The benefits of and barriers to strength training among college-age women.
Harne, Amanda J. ; Bixby, Walter R.
The American College of Sports Medicine (2000) recently highlighted
the importance of strength training, along with aerobic and flexibility
exercises, as key elements to a "well-rounded training
program" for healthy adults. Interestingly, gender differences were
not highlighted as a possible moderating factor for deriving benefits
from strength training, suggesting that both men and women should
participate in strength training. Additionally, various investigations
have shown that women can benefit both physiologically and
psychologically from a strength training program (Ebben & Jensen,
1998; Fleck, 1998; Freedson, 2000; Marble, 1997). However, despite the
benefits associated with strength training, the number of women who
participate in strength training programs is low (Ebben & Jensen,
1998).
Strength training has several physiological benefits for women,
including increasing muscular strength (Brown & Harrison, 1986;
Chilibeck, Calder, Sale, & Webber, 1998; Cullinen & Caldwell,
1998; Wilmore, 1974) and lean body mass (Butts & Price, 1994;
Chilibeck et al., 1998; Chilibeck, Calder, Sale, & Webber, 1996;
Cullinen & Caldwell, 1998; Wilmore, 1974), and decreasing percent
body fat (Butts & Price, 1994; Cullinen & Caldwell, 1998;
Prabhakaran, Dowling, Branch, Swain, & Leutholtz, 1999; Wilmore,
1974) in both young and middle-aged women. In older women, strength
training helps prevent sarcopenia by increasing the strength, mass,
power, and quality of skeletal muscle, and it may also help prevent
age-related losses in bone mineral density (Hurley & Roth, 2000).
Among women of all ages, strength training also enhances the ability to
carry out activities of daily living (American College of Sports
Medicine, 2000).
In addition to the physiological benefits of strength training,
there are numerous psychological benefits for women who strength train.
Among young women, strength training has been found to result in
improvements in self-esteem (Brazell-Roberts & Thomas, 1989-1990;
Melnick & Mookerjee, 1991; Trujillo, 1983), self-concept (Brown
& Harrison, 1986), emotional well-being (Tucker & Maxwell,
1992), body image (Tucker & Maxwell, 1992), body-cathexis (Melnick
& Mookerjee, 1991), and state anxiety (O'Connor, Bryant,
Veltri, & Gebhardt, 1993). Further, participation in a strength
training program results in improved self-concept in middle-aged women
(Brown & Harrison, 1986) and improved psychological health in older
women (Tsutsumi et al., 1998).
Ideally, considering the number of physiological and psychological
benefits associated with strength training, more women should
participate in a strength training program. In recent years, strength
training has reportedly been increasing in popularity among women (Ebben
& Jensen, 1998; Fleck, 1998; Freedson, 2000; Marble, 1997); yet, the
actual number of women who strength train is still relatively low.
According to the Fitness Products Council, the number of women using
free weights increased from 7.4 million in 1987 to 15 million in 1995
(Marble, 1997). However, the U.S. Census Bureau (2000) estimates
reported over 100 million women 18 years of age and older in the United
States in 1995, meaning that only 15% of the female population over the
age of 18 were strength training. Additionally, the gender gap in sport
and fitness has continued to grow with women reporting significantly
less physical activity than men (Robinson & Godbey, 1993).
Surprisingly, the youngest group in the sample, college-aged women, had
the largest declines in physical activity. Thus, it appears that women,
particularly younger women, are not engaging in physical activity or
strength training which have been shown to be beneficial across the life
span. To increase women's participation in strength training, it
would appear important to understand differences between those women who
do strength train and those who do not.
Numerous theories of behavior change suggest that perceived
benefits and barriers related to the behavior play an important role in
whether people engage in that behavior or not. Decision-Making theory
examines the perceived benefits versus the perceived costs (Janis &
Mann, 1977), social cognitive theories examine benefits and outcomes
(Bandura, 1986), while the transtheoretical model (TTM) (Prochaska,
DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992) uses a decision balance concept to
estimate benefits and barriers to progression to the next stage of
behavior change. Marcus and colleagues (Marcus, Rakowski, & Rossi,
1992; Marcus & Owen, 1992) have suggested that individuals in the
early stages of exercise adoption perceive more barriers and fewer
benefits to exercise while those in the latter stages perceive more
benefits and fewer barriers to exercise. Myers and Roth (1997) examined
exercise participation among college students through questionnaires
that assessed their current exercise participation, their intention to
become more active in the next month, the benefits they receive or
expect to receive from exercising, and the barriers that interfere with
exercise. Through these questionnaires a Benefits and Barriers to
Exercise (BBE) (Myers and Roth, 1997) questionnaire was developed. The
results of the investigation illustrated that perceived benefits and
barriers to participation are separate constructs consisting of four
benefit factors and four barrier factors. Those participants in the
early stages of exercise adoption reported fewer benefits and more
barriers than those in the latter stages. Additionally, a gender effect
for certain benefits and barriers was observed. The women in the
investigation reported significantly more psychological and body image
benefits, but fewer social benefits, than men. Women also perceived more
time-effort barriers to exercise than men. However, the majority of this
work has been conducted in aerobic activities and may not apply to young
women and strength training. Thus, examining the differences in benefits
and barriers to strength training in college-age women who regularly
participate in strength training versus those who do not strength
training may further our understanding of why certain women participate
in strength training.
Therefore, the purpose of this investigation was to examine the
perceived benefits and barriers to strength training among college-age
women. Specifically, women who regularly strength train and women who do
not strength train completed questionnaires concerning their perceived
benefits and barriers to strength training participation. It was
predicted that women who currently participate in a strength training
program would report more benefits of and fewer barriers to strength
training than their non-strength training counterparts. Conversely, it
was predicted that non-strength trainers would report more barriers to
and less benefits of strength training than their strength training
counterparts. It was predicted that the benefit factor with the most
influence would be body image, with health having a slightly smaller
influence, while time-effort and body image would be the most
influential barrier factors. It is hoped that an understanding of these
perceived benefits and barriers will aid in the effective design and
marketing of strength training programs that will ultimately lead to
increased participation by women.
Methods
Participants
Female participant volunteers were recruited from sororities,
dormitories, and Kinesiology classes at a large University in the
eastern United States. All participants completed an exercise
participation questionnaire and the BBE questionnaire. Based on the
exercise participation questionnaire, participants were placed into one
of two groups, strength trainers (ST) or non-strength trainers (NST). In
order to maximize the differences between groups, over 300 participants
initially completed questionnaires. The exercise participation
questionnaires were screened in a random manner until 50 participants
were placed in strength and non-strength training groups. Those
participants who met or exceeded, in terms of quantity, American College
of Sports Medicine strength training guidelines for healthy adults
(American College of Sports Medicine, 1998) were selected for the
strength training group (N=50). Participants in this group had a mean
age of 19.88 [+ or -] 1.22 years and strength trained an average of 2.80
[+ or -] 0.90 days per week. In addition to meeting quantity
recommendations, participants had to strength train at an intensity of
either moderate or total fatigue to be included in the strength training
group. Strength trainers also trained aerobically an average of 3.96 [+
or -] 1.80 days per week for 65.30 [+ or -] 48.74 minutes. Those
participants who reported no strength training were placed in the
non-strength training group (N=50). Participants in this group had a
mean age of 20.32 [+ or -] 1.39 years and did not strength train.
Non-strength trainers did participate in an average of 1.86 [+ or -]
1.77 days per week of aerobic training for 33.40 [+ or -] 38.10 minutes.
Those participants who reported participation in strength training, but
did not sufficiently meet ACSM guidelines, were eliminated from further
analysis.
Measures
Exercise Participation Questionnaire. Strength and aerobic training
participation were assessed with an exercise participation
questionnaire. Respondents were asked to indicate if they participated
in strength training (i.e. using free weights, machines, or both, to
increase muscular strength and conditioning). If so, they were asked to
state modality, frequency, number of exercises per day, sets per
exercise, repetitions per set and on a scale of one to four, with one
representing no fatigue, and four representing total fatigue, intensity.
Respondents were also asked if they participated in any aerobic training
(i.e. continuous, rhythmic activity that involves the use of large
muscle groups for an extended period of time). If so, they were asked to
state frequency and duration.
Benefits and Barriers to Exercise Questionnaire. Perceived benefits
of strength training and perceived barriers to strength training were
assessed using a benefits of and barriers to strength training
questionnaire. The Benefits and Barriers to Exercise (BBE) Questionnaire
was developed by Myers and Roth (1997). The BBE contains 48 (24 benefit
and 24 barrier) items presented via a five point Likert scale ranging
from (1) not important to (5) extremely important. The items were based
on surveys of both exercisers and non-exercisers who were asked to list
three benefits they get or would expect to get from regular exercise and
three reasons why they do not exercise at all or why they do not
exercise as much as they would like. A confirmatory factor analysis performed by Myers and Roth (1997) of benefits and barriers revealed an
eight-factor model with four benefit and four barrier factors providing
the best fit to the data. The reliability of the total benefit score for
the BBE was .88 and the reliability of the total barrier score was .68.
Benefit items were clustered into psychological (i.e., lift's
one's spirits, improves attitude towards life), social (i.e.,
provides a way to meet people, builds companionship), body image (i.e.,
improves appearance, helps to stay in shape), and health factors (i.e.,
improves health, improves strength). Barrier items were clustered into
time-effort (i.e., too much work, too tired), physical effects (i.e.,
look silly, causes sore muscles), social (i.e., friends don't
encourage, don't like to exercise alone), and specific obstacles
factors (i.e., no convenient place, interferes with school). Test-retest
reliabilities of individual benefit and barrier subscale scores ranged
from .60 to .86.
The benefits of and barriers to strength training questionnaire
used in this study included all 24 benefit and all 24 barrier items
utilized by Myers and Roth (1997), presented via the same Likert scale,
but modified to address strength training issues. Participants were
instructed to read each statement on the questionnaire and then circle
the appropriate number to indicate the importance of that item in
determining why they would or would not participate in strength
training.
Procedure
Participants were given a packet consisting of a cover sheet and
the questionnaires. The cover sheet explained the purpose and procedures
of the study and participants were instructed to read the cover sheet
before proceeding to the questionnaire. Questionnaires were administered
only once and each participant completed all questionnaires during the
same session. To ensure anonymity, participants were not asked to give
their names on any of the questionnaires. Each set of questionnaires was
numbered and coded (ST, NST) to differentiate between participants. The
questionnaires were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the
University.
Statistical Analyses
One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was performed separately for
each of the benefit and barrier factors to determine differences between
strength trainers and non-strength trainers. Where appropriate, post-hoc
analysis was performed with Bonferonni corrected paired samples t-tests.
Results
Perceived Benefits
One-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences (p > 0.05, see
Table 1 for complete statistical results) between strength trainers and
non-strength trainers on any of the perceived benefit factors, which
included psychological, body image, social, and health. As no group
differences were present in the benefits factors, responses for each
factor were combined across the two groups. Further analysis found all
benefit factors to be significantly different from one another (p <
0.008), except for body image and health, as depicted in Figure 1.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Perceived Barriers
One-way ANOVA revealed significant differences between strength
trainers and non-strength trainers on all of the perceived barrier
factors (p < 0.007, see Table 2 for complete statistical results),
which included physical, time-effort, social, and specific. Mean item
responses for each group on each barrier factor are presented in Figure
2. Further analysis found that among strength trainers, only the
physical and time-effort factors were significantly different from one
another (p < 0.008). For the non-strength trainers, time-effort was
significantly greater than all remaining barrier factors (p < 0.008).
Physical, social, and specific barrier factors were not different from
one another. A within groups comparison of the barrier factors is
depicted in Figure 3.
[FIGURES 2-3 OMITTED]
Discussion
The purpose of this investigation was to determine young
women's perceptions of benefits and barriers related to strength
training. The results revealed that women strength trainers and
non-strength trainers do not differ in terms of perceived psychological,
body image, social, and health benefits related to strength training. It
would appear that young women, whether they strength train or not, are
aware of the physiological and psychological benefits associated with
strength training, a result that was inconsistent with the hypothesis.
This result is also in contrast to previous research, which has found
that individuals in the early stages of exercise adoption perceive fewer
benefits than those in the latter stages (Marcus, Rakowski, & Rossi,
1992; Marcus & Owen, 1992). It is possible that this population of
young college-age women has been exposed to more information concerning
the benefits of strength training than participants in previous
investigations. As previous research has suggested that increasing the
awareness of the benefits of exercise is an important early step to
increasing future exercise behavior, the women in this investigation may
be on the path to becoming more active. This trend may be evident in the
amount of aerobic activity that the non-strength trainers participated
in; an average of 2 days per week for 30 min per day. Based on this
result, initiatives designed to encourage strength training
participation among young women do not necessarily need to focus on the
physiological and psychological benefits of strength training as a
strategy to increase participation.
In contrast to the benefits results, women strength trainers and
non-strength trainers do differ in terms of perceived physical,
time-effort, social, and specific barriers related to strength training,
a result that was consistent with the hypothesis. This result is also
consistent with previous research, which has found that individuals in
the latter stages of exercise adoption perceive fewer barriers than
those in the early stages (Marcus, Rakowski, & Rossi, 1992 &
Marcus & Owen, 1992). It is possible that as participants continue
to participate in strength training and start to see and feel the
physiological and psychological benefits associated with the activity,
they begin to overcome their barriers to participation. Therefore, it
would appear necessary to first illustrate the benefits associated with
strength training and then teach techniques that will allow individuals
to overcome their barriers.
Of the benefit factors examined in this study, body image and
health were found to have the highest ratings, as evidenced by the mean
item responses reported for the combined groups (Figure 1). This result
suggests that both strength trainers and non-strength trainers
understand the importance of strength training to enhance their body
image as well as the health benefits of strength training. Furthermore,
as body image and health were the only benefit factors that were not
significantly different from one another, they are equally important in
terms of perceived benefits of strength training. Considering the strong
emphasis on a thin and fit figure for women in our society, it is not
surprising that a perceived benefit of strength training among young
women would be body image. In addition to the emphasis on physical
appearance in our society, there is also an increasing emphasis on the
role exercise can play in maintaining health, which would lend women to
equally understand the health rewards of strength training
participation.
While they do not differ in terms of perceived benefits, strength
trainers and non-strength trainers do differ in terms of perceived
barriers to strength training. As such, young women who do not strength
train perceive more barriers to strength training than those who do
strength train. In fact, strength trainers are virtually free of any of
the barrier factors. Recall that on the questionnaire used in this
study, participants were asked to indicate via a Likert scale the
importance of items in determining why they would or would not
participate in strength training. The Likert scale ranged from (1) not
important to (5) extremely important. Mean item responses for the
strength trainers on each barrier factor were all between 1.5 and 2.
Taking that into consideration, it becomes evident that strength
trainers in this investigation have little to no barriers to their
participation in strength training.
Non-strength trainers, on the other hand, do experience barriers to
strength training, specifically time-effort. The time-effort factor was
found to be significantly different from all other barrier factors, and
as can be seen in Figure 3, it is the highest rated barrier for
non-strength trainers. This finding is consistent with that of Myers and
Roth (1997), who found that pre-contemplators, or individuals who are
inactive and do not intend to increase their level of physical activity,
perceive more time-effort barriers to exercise than do individuals in
any other stage of exercise adoption. In addition, contemplators, or
individuals who are inactive but intend to become more active, perceive
more time-effort barriers to exercise participation than do individuals
who are in the training stage of exercise. Based on this pattern of
response for the barriers questionnaire, it would appear that
time-effort considerations are paramount in negotiating between academic
requirements and strength training participation for non-strength
trainers in a college environment. As such, initiatives to increase
strength training participation among young women should focus on
strategies for overcoming barriers to strength training, specifically
time-effort.
In conclusion, young women strength trainers and non-strength
trainers are both aware of the benefits of participating in strength
training. However, differences between the two groups emerge when
barriers to strength training are considered. Specifically, non-strength
trainers report higher levels of physical, time-effort, social, and
specific barriers to strength training. Of these, time-effort is the
highest rated barrier factor. As such, initiatives to encourage young
women to participate in and maintain a strength training program should
focus on overcoming time-effort barriers through the use of goal setting
and time management. It is hoped that continued investigation into the
reasons why women do or do not participate in strength training will
lead to improved strategies for encouraging women to initiate and
maintain a strength training program.
Table 1.
One-way Analysis of Variance for Benefit Factors
Factor df SS F p ES Observed
Power
Social 1 00.160 0.242 .624 .08 .078
Error 98 64.667
Psychological 1 00.045 0.091 .764 .05 .06
Error 98 48.014
Body-image 1 00.538 1.522 .220 .20 .231
Error 98 34.622
Health 1 00.548 1.972 .163 .22 .285
Error 98 27.217
Table 2.
One-way Analysis of Variance for Barrier Factors
df SS F p ES Observed
Factor Power
Time-effort 1 23.281 35.674 .000 1.13 1.00
Error 98 63.954
Physical 1 7.654 13.924 .000 .66 .932
Error 98 53.872
Social 1 5.138 7.986 .006 .52 .993
Error 98 63.044
Specific 1 7.290 14.483 .000 .69 .927
Error 98 49.328
References
American College of Sports Medicine. (1998). Position stand: the
recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and
maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness, and flexibility in
healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 30(6),
975-991.
American College of Sports Medicine. (2000). ACSM's guidelines
for exercise testing and prescription (6th ed.). Baltimore: Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins.
Bandura (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Brazell-Roberts, J.V., & Thomas, L.E. (1989-1990). Pumping iron
and self-esteem. Swimming Technique, 26 (3), 21-23.
Brown, R.D., & Harrison, J.M. (1986). The effects of a strength
training program on the strength and self-concept of two female age
groups. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 57 (4), 315-320.
Butts, N.K, & Price, S. (1994). Effects of a 12-week weight
training program on the body composition of women over 30 years of age.
Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 8 (4), 265-269.
Chilibeck, P.D., Calder, A., Sale, D.G., & Webber, C.E. (1996).
Twenty weeks of weight training increases lean tissue mass but not bone
mineral mass or density in healthy, active young women. Canadian Journal
of Physiology and Pharmacology, 74, 1180-1185.
Chilibeck, P.D., Calder, A.W., Sale, D.G., & Webber, C.E.
(1998). A comparison of strength and muscle mass increases during
resistance training in young w omen. European Jour nal of Applied
Physiology, 77, 170-175.
Cullinen, K., & Caldwell, M. (1998). Weight training increases
fat-free mass and strength in untrained young women. Journal of the
American Dietetic Association, 98 (4), 414418.
Ebben, W.P. & Jensen, R.L. (1998). Strength training for women:
Debunking myths that block opportunity. The Physician and
Sportsmedicine, 26 (5), 86-88, 91-92, 97.
Fleck, S.J. (1998). Strong evidence. Athletic Business, 22 (10),
51-52, 54, 56-57.
Freedson, P.S. (2000). Strength training for women. Idea Personal
Trainer, 11 (7), 36-40, 43-44.
Hurley, B.F., & Roth, S.M. (2000). Strength training in the
elderly: Effects on risk factors for age-related diseases. Sports
Medicine, 30 (4), 249-268.
Janis & Mann (1977). Decision making: A psychological analysis
of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press.
Marble, M. (1997, April 28). Women more likely than men to be
fitness enthusiasts [Electronic version]. Women's Health Weekly.
Retrieved October 23, 2000 from the World Wide Web: http
://ehostvgw13.epnet.com/
ehost.asp?key=204.179.122.141_8000_572209800&site=ehost&return=y
Marcus, B.H. & Owen, N. (1992). Motivational readiness,
self-efficacy, and decision making for exercise. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 11,257-261.
Marcus, B.H., Rakowski, W., & Rossi, J.S. (1992). Assessing
motivational readiness and decision-making for exercise. Health
Psychology, 11,257-261.
Melnick, M.J., & Mookerjee, S. (1991). Effects of advanced
weight training on body-cathexis and self-esteem. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 72, 1335-1345.
Myers, R.S., & Roth, D.L. (1997). Perceived benefits of and
barriers to exercise and stage of exercise adoption in young adults.
Health Psychology, 16 (3), 277-283.
O'Connor, P.J. Bryant, C.X., Veltri, J.P., & Gebhardt,
S.M. (1993). State anxiety and ambulatory blood pressure following
resistance exercise in females. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 25 (4), 516-521.
Prabhakaran, B., Dowling, E.A., Branch, J.D., Swain, D.P., &
Leutholtz, B.C. (1999). Effect of 14 weeks of resistance training on
lipid profile and body fat percentage in premeno pausal women. British
Journal of Sports Medicine, 33, 190-195.
Prochaska, J.O., DiClemente, C.C., & Norcross, J.C. (1992). In
search of how people change: Applications to addictive behaviors.
American Psychologist, 47, 1102-1114.
Robinson, J.P. & Godbey, G. (1993). Sport, fitness, and the
gender gap. Leisure Sciences, 14(4), 291-307.
Sidney, K., & Jette, M. (1992). Characteristics of women
performing strength training: compari son of participants and dropouts.
Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 32, 84-95.
Trujillo, C.M. (1983). The effect of weight training and running
exercise intervention programs on the self-esteem of college women.
International Journal of Sport Psychology, 14, 162-173.
Tsutsumi, T., Don, B.M., Zaichkowsky, L.D., Takenaka, K., Oka, K.,
& Ohno, T. (1998). Compari son of high and moderate intensity of
strength training on mood and anxiety in older adults. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 87, 1003-1011.
Tucker, L.A., & Maxwell, K. (1992). Effects of weight training
on the emotional well-being and body image of females: Predictors of
greatest benefit. American Journal of Health Promotion, 6 (5), 338-344.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2000, December 20). Resident population
estimates of the United States by age and sex: April 1 1990 to July 1,
1999, with short-term projection to November 1, 2000 [Online service
text file]. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved
April 2, 2001 from the World Wide Web: http://www.census.gov/
population/estimates/nation/intfile2-1 .txt
Wilmore, J.H. (1974). Alterations in strength, body composition and
anthropometric measure ments consequent to a 10-week weight training
program. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 6 (2), 133-138.
Author Note
A. J. Harne is with the Dept. of Kinesiology, University of
Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. W. R. Bixby, formerly at Maryland, is
now with the Department of Health, Exercise, and Sport Sciences, Texas
Tech University, Lubbock TX 79409. This manuscript represents work
completed by A. J. Harne as an Undergraduate Honors Thesis at the
University of Maryland under the direction of W. R. Bixby.
Amanda J. Harne and Walter R. Bixby
University of Maryland
Address Correspondence To: Walter R. Bixby, Ph.D., Department of
Health, Exercise, and Sport Sciences Exercise Science Center Box 43011
Texas Tech University Lubbock, TX 79409. Phone: 806 742 3371 E-mail:
[email protected] FAX: 806 742 1688