Effects of aerobic and circuit training on fitness and body image among women.
Henry, Ruth N. ; Anshel, Mark H. ; Michael, Timothy 等
Research on body image has been abundant in recent years, partly
due to its role in the development of eating disorders, such as anorexia
nervosa and bulimia nervosa (Cohen & Petrie, 2005). Body image
refers to the accuracy of the perception of the person's bodily
size and to the thoughts and feelings associated with the
individual's view of the body (Cash, 1989). Traditionally, body
image has been viewed as a multidimensional construct comprised of two
independent components, perception (i.e., size estimation) and attitudes
(i.e., body-related affects and cognitions; Cash, 1989, 1994; Rowe,
1999; Shontz, 1969). While the earlier research addressed the perceptual
component of body image (e.g., Thompson, 1995), more recent studies have
focused on assessment and treatment of body self-image disturbances
(Brown, Cash, & Mikulka, 1990; Grant & Cash, 1995; Koff&
Bauman, 1997; Tiggeman & Williamson, 2000).
The collective term referring to body dissatisfaction,
discrepancies between actual and perceived body size, and negative
affect when comparing one's body to perceived societal norms is
called "body image disturbance." The greatest source of body
image disturbance is sociocultural (Heinberg, 1995). In many cultures,
including the U.S., females feel pressured to achieve a near-impossible
degree of thinness and to avoid weight gain and obesity (Striegel-Moore,
McAvay, & Rodin, 1986). Messages from the mass media are partly
responsible (Van den Buick, 2000).
Van den Buick (2000) reported that among adolescents, the degree of
watching television correlated negatively with ideal body image;
frequent viewers preferred a thinner ideal body than less frequent
television viewers. In another study, Harrison and Cantor (1997) found
that the amount of time viewing television significantly predicted
overall body dissatisfaction. The print media is not exempt from its
influence on body image. Harrison and Cantor also reported a significant
relationship between reading of fashion magazines and body
dissatisfaction. Thus, the concomitant self-inflicted need to achieve
the perceived ideal body type has led to increased body dissatisfaction,
especially among adolescents and young adults. Apparently, this issue is
not new. For example, Rodin, Silberstein, and Striegel-Moore (1985)
predicted that dissatisfaction with one's body may one day become
the societal norm, a phenomenon they called normative discontent.
Perhaps the most potentially harmful by-product of negative body
image is increased risk of eating disorders (Thompson, 1995). The media
has been identified as the culprit in promoting unrealistic ideals. For
example, Thomsen, Weber, and Brown (2001) found a strong correlation
between frequency of reading women's health and fitness magazines
and the use of unhealthy weight practices among high school girls.
Because the female adolescent's self-image is usually
interpersonally oriented, post pubertal changes in body shape and weight
are particularly stressful (Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, & Rodin,
1986). Body shape and weight become critical determinants of self-esteem
in adolescence because interpersonal success is increasingly seen as
closely linked to physical attractiveness (Brownell, Rodin, &
Wilmore, 1992). According to Johnson, Steinberg, and Lewis (1988),
thinness has increasingly been associated with a highly valued personal
achievement, demonstrating self-control, autonomy, and success.
Conversely, obesity, or the absence of weight control can lead to social
discrimination and low self-esteem. Thus, the pursuit of thinness is
commonly perceived an action or goal in which young women can obtain
favorable social responses thereby enhancing self-esteem.
Distorted body image among females, then, is a very common cause of
eating disorders. For example, Overdorf (1991) reported that "a
little over half (of 102 high school female U.S. athletes) saw
themselves as heavy, while in reality only 3% might be classified on the
heavy side" (p. 76). Wardle and Foley (1989) found that young women
gave negative appraisals of their bodies, tended to be dissatisfied with
their appearance, overestimated their body size, and in general,
"felt fat." In addition, Maude, Wertheim, Gibbons, and
Szmukler (1993) concluded that female high school students displayed
higher body image disturbance and dissatisfaction than male students.
According to Bruch (1973), disturbances in body image may predispose an
individual to relentless dieting or to lack of recognition of the
effects of dieting. Typical causes are low self-esteem, perfectionism,
and body dissatisfaction (Reel & Gill, 1996).
Although traditional treatment of body image problems has consisted
primarily of cognitive-behavioral therapy (Cash, 1995), more recently,
exercise has become more popular as a treatment modality (Finkenherg,
DiNucci, McCune, & McCune, 1993; Holmes, Chamberlin, & Young,
1994). Researchers have previously documented the positive changes that
occur in physical fitness following either aerobic or strength training.
In a study of young females, Kraimer, et al., (2001) found that
participants participating in bench step aerobics or a combination of
bench step aerobics and resistance training improved in V[O.sup.2] peak,
resting heart rate, and percent fat. The participants who incorporated
resistance training into their regime also improved in muscular strength
and endurance. Several studies have examined the effects of exercise
training interventions on body image. For example, Koffand Bauman (1997)
found that females who participated in step aerobics, weight-training,
or running twice per week for six weeks significantly improved
appearance evaluation, fitness evaluation, fitness orientation, and body
satisfaction as opposed to the control (non-exercise) group. The
researchers did not explore the combination of aerobic and
strength/circuit training exercise. In a study of women ages 20 to 30
yrs, Skrinar et al. (1986) assessed the effects of intensive endurance
training on body consciousness. Participants engaged in an intensive
running program for six to eight weeks in addition to other types of
moderate sport-related exercise an average of 3.5 hours per day. At the
end of the training program, body competence (i.e., effective body
functioning) and private body consciousness (i.e., focus on internal
bodily sensations) significantly increased, while public body
consciousness (i.e., tendency to be concerned about external appearance
of the body) did not markedly change. Again, a strength/circuit training
condition was not included in this study, however.
Selected studies have focused on the effects of strength training
on body image. For example, Tucker and Maxwell (1992) investigated the
effects of weight-training on body image of females participating in a
15-week, 2-days-per-week weight-training program, compared with a
non-exercising control group. With pretest scores controlled, the
weight-training group had significantly higher posttest scores than
controls in both general well-being and body cathexis, defined as the
degree of satisfaction a person feels about various parts and processes
of the body (Secord & Jurard, 1953). The authors concluded that
strength training enhances both body image and overall feelings of
well-being. Characteristics of women who experienced the greatest
improvements included those who were heavier, shorter, and who were not
involved in any other regular physical activity during the program. The
researchers surmise that heavier, previously inactive participants were
less fit, and therefore had more potential for improvement. In a rare
study comparing weight-training with aerobic exercise, Tucker and
Mortell (1994) found that middle-aged women who engaged in a home
strength training program three times per week for 12 weeks markedly
improved body cathexis more than women participating in a walking
program of the same frequency and duration. In a more recent study,
Ahmed, Hilton, and Pituch (2002) investigated the effects of
weight-training on body image in female university students. After 12
weeks of strength training, participants experienced significantly more
strength and improved body image despite a slight increase in percent
body fat. They also reported improved health and physical fitness.
Williams and Cash (2001) investigated the effects of a six-week
circuit weight-training program on college students. Their results
showed that even a relatively brief program reduced social physique anxiety and improved appearance evaluation and body dissatisfaction
among both males and females. Despite the exclusive use of strength
training in this study, the circuit training method they employed may
have had aerobic benefits.
Research involving body image changes during an intervention
program of both strength and aerobic training programs is rare. Perry et
al. (2002) found that high school students participating in a six-month
course involving 40-45 minutes per week of vigorous aerobic exercise and
20-30 minutes per week of resistance training did improve in body
satisfaction (p<.0009). The researchers did not report the training
schedule, that is, the number of days per week, and whether the aerobic
and strength training occurred on the same days of the week as opposed
to alternative days. The measure of body satisfaction was a change in
"real" versus "ideal" figure silhouettes. In
addition, the 6-month intervention in this study was lengthy in
comparison to more common 8-12 week intervention programs.
Contrary to the results of previous studies, Ford, Puckett,
Blessing, and Tucker (1989) found that exercise did not improve body
cathexis in college females participating in an 8-week, 3-hours-per-week
course in either aerobic dance, jogging for fitness, swimming for
fitness, life saving, or weight-training. Although the fitness groups
improved in various fitness parameters (i.e., sit-ups and flexiblity
tests), none of the activity groups differed from the controls in
self-esteem, body cathexis, step test scores, or percent body fat at the
time of posttest. Similarly, Anderson, Foster, McGuigan, Seebach, and
Porcari (2004) found that self-rated appearance scores in adult men were
not altered after 6 weeks of either strength or aerobic exercise.
Although researchers (e.g., Ahmed, Hilton, & Pituch, 2002;
Heinberg, 1995; Koff & Bauman, 1997) have found that individuals who
exercise have a more positive body image than non-exercisers, others
(e.g., Tiggemann & Williamson, 2000) have found that exercisers have
worse body image than non-exercisers. In particular, the researchers
found that participants who exercised for weight control or improved
"tone" had lower body satisfaction, and that exercising for
the purposes of health and fitness was related to enhanced body
satisfaction. Younger women exercised more for weight control and mood
enhancement than did older men and women, who exercised more for health
and fitness. These results indicate that exercise motivation may be a
mediating factor between exercise and body image.
Strelan, Mehaffey, and Tiggemarm (2003) explored motivates for
exercise and its impact on body image among young women. They found that
exercise motive was a mediating factor in body image. Individuals who
exercised for weight control or to improve body tone and attractiveness
did not improve over time in body image or self-esteem. However,
exercising for health, fitness, mood, and enjoyment resulted in a
positive effect on their body satisfaction and self-esteem. Exercising
for appearance was negatively related to body satisfaction (r = -.57),
whereas exercising for health fitness reasons or enjoyment/mood was
correlated positively with body satisfaction (r = .68 and r = .47,
respectively). Exercise for appearance was particularly prevalent in
young women, but less so among older women and men.
A descriptive study by Davis et al. (1994) supported the
authors' hypothesis that a large percentage of eating-disordered
patients had either engaged in excessive exercise (78%) or had been
competitive athletes prior to the onset of their disorder (60%). This
could provide evidence for the idea that exercise may promote
perceptions of an imperfect body for either fitness (physical
effectiveness) or appearance (physical attractiveness). Although several
researchers have addressed the connection between exercise and body
image, most of these studies were descriptive in nature and did not
include an exercise intervention. And, apparently the study of combined
interval circuit training in changes in body image apparently has not
been explored, particularly among females. It is important to determine
the exercise regimen that will provide optimal benefits in improving
body image and other desirable psychological characteristics.
Thus, the purpose of the current study was to determine the effect
of aerobic and strength training on body image among unfit college
women. It was hypothesized that aerobic exercise would significantly
improve body image, however, a program of interval circuit training
would produce even greater improvements in body image than aerobic
training alone.
Methods
Participants
The participants (N = 72) were female college students, ranging in
ages from 18 to 26 yrs (M = 21.4 yrs., SD = 2.21) who volunteered for
the study. Biometric information is included in Table 1. Excluded from
the study were students who were currently engaging in vigorous exercise at least twice per week or individuals with self-reported eating
disorders.
Materials and Equipment
Body Image Assessment. Before the training period all participants
completed the Body Self-Image Questionnaire (BSIQ, Rowe, 1999) to assess
nine separate body image components. The BSIQ is a 51-item Likert-type
scale with items pertaining to feelings and attitudes about one's
body. Responses ranged from 1 (not at all true of myself) to 5
(completely true of myself). A sample item from each subscale and number
of items in each subscale is provided in Table 2.
The BSIQ consists of three evaluative subscales: (1) overall
appearance evaluation--overall estimation of one's appearance; (2)
fatness evaluation-estimation of overall fatness of one's body; (3)
health/fitness evaluation--estimation of one's overall physical
fitness/health status. The instrument also includes the following
subscales which do not involve evaluation: (4) health/fitness
influence--the degree to which one's health and fitness influences
the way one feels about his or her body; (5) attention to grooming--the
amount of time and effort spent in manipulating one's appearance;
(6) social dependence--effect of social situations or social acceptance
on one's body image; (7) height dissatisfaction--desire to be
taller or shorter; (8) negative affect--the negative thoughts and
feelings associated with one's body; and (9) investment in
ideals--importance of and aspirations to achieve a perfect body. The
questionnaire also includes questions concerning amount (days per week)
of current participation in vigorous exercise and involvement in sports,
as well as demographic questions.
The BSIQ was selected for this study because it has received
rigorous psychometric scrutiny (i.e., exploratory and confirmatory
factor analysis), and validated on several samples of normal and
divergent groups, including individuals with eating disorders and
competitive dancers. Internal reliabilities (Cronbach's alpha) of
the nine subscales have ranged from a = .78 (social dependence) to .94
(fatness evaluation) in a study by Rowe (1999). In the present study,
alphas ranged from .68 to .92 (Md = .88). These reliabilities compare
favorably with other body image instruments (Cash, 2000; Cash &
Szymanski, 1995; Franzoi & Shields, 1984). Accurate identification
of body self-image among the research participants in these studies
supports construct validity of this instrument.
Physical Fitness Testing Protocols
Percent body fat. Body density was calculated from the sum of
triceps, suprailium, and thigh skinfolds using the generalized equation
generated by Jackson, Pollock, and Ward (1980). Body density was
converted to percent body fat using the Siri equation (Siri, 1956).
Step test. The Queens College Step Test (Safrit & Wood, 1995)
was used to estimate cardiorespiratory endurance. This test was chosen
because it has demonstrated acceptable validity and reliability, with
coefficients of .75 and .92, respectively, for college women and an SEE
of 2.9 ml/[kg.sup.-1]/[min.sup.-1] (McArdle, Katch, Pechar, Jacobson,
and Ruck, 1972). The test protocol for women consists of 3 minutes of
stepping on a 16 1/4" bench at a cadence of 22 step cycles per
minute after 15 seconds of practicing the tempo with the metronome.
Recovery heart rate is recorded via heart rate monitor 15 seconds after
the test concludes. V[O.sub.2] max is predicted from the pulse count
using the following regression equation (McArdle, Katch, & Katch,
1991): Predicted V[O.sub.2] max = 65.81 - (0.1847 x post-exercise heart
rate).
Bench press test. The YMCA Bench Press Test (Golding, Myers, &
Sinning, 1982) was the tool used to assess muscular strength and
endurance. The protocol requires a female subject to lift a 35-1b.
barbell from the chest position to full arm extension at a cadence of 60
lift cycles per minute, with cadence set by a metronome. The test is
completed when the participant can no longer lift the bar, or can no
longer lift at the specified tempo. This test was chosen because it is a
submaximal test; it was believed to be safer for participants since no
maximal contractions are required. Validity and reliability data have
not been published for the YMCA test; however, submaximal bench press
tests in general have been found to have face validity as an endurance
measure, and concurrent validity as a strength measure. Submaximal bench
press tests have a high correlation with the maximum weight lifted in
one repetition (r >.90). Reliability can be expected to be high if
examinees are motivated to achieve maximal performance (Safrit &
Wood, 1995).
Procedures
All procedures for both testing and training sessions were approved
by the university Institutional Review Board. For the pretest,
participants completed the BSIQ first. The BSIQ was administered in a
group setting, with each of the three groups receiving identical
standardized instructions. Within these instructions, participants were
told that they would be providing information for a research study, and
that they would be asked to answer questions concerning feelings about
their body. Because questionnaires were coded by number anonymity was
assured. Participants were also assured that they were under no
obligation to complete the process if they should decide not to
continue.
During the same group session, participants completed an informed
consent for both testing and training protocols, as well as a PAR-Q
(American College of Sports Medicine, 2000), a physical activity
readiness questionnaire in which the participant identifies past or
current symptoms that occur during exercise, as well as diagnoses of
cardiorespiratory disease. The PAR-Q was employed to identify health
problems that would make testing or training unsafe for the
participants.
After completion of the BSIQ, each subject participated in the
physical fitness evaluation for body composition, cardiorespiratory
endurance, and muscular strength/endurance. Each participant performed
the three tests in the same order, and tests were explained and
administered to each subject by the same tester. Tests were performed
individually, except for the step test, in which three participants were
tested at once. Each participant performed step and bench press tests on
one occasion, whereas skinfolds consisted of the mean of three recorded
readings. Room temperature of the testing laboratory was maintained at
70[degrees]F.
At the conclusion of the 12-week training program, all participants
were retested for body image and physical fitness, using identical
testing procedures as in the pretest. For each participant, order, time
of day, and the examiner were consistent on both the pretest and
posttest.
Interventions
After pre-test procedures were completed, participants were
non-randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups, aerobic
exercise (n = 23) or interval circuit training (n = 28), or to a control
(non-exercise) group (n = 21). Group assignments were based on their
class and work schedules; they did not select nor choose their mode of
exercise. The aerobic and circuit training Classes were scheduled at
2:30 p.m. and 3:30 p.m., respectively. The participants did not know
which class would meet at which time when they selected their class
time, thereby controlling for a selection effect.
Exercise classes met three 50-min. sessions for each of 12 weeks.
As reported in the posttest BSIQ, the participants did not engage in
vigorous exercise beyond these class periods. The class sessions for
both training groups were identical in duration, and the same instructor
taught all classes. Finally, the type of music to which all exercisers
were exposed was upbeat.
Aerobic Exercise
The aerobic exercise group completed 12 weeks of step aerobics,
three times per week. Each session was 50 minutes in duration, with 35
minutes per session devoted entirely to steady state aerobic exercise.
The remainder of the class period consisted of warm-up, cool-down, and
limited (3-4 minutes) muscular endurance exercises for the abdominals.
Participants were urged to adjust exercise intensity to maintain a heart
rate of 60-90% of their age-predicted maximum, and recorded their heart
rates on a chart at least once per class session to insure that they
were exercising at the prescribed intensity. The instructor demonstrated
variations in step exercise to accommodate for individual differences in
fitness levels; therefore, all participants were able to exercise within
the recommended intensity. Although abdominal crunches were included at
the end of the step sessions, no other resistance exercises were
performed during the classes.
Interval Circuit training
Because the degree of compliance in individual workouts in the
weight room is difficult to monitor accurately, this study was designed
to facilitate muscular strength/endurance training in a class setting,
rather than the traditional individual resistance training approach
conducted in a weight room. The interval circuit training group
participated in a class that used a circuit of intervals combining
aerobic training, anaerobic training, and muscular strength/ endurance
training. The circuit involved the use of bench-stepping, jump rope,
cardio-boxing with training gloves, partner exercises with 3-kg medicine
balls, various lifts with 8-lb. dumbbells, body resistance exercises,
and agility drills. Each training session was 50 minutes in duration,
including warm-up and cool-down to pre-exercise levels. Although the
instructor encouraged participants to push themselves to train at a high
level, accommodations for individual fitness levels were made. Although
all participants did not exercise at the same absolute intensity, they
were urged to attain the same relative intensity, that is, 60-90% of
their age-predicted maximum heart rate.
Control Group
The control group was asked to maintain their "usual"
exercise level, which was relatively low, during the 12 weeks.
Participants reported a mean of 3.38 days of low to moderate aerobic
exercise per week for the pretest, and 3.24 days/week for the posttest
resulting in no significant change in their estimated V[O.sub.2] max.
(p> .05). The pretest mean (exercise days/week) for all three groups
was statistically similar (i.e., no significant difference: p =.39). The
pretest V[O.sub.2] max for all groups place them at the 50th percentile
for 20-29-year-old women.
Protocol adherence was maintained in each of the experimental
groups by participants who recorded their heart rate on three occasions
during each class session. Heart rate was also monitored by an assistant
instructor during class to insure that exercise intensity was within the
recommended range to promote cardiovascular fitness (i.e., 65-80% of
age-predicted maximum heart rate). Participants whose heart rate was
below the prescribed rate at any time during the class were verbally
encouraged to increase exercise intensity. Attendance was recorded at
each class session.
Data Analysis
Because assignment to groups was nonrandom, it was necessary to
show that groups were equal in fitness and body image before the
training program began. For each physical fitness and body image
variable, pretest scores were analyzed with one-way analyses of variance
(ANOVA) to detect pre-existing group differences.
Two 3 (groups) x 2 (time) repeated measures MANOVAs were performed
on the three physical fitness variables, and the nine body image
variables to detect whether the three groups differed in rates of change
in fitness and body image during the 12 weeks. The main effects for
group were not of interest; only the main effect for time and the
interaction of group and time were analyzed. The Wilkes Lambda
statistic, transformed to F, was used to test for significance.
Assuming significant MANOVA results, separate univariate 3 x 2
ANOVAs for each physical fitness and body image variable were performed
to determine which variables demonstrated a significant main effect for
time or group x time interaction. The familywise a level of .05 was held
constant for each univariate ANOVA by using a modified Bonferroni
adjustment recommended by Keppel (1991) in cases where the number of
comparisons exceeds [df.sub.among]. The adjustment to a for each
comparison is calculated as: a per comparison = ([df.sub.among] *
familywise [alpha]) / number of comparisons. A modified Tukey post hoe test was performed for each variable with a significant group x time
interaction to determine which groups differed from each other at the
time of posttest, after adjusting for pretest differences. The post hoc method used for this analysis (Hinkle, Wiersma, & Jurs, 1998)
adjusts the Tukey HSD, allowing for use of the pre-training scores as
covariates, thereby allowing pairwise comparisons of adjusted means (1).
For those variables with a significant main effect for time, with no
significant group x time interaction, a post hoe test was performed to
determine which groups improved significantly over time.
Results
The rate of compliance in both experimental groups was high, with
90.2% compliance in the aerobics/strength circuit group and 91.2%
compliance in the aerobics group. Compliance was defined as the number
of classes attended dived by the total number of classes (n = 35) x 100.
Control participants reported on the BSIQ that they did not increase in
exercise frequency during the 12 weeks.
Physical Fitness Variables
Pretest ANOVA for each physical fitness variable confirmed that no
significant differences existed among the three groups in any of three
variables (p>.05) before the training program began. Table 3 depicts
the pretest and posttest means and standard deviations for each
variable. The 3 x 2 repeated measures MANOVA for physical fitness
variables yielded a significant main effect for time (F(3,64) = 39.33,
p<.001) and for time x group interaction (F(6,128) = 12.99,
p<.001). As a follow-up to the significant interaction, univariate
ANOVA was performed on each of the three fitness variables, with the
significance level adjusted to ct = .033 to control for type I error.
Univariate ANOVAs on the three fitness variables also yielded
significant time x group interactions (Table 4) for all variables,
indicating that the there was a significant difference among the three
groups in amount of change during the training program. Tukey post hoc
analyses indicated that the aerobics/strength training group had the
greatest improvement in percent body fat (aerobics/strength <
aerobics < control), with significant posttest differences between
the interval circuit training group and control group (Q = 9.33,
p<.001), between the interval circuit training group and the aerobics
group (Q = 4.29, p = .013), and between the aerobics group and the
control group (Q = 4.92, p = .004).
For the VO2 max results, the interval circuit training group was
significantly higher than the control group at posttest (Q = 4.98; p =
.002). No other pairwise comparisons were significant for VO2 max. In
strength, the interval circuit group performed a significantly greater
number of bench press repetitions at posttest than both the control
group (Q = 7.00; p<.001) and the aerobics group (Q = 4.12, p = .016).
No other pairwise comparisons were significant for the bench press. In
the post hoc analyses, adjusted means were used as posttest means,
taking into account the effect of pretest scores. (1)
Body Image Variables
ANOVA conducted on pretest scores revealed that no significant
differences (p>.05) existed among the three groups in any of the nine
body image variables. Table 3 presents the pretest and posttest scores
for each body image subscale. A 3 (group) x 2 (time) MANOVA with
repeated measures on the last factor yielded a significant main effect
for time (F (9,61) = 3.54, p = .001) and for time x group interaction
(F(18,122) = 2.02; p = .013). The group x time interaction indicates
that the three groups differed on changes in body image during the
training program. Univariate analyses of variance were then performed to
detect which physical fitness variables were responsible for the
significant interaction in the overall MANOVA. A modified Bonferroni
adjustment held the familywise type I error rate constant (.05) by
adjusting the per comparison criterion to ct = .011. In the follow-up
univariate analyses (Table 5), the interaction for group x time was
significant for the body image variables of health/fitness evaluation
(F(2,69) = 5.08, p = .009), and negative affect (F(1,69) = 6.67; p =
.002). Tukey pairwise comparisons were computed to detect the location
of these differences. The aerobics/strength training group had lower
posttest scores than the control group for negative affect (Q = 4.11, p
= .013), and better posttest scores than the aerobics group for
health/fitness evaluation (Q = 3.91, p = .019).
For overall appearance evaluation and health/fitness influence,
group x time interaction was not significant (p> .05), however, the
main effect for time was significant (F(1,69) = 6.78, p = .011; F(1,69)
= 10.52, p = .002, respectively). Post hoc analyses of these variables
indicated that the interval circuit group improved significantly in
overall appearance evaluation and health/fitness influence, while the
aerobics group improved in health/fitness influence.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of aerobic
training and interval circuit training on changes in body image among
unfit college women. It was hypothesized that the group experiencing the
combined aerobic and strength conditioning program would result in
significantly superior body image and selected fitness parameters as
opposed to the aerobic only and control groups. The results supported
our predictions.
With respect to fitness variables, the interval circuit group had a
greater VO2 max at the end of the training program than either of the
other two groups. The success of the circuit training program in the
present study was likely due to the interval training utilized for the
combination treatment group. Elite endurance athletes have trained using
intervals for years, and often attribute their success to interval
training (McArdle, Katch, & Katch, 1991). Because intense exercise
intervals are interspersed with rest intervals or intervals of less
intense exercise, a larger amount of total work can be accomplished than
with steady-state aerobic exercise that is performed continually.
Although the interval circuit training group demonstrated the
greatest improvement in strength, the aerobics group also showed a
modest increase in strength. The strength increase in the aerobics group
was likely due to the continuous exaggerated upper body movements
utilized in the aerobic step classes, because other upper-body exercises
during the aerobics classes were minimal. The participants did not hold
hand weights during the step training, and no pushups or other
resistance exercises were included in the class sessions.
The interval circuit training group had a lower percent body fat at
posttest than either of the other two groups, and the aerobics group had
a lower percent fat than the control group. While these results indicate
that both training programs were effective in lowing percent body fat,
the combination program was the most effective. This result is not
surprising, however, because the increased muscle mass that doubtless
accompanied the increase in strength would have altered body composition
in the direction of more muscle mass and less fat.
The results of this study were in agreement with other previous
findings. For example, Tucker and Maxwell (1992) found the body cathexis
scores of females participating in strength training to be greater than
those of a control group. Ahmed, Hilton, and Pituch (2002) also found
strength training to be beneficial in improving body image in young
females. Results of the current study lend support to these findings in
that the aerobic plus strength group had a significantly better positive
body image scores in several of the body image subscales than controls.
Tucker and Mortell (1994) also found that weight training improved body
image more effectively than walking.
Although the aerobic exercise in the current study was more
vigorous than walking, the interval circuit training was still more
effective than the aerobics-only program for improving body image. It
should be noted that the inclusion of strength training for this group
actually decreased the number of minutes dedicated to aerobic training,
with positive results for both physical fitness and body image.
The findings of the present study are also important because the
strength training program was introduced into the training regimen
without adding extra time to the overall exercise program. Time devoted
to strength training replaced time for aerobic training, thereby using
an interval training format. Since most exercisers are eager to get the
best fitness program possible for limited time investment, the evidence
that a combination program can improve both fitness and body image is
noteworthy.
It would be reasonable to suggest that the body image of the
participants in the current study may have improved because they felt
that they were taking positive steps to reshape their bodies. However,
changes in body image were accompanied by measurable improvements in
fitness. Whether exercise produces increased self-efficacy, thereby
improving all aspects of self-esteem, including body image is open to
speculation. While the results of this study do not prove that an
increase in physical fitness causes body image to improve, the present
results do reveal that the improvements attained after the exercise
program were both psychological and physical.
Exercise improves female body self-image; however, a woman's
ideal body image continues to shift toward a thinner standard (Davis,
1997). Thus, even improved body image due to training may be transient.
The woman is briefly more satisfied with her body until she "raises
the standard" and may become less satisfied with her body again.
Further research is needed that follows female body image over a period
of years.
Davis and Fox (1993) explored the association between excessive
exercise and weight preoccupation in women, theorizing that those who
exercise excessively share characteristics with those who are
preoccupied with their weight. The results did not support this theory.
However, they found that excessive exercisers reported greater body
satisfaction and were less emotionally reactive (i.e., neurotic) than
non-exercisers, while weight preoccupation was associated with less body
satisfaction and more neuroticism. Our results indicated that women who
engage in an extensive, but "normal" exercise regimens do not
experience low body image.
There were selected limitations in this study. For example,
participants were not assigned to exercise groups on a strictly random
basis. It was imperative that the student's class schedule and the
exercise program schedule were compatible; therefore, participants were
assigned to groups based on their class schedules. This non-random
assignment strategy may have compromised internal validity.
Nevertheless, pretest ANOVAs confirmed similar levels of physical
fitness and body image prior to the intervention. A final limitation was
the control was not fully sedentary. They engaged in either a
non-aerobic or low intense exercise program. However, significant group
differences found in our results confirmed that exercise of greater
intensity resulting in improved cardiovascular fitness results in
desired changes in body image.
As indicated earlier, although several studies have compared
changes in body image after different types of aerobic training programs
and/or strength training programs, the effect of the combination of
aerobic and strength training on body image has been virtually ignored
by researchers, at least among college women. Researchers (e.g.,
Skrinar, et al., 1986; Tucker & Maxwell, 1992; Koff & Bauman,
1997) have documented that changes in physical fitness are related to
improvements in body image.
In summary, then, participants who trained for 12 weeks using a
combination of aerobic, anaerobic, and strength circuit training
improved in several components of body image and on selected features of
physical fitness. Greater improvements were made by the combination
group than by those who trained only aerobically. The control group did
not improve during the 12 weeks in body image or in physical fitness.
Taken together, it appears that exercise has a positive effect on the
body image of college-aged women. Although aerobic exercise has been
found to be beneficial in improving some components of body image,
apparently a circuit training program of aerobic, anaerobic, and
strength intervals may elicit more positive changes in both physical and
psychological parameters than no regular exercise or aerobic exercise
only.
Future research is needed comparing the effect of aerobics/strength
circuit training, as opposed to strength training only on changes in
body image as a function of age and gender. In particular, intervention
studies are needed to determine the optimal exercise program duration,
and to ascertain whether exercise-induced improvement in body image has
long-term as opposed to short-term benefits.
References
Ahmed, C., Hilton, W., and Pituch, K. (2002). Relations of strength
training to body image among a sample of female university students.
Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 16, 645-648.
American College of Sports Medicine (2000). Guidelines for exercise
testing and prescription (6th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott,
Williams, & Wilkins.
Anderson, M. L., Foster, C., McGuigan, M. R., Seebach, El, &
Porcari, J. P. (2004) Training vs. body image: does training improve
subjective appearance ratings? Journal of Strength and Conditioning
Research, 18, 255-259.
Brown, T.A., Cash, T. F., & Mikulka, P.J. (1990). Attitudinal
body-image assessment: Factor analysis of the Body-Self Relations
Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 135-144.
Cash, T. F. (1989). Body-image affect: Gestalt versus summing the
parts. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 17-18
Cash, T. F. (1994). Body-image attitudes: Evaluation, investment,
and affect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 1168-1170.
Cash, T. F. (1995). What do you see when you look in the mirror?
Helping yourself to a positive body image. New York, NY: Bantam Books.
Cash, T. F. (2000). The MBSRQ Users' Manual (3rd revision).
(Available from Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA23529-0267).
Cash, T. F., & Szyraanski, M. L. (1995). The development and
validation of the Body-image Ideals Questionnaire. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 63, 466-477.
Cohen, D., & Petrie, T. (2005). An examination of psychosocial correlates of disordered eating among undergraduate women. Sex Roles: A
Journal of Research, 52, 29-42.
Davis, C. (1997). Body image, exercise, and eating disorders. In
K.R. Fox (Ed.), The physical self: From motivation to well-being (lap
143-174). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Davis, C., & Fox., J. (1993). Excess exercise and weight
preoccupation in women. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 201-211.
Davis, C., Kennedy, S. H., Ravelski, E., & Dionne, M. (1994).
The role of physical activity in the development and maintenance of
eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 24, 957-967.
Finkenberg, M. E., DiNucci, J. M., McCune, S. L., & McCune, E.
D. (1993). Body esteem and enrollment in classes with different levels
of physical activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 783-792.
Ford, H. T., Puckett, J. R., Blessing, D. L., & Tucker, L. A.
(1989). Effects of selected physical activities on health-related
fitness and psychological well-being. Psychological Reports, 74,
203-208.
Franzoi, S. L., & Shields, S. A. (1984). The Body Esteem Scale:
Multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college population.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 173-178.
Golding, L. A., Myers, C. R., & Sinning, W. E. (1982). The
Y's way to physical fitness. Chicago, IL: National Board of the
YMCA.
Harrison, K. and Cantor, J. (1997). The relationship between media
consumption and eating disorders. Journal of Communication, 7, 40-67
Heinberg, L. J. (1995). Theories of body image disturbance. In J.
K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image, eating disorders and obesity: An
integrative guide for assessment and treatment. Washington, D.C.:
American Psychological Association.
Hinkle, D. E., Wiersma, W., & Jurs, S. G. (1998). Applied
statistics for the behavioral sciences. Boston, MA: Houston Mifflin.
Holmes, T., Chamberlin, P., & Young, M. (1994). Relations of
exercise to body image and sexual desirability among a sample of
university students. Psychological Reports, 74, 920-922.
Jackson, A. S., Pollock, M. L., & Ward, A. (1980). Generalized
equations for predicting body density of women. Medicine and Science in
Sports, 12, 175-182.
Johnson, A., Steinberg, R., & Lewis, W. (1988). Bulimia. In K.
Clark., R. Parr. & W. Castelli (Eds.), Evaluation and management of
eating disorders, anorexia, bulimia and obesity (pp. 177-190).
Champaign, Illinois: Life Enhancement Publications.
Keppell, G. (1991). Design and analysis: A researcher's
handbook. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Simon and Schuster.
Koff, E. & Bauman, C. L. (1997). Effects of wellness, fitness,
and sport skills programs on body image and lifestyle behaviors.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 555-562.
Kraimer, W. J., Keuning, M., Ratamess, N. A., Volek, J.S.,
McCormich, M., Bush, J. A., Nindl,, B. C., Gordon, S. E., Mazzetti, S.
A., Newton, R. U., Gomex, A L, Wickham, R. B., Rubin, M. R., and
Hakkinen, K. (2001). Resistance training combined with bench-step
aerobics enhances women's health profile. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 33, 259-269.
McArdle, W. D., Katch, F. L., Pechar, G. S., Jacobson, L, &
Puck, S. (1972). Reliability and interrelationships between maximal
oxygen intake, physical work capacity and step-test scores in college
women. Medicine and Science in Sports, 4, 182-186.
McArdle, W. D., Katch, V. L., & Katch, F. I., (1991). Exercise
physiology: Energy, nutrition, and human performance (3rd ed).
Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
Overdorf, V. G. (1991). Eating related problems in female athletes.
In D.R. Black (Ed.), Eating disorders among athletes (pp. 67-85).
Reston, VA: Association s of the American Alliance for Health, Physical
Education, Recreation and Dance.
Perry, A. C., Rosenblatt, E. S., Kempner, L., Feldman, B. B.,
Paolercio, M. A., & Van Bemden, A. L. (2002). The effects of an
exercise physiology program on physical fitness variables, body
satisfaction, and physiology knowledge. Journal of Strength and
Conditioning Research, 16, 219-226.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Striegel-Moore, R. H. (1985).
Women and weight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.),
Psychology and gender: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 267-307).
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Rowe, D. A. (1999). Development of the body self-image
questionnaire. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science,
3, 223-247.
Safrit, J. M., & Wood, T. M. (1995). Measurement in physical
education and exercise science (3rd ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Salem, S. K., & Elovson, A. C. (1993, August). Importance of
ideal body image, self-esteem and depression in females. Paper presented
at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Schuler, P. B., Broxon-Hutcherson, A., Philipp, S. F., Ryan, S.,
Isosaari, R. M., & Robinson, D. (2004). Body-shape perception in
older adults and motivations for exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
98, 1251-1260.
Secord, P. F., & Jourard, S. M. (1953). The appraisal of
body-cathexis: Body cathexis and the self. Journal of Consulting
Psychology, 17, 343-347.
Shontz, F. C. (1969). Perceptual and cognitive aspects of body
experience. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Sift, W. E. (1956). Gross composition of the body. In J. H.
Lawrence & C. A. Tobias (Eds.), Advances in biological and medical
physics (Vol. 4, pp. 239-280). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Skrinar, G. S., Bullen, B. A., Cheek, J. Mm, McArthur, J. W., &
Vaughan, L. K. (1986). Effects of endurance training on
body-consciousness in women. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 483-490.
Strelan, P., Mehaffey, & Tiggemann, M. (2003).
Self-objectification and esterrm in young women: the mediating role of
reasons for exercise. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 48, 89-95.
Striegel-Moore, R., McAvay, G., & Rodin, J. (1986).
Psychological and behavior correlates of feeling fat in women.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 935-947.
Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image, eating disorders, and obesity:
An integrative guide for assessment and treatment. Washington, D.C.:
American Psychological Association.
Thomsen, S. R., Weber, M. M., & Brown, L. B. (2001). The
relationship between health and fitness magazine reading and
eating-disordered weight-loss methods among high school girls. American
Journal of Health Education, 32, 133-138.
Tiggemann, M., & Williamson, S. (2000). The effect of exercise
on body satisfaction and self-esteem as a function of gender and age.
Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 44, 119-129.
Tucker, L. A., & Maxwell, K. (1992). Effects of weight-training
on the emotional well-being and body image of females: Predictors of
greatest benefit. American Journal of Health Promotion, 6, 338-344.
Tucker, L. A., & Mortell, R. (1994). Comparison of the effects
of walking and weight-training program on body image in middle-aged
women: An experimental study. American Journal of Health Promotion, 8,
34-42.
Van den Buick, J. (2000). Is television bad for your health?
Behavior and body image of the adolescent "couch potato."
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 273-281.
Williams, P. A., & Cash, T. F. (2001). Effects of a circuit
weight-training program on the body images of college students. The
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 75-82.
Wylie, R. C. (1974). The self-concept: A review of methodological
considerations and measuring instruments (Rev. ed.). Lincoln, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.
Ruth N. Henry
Lipscomb University
Mark H. Anshel
Middle Tennessee State University
Timothy Michael
Western Michigan University
Address correspondence To: Ruth N. Henry, D.A., Department of
Kinesiology, Lipscomb University, Nahsville, TN 37204 E-mail:
[email protected]
Table 1. Descriptive data and demographics.
Aerobics Interval Circuit
(n=23) * (n=28) *
Mean [+ or -] SD Mean [+ or -] SD
Age (yrs) 19.4 [+ or -] 1.2 19.1 [+ or -] 1.6
Weight (kg) 63.8 [+ or -] 13.2 61.3 [+ or -] 7.3
Height (in) 65.6 [+ or -] 2.2 64.6 [+ or -] 1.9
Body mass index 22.9 [+ or -] 4.3 22.8 [+ or -] 2.5
(kg/m2)
Control
(n=21) *
Mean [+ or -] SD
Age (yrs) 20.1 [+ or -] 1.6
Weight (kg) 63.7 [+ or -] 11.3
Height (in) 66.2 [+ or -] 3.5
Body mass index 22.4 [+ or -] 2.8
(kg/m2)
* One subject withdrew from each group. Sample sizes reflect intact
groups.
Table 2. Example items from the Body Self-Image Questionnaire
Subscale # of Items Example Item
OAE 6 I look good in clothes
FE 9 Parts of my body are fat
HFE 7 My body is strong
HFI 6 I watch carefully what I eat, to maintain
a health body.
AG 4 I'm usually well dressed
SD 3 I compare my body to people I'm close to
(friends, relatives, etc.)
HD 3 I wish I were a different height
NA 6 Most days I feel bad about my body
II 7 I particularly notice how much body fat
other people have
OAE = overall appearance evaluation; FE = fatness evaluation;
AG = attention to grooming; HFE = health/fitness evaluation;
HFI = health/fitness influence; SD = social dependence;
HD = height dissatisfaction; NA = negative affect;
II = investment in ideals
Table 3. Pretest and posttest means and standard deviations for
physical fatness and body image variables.
Aerobics
Pretest Posttest
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
%Fat 23.54 (6.7) 22.09 (5.6)
[VO.sub.2max] 35.77 (4.7) 37.01 (4.1)
Strength 20.82 (9.2) 26.00 (10.8)
OAE 19.65 (5.7) 20.26 (5.5)
FE 29.91 (8.7) 29.35 (9.3)
AG 16.35 (2.4) 16.43 (2.5)
HFE 24.74 (4.8) 24.43 (5.5)
HFI 23.83 (3.5) 25.00 (3.5)
SD 11.43 (2.1) 11.43 (2.5)
HD 8.35 (4.2) 8.52 (4.0)
NA 18.17 (7.4) 16.52 (7.1)
II 29.70 (4.3) 28.65 (5.0)
Interval Circuit
Pretest Posttest
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
%Fat 23.02 (5.0) 20.19 (4.7)
[VO.sub.2max] 35.12 (2.9) 37.40 (3.0)
Strength 24.25 (10.2) 33.50 (10.5)
OAE 18.93 (5.3) 21.50 (4.5)
FE 29.21 (9.4) 26.61 (9.9)
AG 15.89 (2.3) 16.21 (2.2)
HFE 22.32 (4.9) 25.68 (4.8)
HFI 23.29 (3.2) 24.96 (3.1)
SD 11.43 (2.1) 10.82 (2.4)
HD 6.89 (3.8) 5.93 (3.2)
NA 17.93 (6.8) 14.29 (6.0)
II 27.93 (5.2) 27.39 (4.7)
Control
Pretest Posttest
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
%Fat 22.28 (6.1) 22.86 (5.5)
[VO.sub.2max] 36.48 (3.7) 36.30 (3.02)
Strength 26.05 (9.9) 27.10 (11.1)
OAE 21.05 (4.8) 20.95 (3.9)
FE 24.48 (10.1) 25.67 (11.3)
AG 16.14 (2.7) 15.10 (2.9)
HFE 23.76 (5.1) 24.71 (6.8)
HFI 23.38 (5.2) 23.52 (3.7)
SD 10.00 (4.0) 10.10 (3.7)
HD 6.33 (3.9) 6.00 (3.9)
NA 13.29 (7.6) 14.14 (7.1)
II 26.43 (72.) 25.90 (7.4)
[V.sub.O2max] = estimated [V.sub.O2max]; Bench = number of bench press
repetitions; OAE overall appearance evaluation; FE = fatness
evaluation; AG = attention to grooming; HFE = health/fitness
evaluation; HFI = health/fitness influence; SD = social dependence;
HD = height dissatisfaction; NA = negative affect;
II = investment in ideals
Table 4. Results of MANOVA, Univariate ANOVAs, and pairwise comparisons
for physical fitness variables.
Main Effect (Time) Interaction (Group x Time)
F p F p
MANOVA
39.33 <.001 12.99 <.001
Univariate
ANOVAs
Fat 24.08 <.001 * 17.39 <.001 *
VO2 max 10.11 .002 * 7.30 .001 *
Strength 52.63 <.001 * 13.30 <.001 *
Pairwise Comparisons (v)
A vs. I/C A vs. C I/C vs. C
MANOVA
Univariate
ANOVAs
Fat .013 (1) .004 (1) <.001 (1)
VO2 max .217 .202 .002 (2)
Strength .016 (3) .125 <.001 (3)
A = Aerobics group; I/C = Interval circuit group; C = Control group
% Fat = percent body fat; [VO.sub.2max] = estimated [VO.sub.2max];
Strength number of bench press repetitions
* significant with modified Bonferroni adjustment (p<.033)
(1 2 3) Significant pairwise comparisons following significant group
x time interaction:
(1) Interval circuit < aerobics < control (p<.05)
(2) Interval circuit > control (p<.05)
(3) Interval circuit > aerobics and control (p<.05)
Table 5. Results of MANOVA, Univariate ANOVAs, and Pairwise Comparisons
for Body Image Variables.
Main Effect (Time) Interaction (Group x Time)
F p F p
MANOVA 3.54 .001 2.02 .013
Univariate
ANOVAs
OAE 6.78 .011 (1) 4.33 .017
FE 1.51 .224 4.27 .018
AG .82 .368 3.11 .051
HFE 724 .009 (1) 5.08 .009 *
HFI 10.52 .002 (1,2) 2.13 .126
SD .35 .555 .63 .536
HD 2.49 .118 2.08 .133
NA 8.53 .005 (1,2) 6.68 .002 *
II 3.26 .075 0.19 .825
Pairwise Comparison (p)
A vs. I/C A vs. C I/C vs. C
MANOVA
Univariate
ANOVAs
OAE
FE
AG
HFE .0193 (3) .665 .180
HFI
SD
HD
NA .167 .561 .013 (4)
II
A = Aerobics group; I/C = Interval circuit group; C = Control group
OE = overall appearance evaluation; FE = fatness evaluation;
AG = attention to grooming; HFE = health/fitness evaluation;
HFI = health/fitness influence; SD = social dependence; HD = height
dissatisfaction; NA = negative affect; II = investment in ideals
* significant interaction with modified Bonferroni adjustment (p<.011)
Simple main effects for time:
(1) Interval circuit group improvement (p<.001)
(2) Aerobics group improvement (p<.05)
Significant pairwise comparisons in variables with significant group
x time interaction:
(3) Interval circuit > aerobics (p = .019)
(4) Interval circuit < control (p = .013)