The role of relative autonomy in post-exercise affect responding.
Lutz, Rafer ; Lochbaum, Marc ; Turnbow, Kimberly 等
Exercise psychology researchers have sought to determine causal
mechanisms responsible for affect in response to acute bouts of aerobic
exercise. Though numerous mechanisms have been investigated such as the
thermogenic hypothesis or Solomon's theory of acquired motivation,
no one mechanism or theory has gained consistent empirical support
(Petruzzello, Landers, Hatfield, Kubitz, & Salazar, 1991).
Interestingly, though psychological variables have always been prominent
in other exercise psychology research domains (e.g., exercise
adherence), these psychological variables have been overlooked in regard
to affective responding to an acute bout of exercise. A potential reason
for this oversight is that researchers may have believed that affective
responses to a "physical" activity (exercise) should have a
corresponding physiological origin (Tuson & Sinyor, 1993). However,
Tuson and Sinyor (1993) and more recently Ekkekakis and Petruzzello
(1999) have suggested that motivational and psychological variables bear
p articular need for investigation as determinants of exercise-induced
affective change. Tuson and Sinyor have stated that, "quite
conceivably it is not exercise per se that leads to changes in affect,
but rather psychological variables related to the experience of engaging
in exercise" (p. 114). Extraversion, sensation seeking, and
self-efficacy beliefs, for example, have been identified as potential
moderators or mediators of the exercise-affect relationship (Ekkekakis
& Petruzzello, 1999). Though not identified by the latter authors in
this regard, an exerciser's regulatory profile is a leading
candidate for study and is the focus of this investigation.
Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000)
differentiates regulatory processes through which an individual pursues
personal goals and activities. Participation in physical exercise is one
such example of a personal goal. Deci and Ryan (2000) identified six
regulatory processes that fall along the self-determined continuum
(nonself-determined to fully self-determined): amotivation or
non-regulation to external, introjected, identified, integrated and
intrinsic regulation. Individual behaviors, therefore, are said to
differ in the degree to which they are autonomous or self-determined.
For instance, some behaviors are engaged in with a sense of personal
choice or autonomy while external rewards or pressures control others.
The former constitute intrinsic forms of motivation, while the latter
constitute extrinsic motivational forms.
Pertinent to the present series of investigations, a central
feature of this theory is that goal pursuit and behaviors will lead to
positive or negative outcomes (such as improved affect) based on the
extent to which they are more or less self-determined. This is theorized
to occur because intrinsically regulated behaviors more fully satisfy
the basic needs of competence, autonomy, and relatedness, and this need
fulfillment contributes to a range of beneficial effects (Deci &
Ryan, 1995, 2000). Additionally, behaviors originally motivated by
extrinsic sources can become more autonomous by means of
"organismic integration" (Deci & Ryan, 1995, P. 34). While
an individual may, at first, pursue an activity to satisfy external
pressures or to gain rewards, his or her reasons for engaging in this
activity may become integrated into his or her self-concept, making it a
more self-determined activity. Self-determined behaviors, therefore, are
theorized to lead to a variety of positive outcomes because of their
ability to provide basic need fulfillment and contribute effectively to
self-concept (Deci & Ryan, 1995).
Vallerand and Rousseau (2001) contend that motivation for sport and
exercise has affective consequences as well as cognitive and behavioral
consequences. Additionally, they proposed that such consequences will be
increasingly positive as an individual experiences more intrinsic,
self-determined forms of motivation. According to Vallerand's
(1997) hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, there
are three levels of generality at which an individual may be motivated
in either a self- or nonself-determined manner. The broadest level, the
global level, refers to a trait-like orientation to adopt intrinsic,
extrinsic, or amotivated forms of motivation. The second level is
referred to as the contextual level. This level refers to an
individual's typical motivational profile in a given context, such
as exercise environments. The most specific level is the situational
level that refers to an individual's self-determined motivational
pattern at a specific point in time or with respect to a specific act
ivity.
In support of Vallerand's (1997) and Deci and Ryan's
(1985,2000) theorizing, self-determined motivation has been linked to
psychological well-being and mental health (Ryan, Rigby, & King,
1993), persistence of motivated behaviors (Ryan, Frederick, Lepes,
Rubio, & Sheldon, 1997), and well-being, positive affect, and
vitality (Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996). More specifically, Sheldon
and colleagues (1996) found that trait perceptions of autonomy were
positively and significantly (p < .05) related to total well-being (r
= .36), positive affect (r .28), and vitality (r = .34). Most recently,
Sheldon, Elliot, Kim, and Kasser (2001) found that positive affect
during participants' most satisfying events was predicted by
ratings that this event met needs for autonomy (r range = .31 to .51),
competence (r range = .39 to .59) and relatedness (r range = .21 to
.33), and that these needs may be among a fundamental or core set
necessary for human satisfaction. Considering these findings, it is
logical to speculate that th e reasons! goals an individual has for
engaging in exercise may influence his or her affective response to its
completion.
Despite the theoretical appeal, only a handful of studies have been
conducted to examine the relation between positive affective outcomes in
sport and exercise and intrinsic motivation (Blanchard & Vallerand,
1996, 1998, as cited in Vallerand & Rosseau, 2001; Frederick,
Morrison, & Manning, 1996; McAuley & Tammen, 1989). Blanchard
and Vallerand (1996), as detailed by Vallerand and Rosseau (2001), found
that positive affective responses immediately after a basketball game
were positively related to intrinsic and identified forms of motivation.
In another unpublished investigation, the authors reported that
intrinsically motivated individuals who engaged in an exercise-based
weight loss program reported greater levels of experienced positive
emotion(Blanchard & Vallerand, 1998, as cited by Vallerand &
Rosseau, 2001). Additionally, McAuley and Tammen (1989) found that
successful basketball players reported greater levels of enjoyment,
effort, and competence, indicating greater levels of intrinsic
motivation. Fr ederick and colleagues (1996) found that exercisers who
reported greater levels of competence, enjoyment, and social
satisfaction (indicative of intrinsic motivation) reported greater
enjoyment attributable to exercise. Though limited in number, these
studies conducted in sport settings support the idea that affective
response to exercise may be, in part, moderated by an individual's
self-determined motivational profile. Yet, to further corroborate this
link, investigations are needed to test these relationships in exercise
contexts specifically.
Hence, the purpose of the present series of investigations was to
test the predictions of SDT and the hierarchical model of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation specific to affective consequences attributable to
exercise participation. The present studies also uniquely add to the
extant literature in that they utilized a self-determined motivation
measure developed specifically for assessment in exercise settings, and
used affect measures commonly used in the research examining
exercise-affect relationships. None of the aforementioned investigations
have employed these measures.
EXPERIMENT 1
The primary goal of Experiment 1 was to determine if perceived
typical affective response to exercise could be predicted by
participants' self-determined motivational orientations. To achieve
this end, we asked participants to fill out the Exercise Motivation
Scale (EMS) a context-specific measure of self-determined motivation in
exercise (Li, 1999). Subsequently, they were asked to engage in a brief
imagery exercise designed to elicit their typical, contextual reaction
to exercise participation. In this manner, we sought to determine if the
contextual measure of motivation could predict contextual consequences
of exercise as Vallerand's (1997) model would predict. The
examination of the contextual measure of motivation allowed for the
testing of several hypotheses.
Hypothesis one posited that mean values for intrinsic motivation
types, integrated and identified regulation will be greater than those
for less identified forms of motivation. This pattern of motivation has
been found using this scale in college-student exercisers (Li, 1999) and
using a related scale to examine sport participants (Pelletier, Fortier,
Vallerand, Tuson, Briere, & Blais, 1995). Conducting a t-test
comparing the mean of intrinsic motivation types, integrated regulation
and identified regulation with the mean of introjected regulation,
external regulation and a motivation tested this hypothesis. Hypothesis
two predicted that positive correlations would be found between the
three intrinsic forms of motivation, integrated regulation, identified
regulation and the dependent variable of interest, positive affect.
Conversely, negative relationships were predicted between the other
motivational forms (introjected and external regulation, and
amotivation) and positive affect. Last, we predicted that the degree to
which an individual reported self-determined motives for exercise
participation would positively relate to affective outcomes. To quantify self-determination, Deci and Ryan's (2000) Relative Autonomy Index
(RAI, called the Self-Determination Index as described in Vallerand,
1997) was computed, and correlating the RAI with positive affect tested
this relationship.
Method
Participants
Participants were 141 volunteer, college-aged university students
(71 male, 70 female). All participants were recruited via personal
communication from introductory psychology courses at a large
southwestern university. Participants received credit towards completion
of research participation required for successful completion of the
introductory psychology course. Descriptive data are presented in Table
1.
Measures
EMS. The EMS includes 31 potential reasons or lack of reasons for
engaging in exercise participation. Participants are asked to indicate
their agreement with each statements as a reason why they exercise on a
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 "strongly disagree" to 6
"strongly agree." This scale was developed specifically to
assess self-determined motivational orientations in exercise, as opposed
to sport, settings. It has demonstrated adequate levels of internal
consistency [alpha] = .71 to .90), test-retest reliability (r =.78 to
.88), and includes all eight dimensions along the self-determination
continuum. Other SDT-based measures developed for use in sport or
exercise settings have not identified all eight dimensions acceptably
(Li, 1999).
Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). The PANAS
developed by Watson, Clark, and Tellegen (1988) includes 20 adjective words, 10 describing negative mood states and 10 describing positive
mood states. The response set ranges from 1 "very slightly or not
at all" to 5 "extremely." The internal consistency alphas
range from .86 to .90 for positive affect (PA) and from .84 to .87 for
negative affect (NA) (Watson et al., 1988). Participants were asked
complete the adjective checklist in response to their feelings
immediately after their imagined acute bout of exercise. In the present
investigation, only the PA subscale was scored.
Participants were asked to complete a survey requiring
approximately 45 minutes of their time. Within this exercise-focused
survey, participants completed the EMS and an imagery session concerning
exercise and affect. Participants were instructed to imagine completing
a 30-minute exercise session. The mode and intensity of exercise were
self-chosen. After choosing their exercise, participants were asked to
take a few minutes to imagine completing this session. Next,
participants were instructed to immediately complete the PANAS with
regards to the imaged exercise session.
Results
Prior to analyzing our main hypotheses, we first determined whether
any gender differences existed for either the EMS or affective
reporting. A series of one-way ANOVAs revealed that no significant (p
< .05) differences existed between males and females.
To examine our first hypothesis, that participants (who have
self-selected to participate in a fitness based class) would have
intrinsic motivation and integrated and identified regulation as their
primary motivational orientation, the means were examined by conducting
a dependent t-test comparing the mean of the self-determined subscales
(M [+ or -] SD 4.27 + .80) with the mean of the non-self-determined
subscales (M [+ or -] SD 2.44 + .79). The results, t(1, 141) = 20.79, p
< .001, verified this hypothesis. This, in addition to visual
inspection of the pattern of means, confirmed the hypothesized
relationship (see Table 1). Simple correlations (see Table 2) were
computed to examine our second and third hypotheses concerning the
relationship among EMS subscales, RAI, and exercise-induced affect. As
seen in Table 3, participants reported a fairly high level of positive
affect in response to the imagery session concerning their self-chosen
mode and intensity of 30 minutes of exercise. The simple correlations
with positive affect were significant (p < .05) and in the
hypothesized direction for identified and integrated regulation, each
type of intrinsic motivation, and the RAT. Though negative in direction,
the correlations between amotivation, external regulation and positive
affect were not statistically significant. Thus, the modest to moderate
magnitude correlations generally supported our hypotheses with regards
to the proposed relationships along the self-determination continuum
with positive affect reporting in response to an acute bout of exercise.
EXPERIMENT 2
Experiment 2 was conducted to replicate the findings of Experiment
1 within a setting that would provide greater ecological validity; and,
additionally, to address several limitations inherent in the design of
the first investigation. In particular, we sought to measure
participants' affective response to exercise using a paradigm more
typical to this line of investigation (i.e., directly after an actual
exercise bout); implement the use of additional measures of affect;
control for effects of pre-exercise affect; and reduce testing effects
by separating the presentation of the EMS and affective measures in
time. Therefore, Experiment 2 explored whether contextual motivational
orientations could predict situational affective response to exercise.
Participants completed the EMS two days before they returned to
their scheduled class meeting time to engage in a bout of aerobic
exercise. Affect was measured with two scales currently receiving
popular use at three time points: immediately before exercise,
immediately after exercise, and 15 minutes after completion of exercise.
In this manner, entering pre-exercise affect into a regression as a
predictor simultaneously with RAT to predict post-exercise affect would
control for the effects of participants' trait or state dependent
affect (i.e., affect in anticipation of an exercise session, affect as a
result of stimulus prior to class, or trait-orientations to experience
positive affect). Finally, this set-up allowed for an additional test of
Vallerand's (1997) hierarchical model by determining whether
motivation at the contextual level influences situational outcomes.
In Experiment 2, the predictions made by Hypotheses 1-3 in the
first investigation were retained and retested. In order to determine
whether pre-exercise affect influenced post-exercise affective reports,
and to control for potential influences of global or contextual
motivational orientations that may influence contextual affect
(participants knew they would be exercising) or global affect (trait
autonomy has been shown to relate to general well-being, Sheldon et al.,
1996), a fourth hypothesis was advanced. This fourth hypothesis
predicted that contextual motivation would contribute unique variance to
the prediction of post-exercise affect above and beyond pre-exercise
affect.
Method
Participants
Participants were 99 volunteer, college-aged university students.
All participants were female and recruited via personal communications
from aerobic and/or fitness courses at a large southwestern university.
Descriptive data are presented in Table 1.
Measures
Activation Deactivation Adjective Checklist (AD ACL). In addition
to utilizing the PANAS as in Experiment 1, the AD ACL (Thayer, 1989,
Appendix A) was used to assess affect. The AD ACL is a 20-item
self-report inventory that assesses the following two-arousal
dimensions: energetic arousal (EA) and tense arousal (TA). EA and TA are
compatible to dimensions of positive activation (positive affect) and
negative activation (negative affect) in Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, and
Tellegen's (1999) model, respectively. In the present
investigation, only EA was scored. The AD ACL's reliability and
construct validity are well established (Thayer, 1986).
Ratings of Perceived Exertion (RPE). RPE was completed based on
Borg's (1985) 10-point graded scale prior to, during, and post
exercise. This scale is a self-report, psychophysical measure of
perceived effort. The scale ranges from 0 "nothing at all" to
10 "very, very strong" to a single point of maximal. RPE as
measured in a variety of graded scales has been shown to be a valid and
reliable indicator of relative fatigue for over 30 years (Pollock &
Wilmore, 1990).
Procedure
After receiving permission from the class instructor to perform
research in aerobics classes, participants were approached within the
class period and provided an overview of the investigation. Then they
were given the approved informed consent as approved by the second
author's University Human Subject Board to read and sign. The
investigation required the participants to complete a survey that
contained the EMS and demographic information. After completion of the
survey and during a different class period, participants completed a
30-minute step aerobic class. Participants completed ratings of
perceived exertion and affect (PANAS and AD ACL) prior to, during cool
down, and 15 minutes after completion of the aerobic session.
Data Analyses
First, we conducted repeated-measures multivariate analyses of
variance on the affect scores to verify, as would be predicted, that
participants reported increased positive affect from baseline values. To
examine our major hypotheses, simple correlations among the EMS facets,
RAI, and positive affect were examined. Next, regression analyses were
conducted to determine the predictive power of RAI over and above that
of pre-exercise affect.
Due to logistical constraints (i.e., time constraints and the
wishes of the instructor for the given class periods), 37 of the 99
participants failed to complete the PANAS during any of the three time
points and the AD ACL at the delayed post-test. For the analyses
examining pre-and post- AD ACL, therefore, data from all study
participants (N = 99) were entered into the analyses. The analyses
examining the PANAS and delayed AD ACL delayed post-test utilized the
data from the 62 participants who had complete data sets.
Results
To verify the similarity of intensity of the aerobic sessions, RPE
was collected mid-way through the aerobic session and also at the two
time points during recovery. Participants reported statistically similar
(p> .05) RPE values (M [+ or -] SD, During: 5.43 2.06, 5.00 [+ or -]
1.94; Post: 5.88 2.33, 5.47 2.25; Delayed post: 4.03 1.45) that
corresponded to strong (moderate intensity) on the Borg scale in
response to the regardless their grouping, n = 62 or n 99, respectively.
To determine whether self-reported affect followed expected
temporal patterns, a series of repeated measure MANOVAs were conducted.
In each analysis, the Huynh-Feldt epsilon was examined and multivariate
statistics were used when [epsilon] < .75. For the 62 participants
who completed the PANAS and AD ACL at all measurement time points (pre,
post, and delayed post), the main effect for Time was significant, F(2,
122) 4.17, [epsilon] = .79, p <.05. Univariate followup tests were
conducted to determine which dependent variables were contributing to
the multivariate Time effect, and there were significant Time main
effects for both PA (PANAS), F(2,60) = 19.87, p <.05, and for EA (AD
ACL), F(2,60) = 25.32, p <.001. Finally, the data for the 37
participants who failed to complete the PANAS and the delayed AD ACL
were examined in addition to the 62 who had complete data. These 99
participants had completed the AD ACL at pre and posttest, and the main
effect for Time was significant, Wilks' [lambda] = .83, F( 1,98) =
20.34, p <.001. In all analyses, inspection of the means suggested
that the main effects for Time were due to increased and generally
sustained positive affect reporting (compared to pre-exercise positive
affect) in response to the bout of aerobic exercise (see Table 3).
As in Experiment 1, to examine our first hypothesis that
participants who have self-selected to participate in a fitness based
class would have intrinsic motivation and integrated and identified
regulation as their primary motivational orientation, a dependent t-test
was conducted on the means corresponding to self-determined (M [+ or -]
SD = 4.24 [+ or -] .84) and non-self-determined motivation (M [+ or -]
SD = 2.38 .65) and verified hypothesis one, t(1, 98) = 16.28, p <
.001. In addition, visual inspection of the pattern of means confirmed
the hypothesized relationship (see Table 1). Simple correlations (see
Table 2) were examined to test our second and third hypotheses
concerning the relationship among EMS subscales, the RAI, and
exercise-induced positive affect. The simple correlations were
significant (p < .05) and in the hypothesized direction for all
relationships. Thus, the modest to moderate magnitude correlations with
positive affect strongly supported our hypotheses with regards to
relationships al ong the continuum of amotivation to intrinsic
motivation. Last, to examine the predictive ability of the RAI over and
above that of pre-exercise affect, hierarchical regression analyses were
performed (see Table 4). The RAI significantly (p <.05) predicted
unique variance (13 and 6%) in affect immediately after exercise and
also significantly predicted affect (4%) 15 minutes after exercise
completion for the PANAS.
Discussion
The results of the present series of investigations clearly suggest
that self-determined motivational orientations have an impact on
positive feelings reported by participants post-exercise. Additionally,
and in support of Vallerand's (1997) hierarchical model of
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, this research indicates that
contextual motivation appears to influence situational consequences.
Finally, this research supports the exercise adherence literature, which
proposes that enjoyment and intrinsic motivation are important elements
in the adoption and maintenance of habitual exercise programs.
The present series of investigations uniquely contribute to the
exercise psychology literature for several reasons. First, few studies
have examined motivational variables and the influence they may have on
the affective response to exercise (Blanchard & Vallerand, 1996,
1998, as cited by Vallerand & Rosseau, 2001; Frederick et al., 1996;
McAuley & Tammen, 1989; Tate, Petruzzello, & Lox, 1995; Treasure
& Newbery, 1998). Next, even fewer studies have examined
self-determined motivational types in this respect (Blanchard &
Vallerand, 1996, 1998 as cited by Vallerand & Rosseau, 2001;
Frederick et al., 1996). Finally, seldom have these studies examined
affective change after exercise bouts using the typical paradigm (i.e.,
affective pretest, acute exercise bout, and posttests with various delay
periods). Though Treasure and Newbery (1998) and Tate et al. (1995) have
examined self-efficacy states as predictors of affective response using
similar procedures, no known study has examined motivational variables
derive d from SDT in this manner. Two studies examining SDT in this area
have examined affective or enjoyment responses after sport participation
(Blanchard & Vallerand, 1996, as cited by Vallerand & Rosseau,
2001; McAuley & Tammen, 1989), and the one known study examining
affective response to exercise failed to control for pre-exercise affect
and did not assess situational affect, but rather contextual affect
(Frederick et al., 1996). In this manner, Experiment 1 corroborates the
findings of Frederick et al., and Experiment 2 extends their work by
including situational outcomes. Though researchers have focused on
physiological explanations for affective changes attributed to exercise,
clearly a portion of the variance in these situations can be explained
by psychological/motivational variables. It may be of interest to
determine how physiological variables might ultimately influence
psychological responses and vice-versa.
Of the studies that closest approximate the present set of
investigations, Tate and colleagues (1995) found that self-efficacy
states before exercise predicted EA at 30 minutes post-moderate
intensity exercise (55% of VO2max). This corroborates the findings of
the present investigation indicating that positive affective outcomes
ascribed to moderate intensity exercise in the present investigation may
be, at least partially, explained by motivational variables. It is of
interest, in the future, to determine which constructs (self-efficacy,
self-determination or both) have the primary influence upon positive
affect in exercise situations. Also, similar to the present
investigation, Frederick et al. (1996) found that self-reported high
adherers to physical activity reported greater competence, enjoyment,
and social satisfaction due to exercise indicating a more
self-determined motivational profile in comparison to low adherers. In
addition, ratings of competence, enjoyment, and social motives
correlated positive ly with post-workout ratings of enjoyment. Frederick
and colleagues' findings support the present results and suggest a
link between motivation, exercise-related affect, and exercise
participation. Future research should examine the meaningfulness and
intricacies of all of these relationships concurrently.
The present research extends the research conducted by Frederick
and colleagues (1996) by using a measure of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation developed specifically for exercise contexts, examining
affect directly after an actual exercise bout, using affect measures
commonly used to examine exercise-induced affective changes, and
controlling for pre-exercise affect (Frederick and colleagues only
examined contextual enjoyment due to exercise). In the present research,
situational affect (Experiment 2) was measured in addition to a
contextual affect (Experiment 1). In both cases, contextual motivational
orientations had an influence on affective reports, thus corroborating
Vallerand's (1997) theorizing that contextual motivation can have
both contextual and specific consequences. This is noteworthy because
this is the first known research to examine consequences following
exercise within Vallerand's framework using the traditional
paradigm of the study of affective responses to acute exercise.
It is of interest to note the relation between contextual autonomy
(RAI) and pre-exercise affect in Experiment 2 (EA: r .24; PA: r .47). It
follows the theorizing by Vallerand (1997) that contextual motivation
for exercise influenced situational affect (i.e., participants who are
autonomously motivated exhibited more pre-exercise positive feelings
knowing they were going to be exercising). It may also be, however, that
global (trait) self-determination had an influence on situational
affect. Higher trait autonomy levels are positively correlated with
daily wellbeing (Sheldon et al., 1996). Sheldon and colleagues found
that trait (global) and state (situational) feelings of competence or
autonomy predicted positive feelings. The relation between contextual
motivation and situational affect at the pre-test may, in part, be due
to similar, positive effects of global orientations on both variables.
This would be predicted by Vallerand's model as well. It remains
for future studies, however, to determine any diffe rential effects of
global, contextual, and situational motivation on "average"
well-being and situational positive affect after exercise. The results
of the present investigation do confirm that contextual motivation
appears to have an influence on both pre- and post-exercise affect. This
finding has strong implications for the manner in which affective
changes due to exercise is 'currently studied. Depending on the
nature of the relationship, exercise participation may influence
affective response before exercise, in addition to post exercise. The
correlation between contextual autonomy and pre-exercise affect suggests
that it may be important to study affective response to exercise within
the context of an individual's average daily affect.
Similar to Frederick and colleagues' (1996) study, other
researchers have found that exercise participation and adherence appear
to be related to autonomous motivation (Oman & McAuley, 1993; Ryan
et al., 1997). Oman and McAuley (1993) noted that different goals cause
an individual to begin an exercise program (usually health benefits) in
comparison to the goals with the goals that lead to continued
involvement in such a program (such as social reasons). Oman and McAuley
described how situations may influence the level of intrinsic
motivation.
Exercisers that pursue fitness for controlled reasons should
exhibit less intrinsic motivation than those that pursue fitness for
less controlling reasons. Thompson and Wankel (1980) is an example of a
study where this relationship is supported. They found that exercisers
who perceived more choice in the physical activities they pursued were
more likely to exhibit feelings of self-determination, intrinsic
motivation and better program attendance. Oman and McAuley (1993)
further examined the relation between intrinsic motivation and exercise
behavior by administering the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) to
community aerobics exercisers. It was found that participants with
higher intrinsic motivation scores attended more exercise classes. Also,
participants' confidence in their intentions to continue to
exercise after the end of the exercise program was related to intrinsic
motivation scores measured before program initiation. These findings, in
addition to the present findings, suggest that motivation infl uences
affect and participation. Whether affect mediates that relationship
between self-determined motivation and exercise participation remains to
be studied.
Overall, the present research confirms Tuson and Sinyor's
(1993) suggestion that exercise psychologists should broaden their scope
to include psychological/motivational variables in the investigation of
the exercise-affect relationship. Self-determined motivational types
predicted both contextual and situational positive affect in exercise
settings. The present studies are limited by the use of small,
college-age samples that appear to exhibit a high level of autonomous
regulation for exercise. Nonetheless, these studies confirm the
predictions of SDT (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1995, 2000) and the
hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Vallerand,
1997), and extend our understanding of the exercise-affect relationship.
Table 1
Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Participants in Both Studies
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
(n=141) (n=99)
Variable Mean SD Mean SD
Descriptive
Age 20.14 2.94 20.46 2.32
EMS subscales
Amotivation 1.64 0.88 1.69 0.87
Extrinsic Motivation
External Regulation 2.18 1.10 2.08 0.86
Introjected Regulation 3.52 1.27 3.38 1.06
Identified Regulation 4.84 0.91 4.92 0.87
Integrated Regulation 4.15 0.90 4.18 0.96
Intrinsic Motivation
To learn 3.41 1.24 3.43 1.13
To accomplish things 4.40 0.98 4.21 0.96
To experience sensations 4.58 0.97 4.46 0.99
Relative Autonomy Index 12.75 6.54 12.78 7.48
Note. EMS = Exercise Motivation Scale.
Table 2
Correlations Among Post Exercise Positive Affect and EMS Subscales and
Total RAI Score
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
Post Imagine Pre
PA PA EA
n 141 62 99
EMS subscales
Amotivation -.09 -.38 ** -.24 *
Extrinsic Motivation
External Regulation -.08 -.31 ** -.15
Introjected Regulation .19 * -.11 .05
Identified Regulation .43 *** .32 * .14
Integrated Regulation .39 *** .35 ** .13
Intrinsic Motivation
To learn .21 * .25 * .11
To accomplish things .42 *** .37 ** .21 *
To experience .42 *** .39 ** .21 *
sensations
Relative Autonomy .36 *** .47 *** .24 *
Index
Experiment 2
Post Delayed Post
PA EA PA EA
n 62 99 62 62
EMS subscales
Amotivation -.44 *** -.33 ** -.38 ** -.32 *
Extrinsic Motivation
External Regulation -.40 ** -.22 * -.38 ** -.17
Introjected Regulation -.15 .03 -.12 .05
Identified Regulation .32 * .39 *** .34 ** .29 *
Integrated Regulation .38 ** .35 *** .38 ** .20
Intrinsic Motivation
To learn .26 * .31 ** .23 .21
To accomplish things .41 ** .42 *** .42 ** .33 **
To experience .31 * .41 *** .30 * .24
sensations
Relative Autonomy .52 *** .45 *** .49 *** .34 **
Index
Note. EMS = Exercise Motivation Scale; RAI = Relative Autonomy Index; PA
Positive Affect (PANAS); and EA Energetic Arousal (AD ACL). *** p <.001;
** p<.01; * p <.05 Procedure
Table 3
Variable Means (Standard Deviations, Effect Sizes, and Sample Size for
Self-Reported Affect for Both Studies)
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
Variable Post Imagine Pre Post
PANAS
Positive Affect 38.23 (7.34) 25.89 (9.42) 28.29 (9.68)
ES -- -- .25
n 141 62 62
AD ACL
Energetic Arousal 23.29 (7.29) 29.61 (7.24)
ES -- -- .87
n 62 62
Energetic Arousal 23.36 (6.44) 26.72 (7.24)
ES -- .52
n 99 99
Ratings of Perceived 5.04 (1.98) 5.54 (2.26)
Exertion
Experiment 2
Variable Delayed Post
PANAS
Positive Affect 27.90 (10.03)
ES .21
n 62
AD ACL
Energetic Arousal 28.50 (7.53)
ES .71
n 62
Energetic Arousal
ES
n
Ratings of Perceived 4.12 (1.64)
Exertion
Note. ES = effect size.
Table 4
Regression Analyses
Model Results
[R.sup.2] [R.sup.2.sub.adjusded]
Criterion Variable:
Immediate Post
AD ACL (a)
Pretest .17 .17
RAI .31 .29
PANAS (c)
Pretest .41 .40
RAI .47 .45
Criterion Variable:
Delayed Post
AD ACL (e)
Pretest .33 .32
RAI .37 .35
PANAS (g)
Pretest .47 .46
RAI .50 .49
Model Results Coefficient Results
[R.sup.2.sub.change] Beta t
Criterion Variable:
Immediate Post
AD ACL (a)
Pretest .17 .33 3.76
RAI .13b .38 4.28
PANAS (c)
Pretest .41 .51 4.74
RAI .06 (d) .28 2.57
Criterion Variable:
Delayed Post
AD ACL (e)
Pretest .33 .52 4.89
RAI .04 (f) .20 1.86
PANAS (g)
Pretest .47 .58 5.59
RAI .04 (h) .21 2.05
Coefficien
t Results
p
Criterion Variable:
Immediate Post
AD ACL (a)
Pretest .000
RAI .000
PANAS (c)
Pretest .000
RAI .012
Criterion Variable:
Delayed Post
AD ACL (e)
Pretest .000
RAI 0.68
PANAS (g)
Pretest .000
RAI .045
Note. (a)Final model, F(2,96) = 21.22, p<.001;
(b)[F.sub.change] (1,96) = 18.35, p <.001;
(c)Final model, F(2,59) = 26.19, p <.001;
(d)[F.sub.change] (1,59) = 6.65, p <.05;
(e)Finl model, F(2,59) = 17.45, p <.001;
(f)[F.sub.change] (1,59) = 3.46, p <.05;
(g)Final model, F(2,59) = 29.72, p <.001;
(h)[F.sub.change] (1,59) = 4.19, p <.05.
References
Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (1996). On the relations
between situational motivation and situational consequences in
basketball. Unpublished manuscript, University of Quebec, Montreal,
Canada.
Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. 1. (1998). On the relations
between situational motivation and situational consequences toward
exercise. Unpublished manuscript, University of Quebec, Montreal,
Canada.
Borg, G. (1985). An introduction to Borg's RPE scale. Ithaca,
NY: Mouvement Publications.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis
for true self-esteem. In M. H. Kemis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and
self-esteem. (pp. 31-49). New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and
"why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination
of behavior. Psychological Inquiry 11, 227-268.
Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (1999). Acute aerobic
exercise and affect: Current status, problems and prospects regarding
dose-response. Sports Medicine, 28(5), 337-374.
Frederick, C.M., Morrison, C., & Manning, T. (1996). Motivation
to participate, exercise affect, and outcome behaviors toward physical
activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 691-701.
Li, F. (1999). The exercise motivation scale: Its multifaceted
structure and construct validity. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
11, 97-115.
McAuley E., & Tammen, V. V. (1989). The effects of subjective
and objective competitive outcomes on intrinsic motivation. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology 11, 84-93.
Oman, R., & McAuley, E. (1993). Intrinsic motivation and
exercise behavior. Journal of Health Education, 24, 232-238. Pelletier,
L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Tuson, K. M., Briere, N. M.,
& Blais, M. R. (1995). Toward anew measure of intrinsic motivation,
extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: The Sport Motivation
Scale (SMS). Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 17, 35-53.
Pelletier, L.G., Fortier, M.S., Vallerand, R.J., Tuscon, K.M.,
Briere, N.M. & Blais, M.R. (1995) Toward a measure of intrinsic
motivation, extrinsic motivation and amotivation in sport: The Sports
Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
17,35-53.
Petruzzello, S.J., Landers, D.M., Hatfield, B.D., Kubitz, K.A.,
& Salazar, W. (1991). A meta-analysis on the anxiety reducing
effects of acute and chronic exercise. Sports Medicine, 11, 143-182.
Pollock, M. L., & Wilmore, J. H. (1990). Exercise in health and
disease. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Ryan, R. M., Fredrick, C. M., Lepes, D., Rubio, N., & Sheldon,
K. M. (1997). Intrinsic motivation and exercise adherence. International
Journal of Sport Psychology 28, 335-354.
Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of
religious internalization and their relations to religious orientations
and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65,586-596.
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y, & Kasser, T. (2001).
What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate
psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
80,325-339.
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes
for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the person.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,1270-1279.
Tate, A. K., Petruzzello, S. J., & Lox, C. L. (1995).
Examination of the relationship between self-efficacy and affect at
varying levels of aerobic exercise intensity. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 25, 1922-1936.
Thayer, R. E. (1986). Activation-deactivation adjective checklist:
Current overview and structural analysis. Psychological Reports, 58,
607-614.
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Thompson, C. E., & Wankel, L. M. (1980). The effects of
perceived activity choice upon frequency of exercise behavior. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 436-443.
Treasure, D.C., & Newbery, D. M. (1998). Relationship between
self-efficacy, exercise intensity, and feeling states in a sedentary population during and following an acute bout of exercise. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 1-11.
Tuson, K. M., & Sinyor, D. (1993). On the affective benefits of
acute aerobic exercise: Taking stock after twenty years of research. In
P. Seraganian (Ed.), Exercise psychology: The influence of physical
exercise on psychological processes (pp. 122-145). New York: Wiley.
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic
and extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (pp. 271-360). New York: Academic Press.
Vallerand, R. J., & Rosseau, F. L. (2001). Intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation in sport and exercise: A review using the
Hierarchical Model of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation. In R. N.
Singer, H. A. Hausenbias, & C. M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport
psychology (pp. 389-414). New York: Wiley.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development
and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
1063-1070.
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, I., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The
two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings,
evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820-838.
Address Correspondence To: Marc Lochbaum, Ph.D., Department of
Heath, Exercise, and Sport Sciences, Texas Tech University, Box 43011,
Lubbock, TX 79409-3011. Telephone: (806) 742-3371. Fax: (806) 742-1688.
E-mail:
[email protected]