Examining the potential causal relationship between sport team identification and psychological well-being.
Wann, Daniel L.
A number of social scientists have proposed theoretical models
suggesting that social support networks can serve as important buffers
from anxiety, loneliness and the like, thereby assisting individuals in
maintaining their psychological health (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Rowe
& Kahn, 1998; Thoits, 1982; Wann& Hamlet, 1994, 1996). One of
the more extensive explanations of the impact of social groups on
self-esteem and psychological health is provided by Tajfel (1981) in his
social identity theory. Social identity theory posits that individuals
prefer and attempt to maintain positive self- and social-identities. By
identifying with and maintaining memberships in valued social groups and
networks, a person's social-identity can be enhanced, thereby
benefiting their overall self-concept (Hogg & Abrams, 1990).
The benefits of social connections are found among many groups,
including those who are stigmatized (Crocker & Major, 1989), and in
a variety of settings, including sport and physical activity (Bianco
& Eklund, 2001). In fact, recent work indicates that social
connections are particularly important for stigmatized athletes (Krane,
Barber, & McClung, 2002). Further, the benefits of social support
are not limited to one's psychology but, rather, also predict both
physical health (Cohen, Doyle, Skoner, Rabin, & Gwaltney, 1997) and
longevity of life (Rowe & Kahn, 1998).
For many years, authors have suggested that sport fandom is a
societal connection that may assist in the maintenance of psychological
health (Curtis, Loy, & Karnilowicz, 1986; Eastman & Land, 1997;
Melnick, 1993; Pan, Gabert, McGaugh, & Branvold, 1997; Smith, 1988,
1989; Zillmann, Bryant, & Sapolsky, 1989). Indeed, as early as 1929,
Brill stated "are you a fan? It is altogether to be hoped, for your
psychic health and well-being, that you are" (p. 429). However,
until the last decade, claims of the potential psychological benefits of
sport fandom had remained largely speculative. To remedy this, Wann and
his colleagues have conducted a series of empirical investigations into
the relationship between fandom and psychological health. Wann's
research program has tested the hypothesis that high levels of
identification with (i.e., psychological connection to) a local sport
team are related to psychological well-being (see Wann, Melnick,
Russell, & Pease, 2001). By identifying with a local sport team, an
individual becomes attached to a larger social group. These associations
to other fans form the basis for a valuable connection to society at
large and serve as a buffer to loneliness, isolation, and so forth.
Consequently, those with strong ties to the local team would likely
exhibit a more positive psychological profile than those lacking in team
identification.
Identification with a distant team and mere sport fandom per se are
not expected to provide significant well-being benefits because these
factors do not readily lead to social connections, the driving force
behind the benefits of team identification (Wann et al., 2001). With
respect to identification with a distant team, it is likely difficult
for such fans to find and interact with fellow fans of their team.
Rather, they are more likely to either be surrounded by fans of rival
teams (who may openly dislike and perhaps ridicule the fan's
choice) or find that other fans are disinterested in their preferred
team. In either situation, it would be difficult to generate social
connections through their association with their team. In the case of
general fandom, again, the important social connections that accompany
identification with a local team are lacking. Social support benefits
result from active group participation in an activity, not simple
interest (Rowe & Kahn, 1998). For instance, sport consumption can
easily be done in the privacy of one's own home via television,
radio, and the Internet. Certainly, fans can commune with one another,
regardless of their level of interest in any one team (e.g., Monday
morning water cooler conversations following Sunday football games).
However, interacting with others who share in one's passion for a
specific team or player adds an additional interpersonal link. One need
only watch the interactions occurring at tail-gating parties to see this
in action. Simply put, there is likely to be a greater bond between two
die-hard fans of a specific team than between two die-hard fans of a
specific sport (similar to the bond that fans of a specific musical
group feel, versus the bond felt by fans of a specific type of music).
Such logic is supported by decades of research indicating that
interpersonal attraction, cohesion, and the like are strongly linked to
the similarity of the parties involved (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1992;
Kelley et al., 1983; Turner, 1999).
Wann's first attempt to empirically examine the team
identification-psychological health relationship involved college
students who completed the Sport Spectator Identification Scale (SSIS;
Wann & Branscombe, 1993) for their university's men's
basketball team and scales assessing personal self-esteem and depression
(Branscombe & Wann, 1991). Consistent with expectations, the results
revealed a positive correlation between level of identification and
self-esteem and a negative correlation between identification and
frequency of depression. Branscombe and Wann found that higher levels of
identification were positively correlated with the frequency of positive
emotions and negatively correlated with the frequency of negative
emotions and alienation. Warm (1994) was able to replicate the
relationship between identification and self-esteem using collective
(i.e., social) self-esteem.
A somewhat more extensive examination of the psychological health
of highly identified sport fans was recently published by Wann, Inman,
Ensor, Gates, and Caldwell (1999). These authors used the SSIS to
classify college students into two groups: those with high levels of
identification with their university's men's basketball team
and those with low levels of identification with the team. Using the
Profile of Mood State (POMS; Grove & Prapavessis, 1992), Wann et al.
compared the mood profiles of the two identification groups. As
hypothesized, the highly identified groups reported a more positive
profile. Specifically, compared to those in the low identification
group, the highly identified group reported higher levels of self-esteem
and vigor, and lower levels of depression, anger, confusion, tension,
and fatigue. However, when the participants were classified into high
and low general fandom groups, no group differences in psychological
health emerged. In a second study, participants were classified into
groups high and low in identification with a distant (but equally
popular) team and asked to complete the POMS. There were no differences
in psychological health between the two groups. Thus, Wann et al. found
that it was neither mere sport fandom nor identification with a distant
team that predicted psychological health. Rather, psychological health
was only related to a high level of team identification with a local
team.
Wann, Dunham, Byrd, and Keenan (2004) conducted another recent
study on the psychological well-being of highly identified sport fans.
These authors attempted to extend previous research by using the NEO
PI-R, an instrument assessing the five-factor model of personality
(John, 1990). Previous research (Costa & McCrae, 1992; McCrae &
Costa, 1991) had found that several of the personality domains assessed
by the NEO PI-R were related to psychological well-being and that the
instrument was a valid measure of this construct (i.e., psychological
health corresponded with higher levels of extraversion, openness, and
conscientiousness, and lower levels of neuroticism). Consequently, Wann
and his colleagues expected positive relationships between
identification with a local team and extraversion, openness, and
conscientiousness while a negative relationship was expected with
neuroticism. The results confirmed each hypothesis, with the exception
of the expected negative association between identification and
neuroticism where no relationship was found. There was no relationship
between NEO PI-R domains scores and identification with a distant team.
By combining the aforementioned empirical investigations with the
theoretical suggestions offered by Wann and his colleagues, a
theoretical model can be developed. This theory, referred to here as the
Team Identification--Psychological Health Model, contains three
predictions:
1) High levels of identification with a local sport team will lead
to positive psychological health.
2) High levels of identification with a distant sport team will not
be sufficient to garner the psychological well-being effects.
3) High levels of mere sport fandom will not be sufficient to
garner the psychological well-being effects.
The previously described literature strongly supports both
Prediction 2 and Prediction 3. Further, tentative support has been found
for Prediction 1. That is, the literature to date supports the notion
that strong psychological ties to a local sport team are positively
related to psychological health. However, this body of evidence is only
tentative support for the Prediction 1 because, as noted by Warm and his
associates (Wann, 1994; Wann et al., 1999), the research to date has
been correlational in nature. Information regarding the causal direction
of the relationship is lacking and it is not possible to determine if
higher levels of identification cause better psychological health, vice
versa, or if the relationship tends to be bidirectional (i.e.,
circular). This is an important point, and a vital piece missing in the
research to date, because the model being proposed here explicitly
hypothesizes a causal pattern in which identification with a local team
has a direct and positive effect on one's psychological health.
While a number of persons have proposed such a pattern of effects (e.g.,
Melnick, 1993; Smith, 1989; Wann et al., 2004; Warm et al., 2001;
Zillmann et al., 1989), to date empirical support for the causality component of the relationship between identification with a local team
and wellbeing was lacking.
Using longitudinal research and structural equation modeling, the
focus of the current investigation was to examine the potential causal
relationship between sport team identification and psychological
well-being. Specifically, Prediction 1 of the model was tested using a
cross-lagged longitudinal design methodology. It was hypothesized that
there would be a significant relationship between identification with a
local sport team and psychological health at Time 1 (i.e., the
synchronous correlation). This prediction simply reflects the
aforementioned research indicating a positive relationship between these
variables (i.e., the tentative support for Prediction 1 noted above).
Also, the Time 1 to Time 2 autocorrelation paths for both team
identification and psychological health were expected to be significant,
which simply reflects the consistency found in one's level of
identification and psychological wellbeing. The key predictions, .of
course, involved the cross-lagged paths from team identification at Time
1 to psychological health at Time 2, and from psychological health at
Time 1 to team identification at Time 2. It was hypothesized that the
identification to psychological health chronological pattern would be
significant while the psychological health to identification path would
not.
Method
Time 1
Participants. The initial, Time 1 sample consisted of 214 (90 male;
124 female) university students earning extra course credit in exchange
for participation. They had a mean age of 21.64 years (SD = 4.63, range
= 18 to 53).
Materials and Procedure. Upon entering the testing room and
providing their consent to participate, the participants (tested in
small groups) were asked to complete a questionnaire packet containing
three sections. The first section contained demographic items assessing
age, gender, name, and telephone number. The participants were informed
that their name and telephone number were requested because this was a
two-part study and some of the participants would be contacted at a
later date to, if they so chose, participate in the second phase of the
research. They were informed that once the participants had been
contacted, the portion of the packet containing identifying information
would be removed and destroyed, thus insuring anonymity.
The second section of the questionnaire packet contained the Sport
Spectator Identification Scale (SSIS, Wann & Branscombe, 1993). The
SSIS contains 7 Likert-scale items with response options ranging from 1
(low identification) to 8 (high identification). The participants
targeted their university's men's basketball team when
completing the SSIS. Wann and Branscombe report strong internal
consistency, test-retest reliability, and criterion validity for the
SSIS, and the questionnaire has been successfully used in a number of
countries (see Wann et al., 2001). A sample item contained in the SSIS
reads "How important to you is it that (name of target team)
wins?"
The third section contained four measures designed to assess
psychological health. First, participants completed the 10-item
Rosenberg (1979) Personal Self-esteem Scale. This highly reliable and
valid instrument is a standard measure of an individual's personal
self-evaluation. Next, they completed Luhtanen and Crocker's (1991)
16-item Collective Self-esteem Scale. This reliable and valid scale was
selected because it is currently the most psychometrically sound
assessment of social-level (i.e., group) self-esteem. Third,
participants completed the 20-item UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell,
Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980), a reliable and valid measure of
loneliness. And finally, respondents completed the 14-item Perceived
Stress Scale developed by Cohen, Kamarck, and Mermelstein (1983). This
measure is a standard assessment of stress. In addition to being
selected based on sound psychometric qualities, these inventories were
used because past work had successfully incorporated then into research
assessing the mental well-being of sport fans (e.g, Branscombe
&Wann, 1991; Warm, 1994; Wann, Dimmock, & Grove, in press).
These measures were scored so that higher numbers reflected better
psychological health (i.e., higher personal and collective self-esteem,
lower levels of loneliness and perceived stress).
After the participants had completed their packet, they returned it
to a researcher who handed them a debriefing statement. This statement
reminded the participants that they might be contacted at a later date
to complete the second phase of the research (it was explicitly noted
that any further involvement was strictly voluntary). The statement also
contained information on contacting the researcher for a final report of
the project. Once the participants had received the debriefing
statement, they were excused from the testing session. The sessions
lasted approximately 30 minutes.
Time 2
Participants. The Time 2 sample consisted of 45 (10 male; 35
female) university students earning a five-dollar girl certificate to
the university's bookstore in exchange for participation (21
percent of the original sample participated in the second phase of the
project). They had a mean age of 20.18 years (SD = 3.60, range = 18 to
39).
Materials and Procedure. Approximately three months after the
initial testing was complete, the participants were contacted by phone
and asked if they would be willing to participate in the second phase of
the study. They were informed that the second phase would involve
completing a questionnaire packet similar to that which they had
completed earlier. Those agreeing to participate were given information
on where and when the second phase would occur.
Upon entering the testing room for Phase 2 and providing their
consent to participate, participants (again tested in small groups) were
asked to complete a questionnaire packet nearly identical to the packet
used in Time 1. The only difference between the packets was that the
demographic section was omitted from the packet used in Time 2. After
the participants had completed their Time 2 packet, they returned it to
a researcher who handed them a debriefing statement. This statement
described the nature and hypotheses of the study and conmined
information on contacting the researcher for a final report of the
project. Once the participants had received the debriefing statement,
they were excused from the testing session. The sessions again lasted
approximately 30 minutes.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Prior to testing the hypotheses, a series of preliminary analyses
were conducted. First, to simplify the analyses, scores on the four
assessments of psychological well-being (i.e., personal and collective
self-esteem, loneliness, and perceived stress) were transformed into z
scores and combined to form a single index of psychological health. Such
a procedure was appropriate because each of the four measures of
psychological health were intercorrelated, with coefficients ranging
from .55 to .81 (all ps < .01).
Next, to insure that the Time 2 sample was representative of the
Time 1 sample, a series of one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were
conducted to examine scores on the measures of identification and
psychological health for those participants who did and did not
participate in the second phase. This series of analyses revealed that
the two groups did not differ on either team identification, F(1,210) =
0.08,p >.77, or psychological health, F(1,210) = 0.96,p > .32
(Time 1 scores). Thus, it was reasonable to conclude that the original
sample of 214 persons was accurately represented by the Time 2 sample.
The final set of preliminary analyses involved testing for gender
differences in the measures of identification and psychological health
for those participating in the second phase of the research. A series of
one-way ANOVAs revealed that males and females did not significantly
differ in psychological health at Time 1, F(1, 43) = 0.67,p > .41, or
Time 2, F(1,43) = 1.60,p > .21. Further, males and females did not
significantly differ in level of identification at Time 1, F(1,43) =
0.04,p > .83, or Time 2, F(1,43) = 0.82,p > .36.
Structural Equation Modeling Analyses
The temporal relationships between team identification and
psychological health were tested using structural equation modeling
analyzed with Bentler's (1995) EQS program. The correlation matrix for the variables in the equations appears in Table 1. This analysis
indicated that the model was highly consistent with the data, [x.sup.2]
(1) = .09, p = .77, comparative fit index = 1.00. An examination of the
pattern of coefficients shown in Figure 1 (in particular the crosslagged
coefficients) indicates strong support for the hypothesized pattern of
effects. Specifically, the path from team identification at Time 1 to
psychological health at Time 2 was significant (and positive) while the
inverse chronological pattern was not.
Discussion
The results presented above, in combination with previous research,
provide strong support for each of the three predictions comprising the
Team Identification--Psychological Health Model. Specifically, level of
identification With a local sport team is positively correlated with
psychological well-being (Branscombe &Wann, 1991; Wann, 1994; Wann
et al., 1999; Warm et al., 2004), the relationship appears to involve an
identification to psychological health causal pattern (the current
study), and identification with distant teams and general sport fandom
are not related to psychological health (Wann et al., 1999; Wann et al.,
2004). However, it does warrant mention that while structural equation
modeling and cross-lagged methodologies provide important clues with
respect to causal patterns among variables, they do not provide the
definitive answers gained through true experimental methodologies. Thus,
while the data and analyses reported above provide strong support for
Prediction 1 of the Team Identification--Psychological Health Model,
additional experimental research is still needed. In all likelihood,
this research would involve randomly assigning individuals to various
groups, manipulating level of identification with a local sport team,
and some time later, assessing and comparing the psychological health of
the two groups. Although such an undertaking would be cumbersome, it
would be feasible given current information on the antecedents of team
identification. For instance, Wann, Tucker, and Schrader (1996) found
that a number of factors influenced fans' decisions to originally
identify with a particular sport team. Some of these factors, such as
the socialization fans received from their parents would be difficult if
not impossible to manipulate. The experimental manipulation of other
factors, however, such as the importance of geography (i.e., supporting
a local team), the desire to support one's own school's team,
and importance of group affiliations gained through one's
identification with the team, would be plausible. For instance,
individuals randomly assigned to the "high identification"
condition could be invited to social gatherings with other fans, receive
special seating in the arena, and be reminded that the team is a
representation of their university. Because there appear to be cultural
differences in identification decisions (Jones, 1997; see Wann et al.,
2001), one would need to be particularly careful in selecting the
factors to be manipulated.
The finding that team identification has direct positive effects on
psychological wellbeing is particularly important in light of the fact
that many traditional connections to society (e.g., religion and
extended family) appear to be declining (Denney, 1979; Guttmann, 1978;
Putnam, 1995). Affiliations with others stemming from religious
institutions, work-related organizations, and relationships with
extended family members have all have shown reduced numbers in recent
years. It has been argued that identification with sport teams may serve
to replace the traditional but declining social ties as members of
society attempt to reestablish and maintain their social connectedness (Melnick, 1993). Consequently, these identifications become more vital
to the maintenance of psychological well-being. That is, because social
support opportunities are becoming sparse, identifications with local
sport teams may become increasingly valuable as "effective venues
for social capital formation" (Wann et al., 2001, p. 187).
Although the data detailed above support the notion that high
levels of identification with a local sport team can have measurable
psychological benefits, this does not imply that such fans are immune
from the negative affect fans experience subsequent to a favorite
team's defeat. Rather, highly identified fans are particularly
likely to report negative emotion (Bernhardt, Dabbs, Fielden, &
Lutter, 1998; Wann, Dolan, McGeorge, & Allison, 1994) and depression
(Schwarz, Strack, Kommer, & Wagner, 1987; Schweitzer, Zillmann,
Weaver, & Luttrell, 1992) upon witnessing a poor performance by
their team. Thus, we are left with a bit of a paradox. How can highly
identified persons possess a particularly positive mental health profile
if these same persons experience such intense negative responses to
their team's losses? According to Warm and his colleagues (2001),
the solution to this paradox lies in the defense mechanisms highly
identified fans develop to help them cope with their team's poor
play and, ultimately, return to a positive mental state. Warm et al.
noted several such defense mechanisms including derogation of outgroup
fans (Branscombe & Wann, 1994), biased attributions that place the
blame for the team's failures on outside forces (Hastorf &
Cantril, 1954; Wann & Dolan, 1994a; Warm & Schrader, 2000), and
biased evaluations of the team's past and future (Dietz-Uhler &
Murrell, 1999; Warm & Dolan, 1994b). By employing these and other
defense strategies, highly identified fans are able (with time) to
handle their team's failures and once again benefit from the social
connections stemming from their allegiance to a local team.
One portion of the Team Identification--Psychological Health Model
that could use further development concerns the notion of local and
distant teams. As mentioned previously, research indicates that
significant psychological health benefits are associated with local team
attachments but not associations with distant teams (Warm et al., 2004;
Warm et al., 1999). However, precise operational definitions of
"local" and "distant" teams have yet to be
articulated. Should classifying teams as local or distant be a function
of the number of miles separating the team from the fan, the amount of
media exposure the teams receive, or the number of other supporters for
the team found in the general vicinity (to name but a few of the
potential operationalizations of these terms)? Precise definitions of
the team types are important because they would imply which fans would
benefit from their association with the team. For instance, Wann and his
associates (2004) have suggested that when highly identified fans of
distant teams are in the company of other fans of this team, they may
gain temporary increases in their psychological health as a consequence
of their identification. Their argument was that when individuals are in
the presence of fellow fans of the distant team, these temporary social
connections should provide these persons with a valuable link to society
at large and, consequently, result in psychological benefits. Such a
suggestion seems logical and has support from research indicating that
the salience of a social category can have a powerful impact on
intergroup behavior (Smith, Spears, & Hamstra, 1999; Spears, Doosje,
& Ellemers, 1999; Turner, 1999; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, &
Wetherell, 1987). Perhaps salience impacts the Team
Identification--Psychological Health Model as well. That is, it is
certainly logical to view local teams as more salient than distant
teams--they receive greater levels of media coverage, the fans of the
team are more visible, consumption of the team is more convenient (e.g.,
attending games and buying team paraphernalia), and so forth. However,
in certain instances distant teams become quite salient, such as when
one is surrounded by other fans of the distant team. Likewise, there may
be situations in which the local team is not salient, for instance, when
one is alone or during the team's off-season. In the former
situation, the "distant" social category of team follower
would be salient, even though the team originated some distance from the
fan. In the latter instance, the social category would not be salient,
even though the team was "local". These situations may reveal
patterns that are reversed from past work; for fans of distant teams
(because the category of team follower is salient) identification may be
positively related to psychological health, but for fans of local but
not salient teams, identification might not be associated with
well-being. Future work that manipulates salience is needed to determine
the impact of this variable on the psychological health benefits
reported by highly identified fans.
Although examining potential causality was the logical next step in
validating the Team Identification--Psychological Health Model, there
are many aspects to the relationship between identification and
well-being that remain unclear and untested. For example, the impact of
multiple ties to local (or salient) sport teams is yet unknown. If high
levels of identification with one local sport team are associated with
psychological well-being, would identifying strongly with multiple teams
lead to an even better state of mental health? Or, rather, is
identification with one team sufficient to garner the maximum benefits
to be accrued through sport team identification? The impact of
identification with "lesser" sports is also unknown. In the
current and past research (e.g., Branscombe & Wann, 1991; Wann,
1994), the target of the identification was a locally popular sport
team. Although the Team Identification--Psychological Health Model would
predict that identification with lesser teams would also be related to
psychological health (as long as societal connections could be obtained
through associations with the team), such a prediction is as yet
untested. And finally, because most of the past research has examined
identification with university basketball teams (e.g., Branscombe &
Wann, 1991; Wann et al., 2004), future endeavors should focus on
additional sports and levels of competition.
Acknowledgements
This project was partially supported by funds from the Murray State
University Provost's Office. The author thanks Brian Keenan for his
assistance with the data collection and entry.
References
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual.
Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.
Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, C. D.
(1998). Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and
losing among fans at sporting events. Physiology and Behavior, 65,
59-62.
Bianco, T., & Eklund, R. C. (2001). Conceptual considerations
for social support research in sport and exercise settings: The case of
sport injury. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 23, 85-107.
Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1991). The positive social
and self-concept conse-quences of sport team identification. Journal of
Sport & Social Issues, 15, 115-127.
Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1994). Collective self-esteem
consequences ofoutgroup derogation when a valued social identity is on
trial. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 641-657.
Brill, A. A. (1929). The why of the fan. North American Review,
228, 429-434.
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Rabin, B. S., &
Gwaltney, J. M. Jr. (1997). Social ties and susceptibility to the common
cold. Journal of the American Medical Association, 277, 1940-1944.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, D. R., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global
measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24,
385-395.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and
the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357.
Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO Personality
Inventory Revised manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem:
The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96,
608-630.
Curtis, J., Loy, J., & Karnilowicz, W. (1986). A comparison of
suicide-dip effects of major sports events and civil holidays. Sociology
of Sport Journal, 3, 1-14.
Dietz-Uhler, B., & Murrell, A. (1999). Examining fan reactions
to game outcomes: A longitudinal study of social identity. Journal of
Sport Behavior, 22, 15-27.
Denney, R. (1979). Feast of strangers: Varieties of social
experience in America. In H. J.
Gans, N. Glazer, J. R. Gusfield, & C. Jencks (Eds.), On the
making of Americans: Essays in honor of David Riesman (pp. 251-269).
Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Eastman, S. T., & Land, A. M. (1997). The best of both worlds:
Sports fans find good seats at the bar. Journal of Sport & Social
Issues, 21, 156-178.
Grove, J. R., & Prapavessis, H. (1992). Preliminary evidence
for the reliability and validity of an abbreviated profile of mood
states. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 93-109.
Guttmann, A. (1978). From ritual to record: The nature of modern
sports. New York: Columbia University Press.
Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case
study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129-134.
Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (1992). Liking, loving, and
relating (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1990). Social motivation,
self-esteem, and social identity. In D. Abrams & M. Hogg (Eds.),
Social identity theory: Constructive and critical advances (pp. 28-47).
New York: Springer-Verlag.
John, O. P. (1990). The "Big Five" factor taxonomy:
Dimensions of personality in the natural language and in questionnaires.
In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp.
66-100). New York: Guilford.
Jones, I. (1997). A further examination of the factors influencing
current identification with a sports team, a response to Wann, et al.
(1996). Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 257-258.
Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H.,
Hutson, T. L., Levinger, G., McClintock. E., Peplau, L. A., &
Peterson, D. R. (1983). Close relationships. New York: Freeman.
Krane, V., Barber, H., & McClung, L. R. (2002). Social
psychological benefits of Gay Games participation: A social identity
theory explanation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 27-42.
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1991). Self-esteem and intergroup
comparison: Toward a theory of collective self-esteem. In J. Suls &
T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparisons: Contemporary theory and research
(pp. 211-234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1991). Adding Liebe und
arbeit: The full five-factor model and well-being. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227-232.
Melnick, M. J. (1993). Searching for sociability in the stands: A
theory of sports spectating. Journal of Sport Management, 7, 44-60.
Pan, D. W., Gabert, T. E., McGaugh, E. C., & Branvold, S. E.
(1997). Factors and differential demographic effects on purchases of
season tickets for intercollegiate basketball games. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 20, 447-464.
Putnam, R. D. (1995). Bowling alone: America's declining
social capital. Journal of Democracy, 6, 65-78.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1998). Successful aging. New York:
Dell.
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The
revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480.
Schwarz, N., Strack, E, Kommer, D., & Wagner, D. (1987).
Soccer, rooms, and the quality of your life: Mood effects on judgments
of satisfaction with life in general and with specific domains. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 69-79.
Schweitzer, K., Zillmann, D., Weaver, J. B., & Luttrell, E. S.
(1992). Perception of threatening events in the emotional aftermath of a
televised college football game. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media, 36, 75-82.
Smith, G. J. (1988). The noble sports fan. Journal of Sport &
Social Issues, 12, 54-65.
Smith, G. J. (1989). The noble sports redux. Journal of Sport &
Social Issues, 13, 121-130.
Smith, H. J., Spears, R., & Hamstra, I. J. (1999). Social
identity and the context of relative deprivation. In N. Ellemers, R.
Spears, & B. Doosje (Eds.), Social Identity (pp. 205229). Malden,
MA: Blackwell.
Spears, R., Doosje, B., & Ellemers, N. (1999). Commitment and
the context of social perception. In N. Ellemers, R. Spears, & B.
Doosje (Eds.), Social Identity (pp. 59-83).Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Thoits, P. A. (1982). Conceptual, methodological, and theoretical
problems in studying social support as a buffer against life stress.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 23, 145-159.
Turner, J. C., Hoog, M., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., &
Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A
self-categorization theory. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
Warm, D. L. (1994). The "noble" sports fan: The
relationships between team identification, self-esteem, and aggression.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 864-866.
Warm, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring
degree of identification with the team. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 24, 1-17.
Warm, D. L., Dimmock, J. A., & Grove, J. R. (in press).
Generalizing the Team IdentificationPsychological Health Model to a
Different Sport and Culture: The Case of Australian Rules Football.
Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice.
Warm, D. L., & Dolan, T. J. (1994a). Attributions of highly
identified sport spectators. The Journal of Social Psychology, 134,
783-792.
Warm, D. L., & Dolan, T. J. (1994b). Influence of
spectators' identification on evaluation of the past present and
future performance of a sports team. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78,
547-552.
Warm, D. L., Dolan, T. J., McGeorge, K. K., & Allison, J. A.
(1994). Relationships between spectator identification and
spectators' perceptions of influence, spectators' emotions,
and competition outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 16,
347-364.
Warm, D. L., Dunham, M. D., Byrd, M. L., & Keenan, B. L.
(2004). The five-factor model of personality and the psychological
health of highly identified sport fans. International Sports Journal,
8(2), 28-36.
Wann, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1994). The Joiners Scale:
Validation of a measure of social-complexity. Psychological Reports, 74,
1027-1034.
Warm, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1996). Being a "joiner"
and psychological well-being. Psychological Reports, 79, 1186.
Warm, D. L., Inman, S., Ensor, C. L, Gates, R. D., & Caldwell,
D. S. (1999). Assessing the psychological well-being of sport fans using
the Profile of Mood States: The importance of team identification.
International Sports Journal, 3, 81-90.
Warm, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W., & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York: Routledge Press.
Warm, D. L., & Schrader, M. P. (2000). Controllability and
stability in the self-serving attributions of sport spectators. Journal
of Social Psychology, 140, 160-168.
Warm, D. L., Tucker, K. B., & Schrader, M. P. (1996). An
exploratory examination of the factors influencing the origination,
continuation, and cessation of identification with sports teams.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 995-1101.
Zillmann, D., Bryant, J., & Sapolsky, B. S. (1989). Enjoyment
from sports spectatorship. In J. H. Goldstein (Ed.), Sports, games, and
play: Social and psychological viewpoints (2nd ed., pp. 241-278).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Daniel L. Wann
Murray State University
Address Correspondence To: Daniel L. Wann, Department of
Psychology, Murray State University, Murray, KY 42071,
email:
[email protected].
Table 1
Correlation Matrix for the Variables Included in the Structural
Equation Modeling Analyses.
Time 1 Time 2
Identification Identification
Time 1 Identification --
Time 2 Identification .77 ** --
Time 1 Well-being .34 * .18
Time 2 Well-being .43 ** .28
Time 1 Time 2
Well-being Well-being
Time 1 Identification
Time 2 Identification
Time 1 Well-being --
Time 2 Well-being .85 ** --
Notes: * indicates p <.05, ** indicates p <.01.