm concerning psychological benefits
of exercise, lower initial mood scores may have contributed to a lack of
differential pre-post exercise mood changes across conditions. In short,
a ceiling effect on mood may have occurred such that since
participants' moods indicated a neutral profile, subsequent
exercise while engaging in task distraction may had minimal effects.
Further indirect support the contention that distractor tasks may
not have been motivating was provided in that, while one-way ANOVA
results comparing exercise conditions on TMD change scores were
nonsignificant, mood change scores were actually more favorable in the
control condition (M=13.35, SD=32.34) than reading (M=5.88, SD=17.84) or
video conditions (M=-5.96, SD=14.09). Participants in the exercise
control condition may have been able to engage in "time-out"
dissociative strategies without being constrained to attending to
distraction material that may not have been perceived as enjoyable.
Thus, the video and reading passage material which contained similar
standardized content may not have provided participants with the same
"cognitive freedom" to choose one's own distraction
activity that may have occurred during the control condition. Moreover,
advantages due to "time-out" in the current study were equated
across all levels of experimental manipulation. Future exercise-mood
studies might create experimental conditions in which exercise
distraction is either externally imposed or is self-selected by the
participant.
It has recently been found that enjoyment may mediate acute mood
changes associated with physical activity (Motl, Berger, & Leuschen,
2000). Berger and Owen (1986) examined the relationship between acute
bouts of swimming and mood changes and found beneficial mood changes
were not evident under disagreeable environmental conditions such as
high water and air temperatures. Social interaction enjoyment has been
as effective as meditation in lowering levels of tension (Hewitt &
Miller, 1981) and enjoyable music has been related to short-term mood
changes in college students (Wheeler, 1985). Individuals who have
experienced enjoyment during work and leisure, operationally defined as
flow, have reported higher positive affect scores than individuals who
did not experience enjoyment (Csikzentmihyli & LeFere, 1989).
Finally, Motl, Berger, and Lesuchen (2000) found that subjects engaged
in rock-climbing displayed greater mood benefits than a health education
class that included discussion of a rock-climbing video, and it was
concluded that enjoyment appeared to mediate the acute mood changes
associated with both activities and supported the enjoyment factor in
improving mood.
Therefore, it may be that the mediating benefit for mood
improvement and "time-out" efficacy during exercise may stem
directly from enjoyment of the activity. If subjects within the
distraction conditions had been able to select personally enjoyable
video or reading material, the combined effects of aerobic exercise and
distraction may have been more pronounced. Since enjoyment may mediate
mood effects, future studies should allow for personal selection of
distraction materials during exercise to determine the combined effects
of aerobic exercise and distraction activities on acute mood change. In
addition, a systematic problem with this type of research is the
inability to closely control for cognitive content in the control
condition. A suggestion for future research in this area might be to add
structure to the control condition that requires participants to engage
in a "boring" task (i.e. counting the number of specific
numerals contained within number-digit strings) during the exercise
control, along with a non-experimental manipulation control (let do)
while participants exercise. This would allow future researchers to more
accurately decipher cognitive content in the "true" control
condition.
Another explanation for the nonsignificant results may have been
the forced-choice of exercise mode. While current participants
self-reported they were regularly active, the mode used may not have
been a preferred exercise for some of the subjects. Berger and Owen
(1986) found that when comparing mood responses across swimming, body
conditioning, yoga, fencing, and lecture control condition, the mode of
activity (yoga) significantly affected mood improvement, further
supporting that mode selection mediate the effects of enjoyment on mood.
There were also no significant differences of subjective appraisal
of effort across exercise conditions as measured by RPE values.
Intuitively, it follows that since the distraction exercise conditions
did not result in impaired mood, that the distraction components of the
exercise activity were also insufficient in reducing effort sense during
exercise. It has been previously proposed that in order for dissociative
or "distractive" attentional strategies to be effective, there
must be some motivational aspect of the distractor activity (Morgan,
Horstmann, Cymermann, & Stokes, 1983; Rejeski & Kenny, 1987;
Russell & Weeks, 1994; Weinberg, Smith, Jackson, & Gould, 1984).
Individually-selected or self-motivating material may be more effective
in not only mood improvement through increased enjoyment but also by
increased dissociative aspects of exercise that would be reflected in
RPE scores. The relationship between mood and reaction to situational
factors (emotion) is proposed to be transactional in nature (Parkinson,
Totterdell, Briner, & Reynolds, 1996). A transactional relationship
would infer that general feeling states (mood) contribute to the
interpretation of, and reaction to, a situation (emotion) and the
subsequent emotional experience in turn contributes to mood. Therefore,
the positive emotions derived from motivational distractor activities
may be capable of post-exercise mood improvement. We suggest that future
researchers investigate the possibility that a dissociative or
distractor task with strong motivational properties may be more
effective than a task with low motivational properties.
An acknowledged limitation of the present study was that the amount
of distraction within the distraction conditions was not directly
measured. The present manipulation check in the distraction conditions
asked a series of twenty specific questions which all subjects correctly
responded to, verifying that at least some dissociation was occurring.
However, specific assessment regarding both associative and dissociative
attentional use have been used to gauge attentional strategy use in
runners (Masters & Lambert, 1989; Silva & Appelbaum, 1989) and
differences in relaxation and dissociation use have shown that the
methods in which attentional strategies are used are capable of
influencing factors such as running economy (Smith, Gill, Crews,
Hopewell, & Morgan, 1995), and may also be capable of influencing
mood. The accurate assessment of attentional style in recreational
exercisers appears to be needed if future research addresses the
"time-out" explanation for mood enhancement of distraction
activities. The Running Styles Questionnaire has shown acceptable
content validity and reliability (test-retest, r =.73; alpha =.69-.88)
to assess associative/dissociative strategy use, however this test was
developed for marathon runners and is not appropriate to assess amount
of distraction in recreational exercisers (Silva & Appelbaum, 1989).
Recent research (Wright & Loftus, 2000) has attempted to measure
self-reported dissociative experiences using the Dissociative
Experiences Scale (DES-C), which asks subjects how often they have
dissociative experiences compared to most other people. Yet this scale
measures dissociative patterns as a trait measure in non-exercise
situations and is also inappropriate for bout-specific measures of
distraction during exercise. Future assessment of distraction amount
might require a simple open-ended assessment, similar to Smith et al.
(1995) in which runners were asked what they were thinking about during
the run, and to rate (0 - 100%) the amount of time they attended to the
attentional instructions for both the total run and the final two
minutes of the run.
An additional limitation in the present study was that
post-exercise mood states were only examined at 5 minutes post-exercise.
Among noted limitations in the exercise and mood literature have been
the discrepancies related to the timing of mood measures following
exercise. For example, Steptoe et al. (1993) compared active and
inactive men's' mood following maximal and submaximal exercise
and found that mood scores were significantly different immediately
following and 30 minutes post-exercise. In addition, increases in
anxiety and mental fatigue have been shown in the first five minutes
following exercise, with reductions only emerging after 15-20 minutes
(Morgan, 1979; Steptoe & Bolton, 1988).
While the major hypotheses were not supported, important
information regarding environmental influences on the exercise-mood
relationship was obtained. The current study did provide evidence that
it is not the distraction activity, per se, that leads to positive mood
benefits and distraction from physical sensations of exercise (Berger
& Motl, 2000). Although not directly investigated in the current
study, it may be that personal enjoyment of the distraction activity may
mediate any mood benefits and may be a central criterion for short-term
mood improvement with exercise. Future research should focus on how
environmental influences or distraction activities interact with
objective measures of enjoyment (Kendzierski & DeCarlo, 1991) to
determine positive mood outcomes. The use of self-selected or
motivational distraction material may be a central component to
supporting the "time-out" hypothesis of the exercise-mood
enhancement relationship.
Address Correspondence To: William D. Russell, Ph.D., 1010 McAfee
Gymnasium, Eastern Illinois University, Charleston, IL 61920. E-mail:
[email protected]
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Pre- and Post-exercise Mood State
Subscale and Total Mood Disturbance Scores
Group TMD Ten Dep
Exercise Control (n=17)
Pre-test 119.53 9.35 5.53
(31.56) (7.57) (8.99)
Post-test 106.18 6.29 2.36
(21.99) (4.84) (3.18)
Reading (n=16)
Pre-test 107.63 6.63 3.25
(19.29) (5.04) (3.79)
Post-test 101.75 4.88 2.00
(17.46) (3.65) (3.41)
Video (n=20)
Pre-test 105.35 6.00 3.90
(24.89) (3.88) (7.31)
Post-test 99.40 5.15 1.75
(18.80) (3.75) (5.14)
Group Ang Vig Fat
Exercise Control (n=17)
Pre-test 5.59 15.41 7.82
(8.74) (6.02) (5.76)
Post-test 4.41 19.35 7.47
(7.32) (5.77) (6.80)
Reading (n=16)
Pre-test 2.63 17.06 6.94
(3.28) (4.85) (5.59)
Post-test 1.69 17.44 6.69
(4.08) (6.17) (4.39)
Video (n=20)
Pre-test 4.25 16.45 3.85
(6.24) (5.05) (3.48)
Post-test 2.90 18.40 4.30
(4.46) (6.34) (3.53)
Group Con
Exercise Control (n=17)
Pre-test 6.65
(4.95)
Post-test 5.00
(3.64)
Reading (n=16)
Pre-test 5.25
(2.54)
Post-test 3.94
(2.21)
Video (n=20)
Pre-test 3.70
(3.59)
Post-test 3.85
(3.12)
Note: TMD = total mood disturbance, Ten = tension, Dep = depression,
Ang = anger, Vig = vigor, Fat = fatigue, Con = confusion.
Table 2
Intercorrelations Between Pre-and Post-Exercise Profile of Mood States
Scores
PreTMD PreTEN PreDEP
PostTMD .55 ** .47 ** .50 **
PostTEN .39 * .42 * .37 *
PostDEP .62 ** .45 ** .68 **
PostANG .49 ** .45 ** .40 *
PostVIG -.03 .01 .01
PostFAT .44 * .43 * .40 *
PostCON .46 ** .32 * .41 *
PreANG PreVIG PreFAT
PostTMD .54 ** -.17 .36 **
PostTEN .41 * .03 -.29 *
PostDEP .66 ** -.14 .31 *
PostANG .54 ** -.03 .53 **
PostVIG .05 .45 ** .18
PostFAT .40 * .09 .51 **
PostCON .45 ** -.15 .35 *
PreCON
PostTMD .47 **
PostTEN .30 *
PostDEP .53 **
PostANG .66 **
PostVIG -.01
PostFAT .40 *
PostCON .44 *
* p < .05.
** p < .001.
References
American College of Sports Medicine (1998). ACSM position stand:
The recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and
maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness, and flexibility in
healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 30,
975-991.
Berger, B.G (1996). Psychological benefits of an active lifestyle:
What we know and what we need to know. Quest, 48, 330-353.
Berger, B.G. & McInnman, A. (1993). Exercise and the quality of
life. In R,N, Singer, M. Murphy, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of
research on sport psychology (729-760). New York: Macmillan Publishing.
Berger, B.G., Motl, R.W. (2000). Exercise and mood: A selective
review and synthesis of research employing the Profile of Mood States.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 69-92.
Berger, B.G. & Owen, D.R. (1986). Mood alteration with
swimming: A reexamination. In L. Vander Velden & J.H. Humprey
(Eds.), Psychology and sociology of sport: Current selected research
(97-113) New York: AMS Press.
Berger, B.G. & Owen, D.R. (1988). Stress reduction and mood
reduction and mood enhancement in four exercise modes: Swimming, body
conditioning, Hatha yoga, and fencing. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 59, 148-159.
Berger, B.G., & Owen, D.R. (1992). Mood alteration with yoga
and swimming: Aerobic exercise may not be necessary. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 75, 1331-1343.
Berger, B.G., Owen, D.R., & Man, F. (1993). A brief review of
literature and examination of acute mood benefits of exercise in
Czechoslovakian and United States swimmers. International Journal of
Sports Psychology, 24, 130-150.
Berger, B.G., Owen, D.R., Motl, R.W., & Parks, L. (1998).
Relationship between expectancy of psychological benefits and mood
alteration in joggers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 29,
1-16.
Borg, G.A.V. (1982). Psychophysiological bases of perceived
exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 14, 377-381.
Brown, D.R., Wang, Y., Ward, A., Ebbeling, C.B., Fortlage, L.,
Puleo, E., Benson, H., & Rippe, J.M. (1995). Chronic psychological
effects of exercise and exercise plus cognitive strategies. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 27, 765-775.
Csikzentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience
in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
815-822.
Ewing, J.H., Scott, D.G., Mendez, A.A., & McBride, T.J. (1984).
Effects of aerobic exercise upon affect and cognition. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 59, 407-414.
Farrell, P.A., Gustafson, A.B., Garthwaite, T.L., Kalkoff, R.K.,
Cowley, A.W., & Morgan, W.P. (1986). Influence of endogenous opiods
on the response of selected hormones to exercise in humans. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 61, 1051-1057.
Farrell, P.A., Gustafson, A.B., Morgan, W.P., & Pert, C.B.
(1987). Enkephalins, catehchomaines, and psychological mood alterations:
Effects of prolonged exercise. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 19, 347-353.
Hewitt, J., & Miller, R. (1981). Relative effects of meditation
vs. other activities on ratings of relaxation and enjoyment of others.
Psychological Reports, 48, 395-398.
Hill, D.W., Cureton, K.J., & Collins, M.A. (1988). Effect of
circadian rhythm and body temperature on oxygen uptake. Journal of
Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 28, 310-312.
International Society of Sport Psychology. (1992). Physical
activity and psychological benefits: A position statement from the
international society of sport psychology. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 4, 94-98.
Kendzierski, D., & DeCarlo, K.J. (1991). Physical activity
enjoyment scale: Two validation studies. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 13, 50-64.
Koltyn, K.F., Lynch, N.A., & Hill, D.W. (1998). Psychological
responses of brief exhaustive cycling exercise in the morning and the
evening. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 29, 145-156.
Masters, K., & Lambert, M.J. (1989). The relations between
cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury and performance
of marathon runners. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11,
161-170
McGowan, R.W., Talton, B.J., & Thompson, M. (1996). Changes in
scores on the Profile of Mood States following a single bout of physical
activity: Heart rate and changes in affect. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
83, 859-866.
McNair, D.M., Lorr, & Dropplemann, L.F. (1971). Profile of Mood
States manual. San Diego: Education and Industrial Testing Service.
Morgan, W.P. (1979). Anxiety reduction following acute physical
activity. Psychiatric Annals, 9, 141-147.
Morgan, W.P. (1985). Affective beneficence of vigorous physical
activity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 17, 94-100.
Morgan, W.P. (Ed.) (1997). Physical activity and mental health.
Washington, DC: Taylor and Francis.
Morgan, W.P, Horstmann, W.P, Cymermann, A., & Stokes, J.
(1983). Facilitation of physical performance by means of a cognitive
strategy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 251-264.
Motl, R.W., Berger, B.G., & Leuschen, P.S. (2000). The role of
enjoyment in the exercise-mood relationship. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 31, 347-363.
Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R.B., & Reynolds, S.
(1996). Changing moods: The psychology of mood and mood regulation.
London: London.
Pierce, E.F., & Pate, D.W. (1994). Mood alterations in older
adults following acute exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79,
191-194.
Rejeski, W.J., & Kenney, E. (1987). Distracting attentional
focus from fatigue: Does task complexity make a difference? Journal of
Sport Psychology, 9, 66-73.
Roth, D.L. (1989). Acute emotional and psychophysiological effects
of aerobic exercise. Psychophysiology, 26, 593-602.
Russell, W.D., & Weeks, D.L. (1994). Attentional style in
ratings of perceived exertion during physical exercise. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 78, 779-783.
Silva, J.M., & Appelbaum, M.I. (1989). Association-dissociation
patterns of United States Olympic marathon trial contestants. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 13, 185-192
Smith, A.L., Gill, D.L., Crews, D.J., Hopewell, R., Morgan, D.W.
(1995). Attentional strategy use by experienced distance runners:
Physiological and psychological effects. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 66, 142-150.
Steptoe, A., & Bolton, J. (1988). The short-term influence of
high and low intensity physical exercise on mood. Psychology and Health,
2, 91-106.
Steptoe, A., & Cox, S. (1988). Acute effects of aerobic
exercise on mood. Health Psychology, 7, 329-340.
Steptoe, A., Kearsely, N., & Walters, N. (1993). Acute mood
response to maximal and submaximal exercise in active and inactive men.
Psychology and Health, 8, 89-99.
Stevens, J. (1992). Applied multivariate statistics for the social
sciences. (2nd Ed.), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlaum Associates.
Thayer, R.E., Newman, J.R., & McClain, T.M. (1994).
Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising
energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67, 910-925.
US Department of Health and Human Services (1996). Physical
activity and health: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US
Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health
Promotion.
Weinberg, R.S., Smith, J., Jackson, A., & Gould, D. (1984).
Effect of association, dissociation, and positive self-talk strategies
on endurance performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
9, 25-32.
Wheeler, B.L. (1985). Relationship of personal characteristics to
mood and enjoyment after hearing live and recorded music to musical
taste. Psychology of Music, 13, 81-92.
Wright, D.B., & Loftus, E.F. (2000). Measuring dissociation:
Comparison of alternative forms of the Dissociative Experiences Scale.
Australian Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 28, 103-126.
William Russell, Brian Pritschet, Beth Frost, John Emmett, T.J.
Pelley, Judy Black, and Jill Owen Eastern Illinois University