Stressors, coping, and coping effectiveness among players from the England under-18 rugby union team.
Nicholls, Adam R. ; Polman, Remco C.J.
International adolescent sport has the potential to be an extremely
stressful experience and these athletes have reported a variety of
stressors including concerns about errors, outcome, opponents, and the
weather (Nicholls, 2005a; Nicholls, Holt, & Polman, 2005; Nicholls,
Holt, Polman, & James, 2005). The failure to cope with stress can
result in a variety of negative consequences such as decreased
performance (Lazarus, 2000), injury (Smith, Ptacek, & Smell, 1992),
decreased satisfaction (Scanian & Lewthwaite, 1984), and sport
withdrawal (Kolt, Kirby, & Lindner, 1995). It is widely accepted
among sport psychology researchers that athletes must learn to cope with
stressors to reduce these undesirable consequences. Of particular
relevance to the current sample, who were aiming for professional
contracts, is the notion that adolescent athletes must learn to cope
with the stressors they experience to pursue a career in professional
sport (Holt and Dunn, 2004a).
Coping has been defined as "constantly changing cognitive and
behavioral efforts to manage specific external and/or internal demands
that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources of the
person" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 141). Lazarus (1999)
categorized coping strategies into two broad 'higher-order'
functions; problem- and emotion-focused coping. Problem-focused coping
describes strategies directed at managing the environment (e.g., problem
solving, goal setting, and increasing efforts). Emotion-focused coping
involves managing emotional responses to stress (e.g., relaxation,
acceptance, and wishful thinking).
A third coping function, avoidance describes both behavioral (e.g.,
removing self from the situation) and psychological (e.g., cognitive
distancing) attempts to disengage from a stressful situation (Anshel,
1996; Krohne, 1993). Categorizing coping into these three dimensional
classifications of coping functions for youth sport athletes has been
supported by Kowalski and Crocker (2001).
Relatively little is known about the nature of coping during
adolescence (Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Harding Thomsen, &
Wadsworth, 2001) and only a few studies have examined the coping
attempts of adolescent athletes (Kowalski & Crocker, 2001). The
majority of sport coping research has focused on adult athletes
(Nicholls & Polman, 2007). This is somewhat surprising as
adolescence is thought to be a particularly important period implicated in the acquisition of a coping repertoire (Seiffe-Krenke, 1995).
Adolescence is also considered the time when athletes can become experts
in their chosen sport, and when sport can start to dominate their lives
(Bloom, 1985; Cote & Hay, 2002).
The studies that have examined the coping responses of adolescent
athletes have provided some insight into the coping experiences of this
population. However, these studies are limited in their generalizability
because they have been exclusively conducted on athletes performing in
individual sporting activities. Crocker and Isaak (1997), with a sample
of adolescent swimmers, found that coping in practice was consistent,
but in competition the coping responses varied. Gaudreau, Lapierre, and
Blondin (2001) examined pro-competitive, during competition, and
post-competitive coping responses among adolescent golfers. Their
findings suggested that the golfers' coping responses changed
across all three phases of competition. In a follow up study, Gaudreau,
Blondin, and Lapierre (2002) found that golfers who did not achieve
their performance goal for the round decreased task-orientated coping
(e.g., strategies that are used to change or master aspects of a
situation), emotion, and avoidance coping from pro- to post-competition.
These three studies found active coping, increased effort, and positive
reappraisal to be the most commonly used coping strategies.
More recently, Nicholls, Holt, and Polman (2005) examined
stressors, effective, and ineffective coping experiences among a sample
of international Irish adolescent golfers. Results revealed that the
main stressors were outcome, mistakes, score, evaluation, and opponents.
Strategies associated with effective coping experiences were
rationalizing, re-appraising, blocking, positive self-talk, following a
routine, breathing exercises, physical relaxation, and seeking on-course
social support. Alternatively, different types of coping responses such
as trying too hard, speeding up, routine changes, and a lack of coping
were associated with ineffective coping experiences. A limitation of the
Nicholls, Holt, and Polman (2005) paper, and other studies on stress and
coping in sport, was that the data were collected retrospectively (Crocker, Kowalski, & Graham, 1998; Nicholls & Polman, 2007).
With the passage of time people make errors in recalling stress and
coping experiences (e.g., Smith, Leffingwell, & Ptacek, 1999).
To address these limitations Nicholls, Holt, Polman, and James
(2005) examined the stressors and coping strategies utilized by a sample
of Welsh international adolescent golfers during a 31-day diary study.
The four most-frequently reported stressors in this study were making a
physical error, making a mental error, observing an opponent play well,
and difficult weather conditions. Strategies that were classified as
serving a problem-focused coping function were cited more frequently
than those serving emotion-focused or avoidance coping functions. The
highest frequency of coping strategies coincided with the days when the
most stressors were reported. A limitation of the Nicholls, Holt,
Polman, and James paper is that it failed to address coping
effectiveness. Folkman and Moskowitz (2004) suggested that the
underlying motivation for studying coping is belief that some forms of
coping will be more effective than others, and that this information
would help guide coping interventions to teach people to cope with
stress more efficiently.
A weakness of the coping research to date is that little is known
about the notion of coping effectiveness in a sport setting (Crocker et
al., 1998; Nicholls, Holt, & Polman, 2005; Nicholls & Polman,
2007; Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1998). Somerfield and McCrae (2000)
suggested that this is one of the most difficult issues facing coping
researchers. This is because of the complexity of the measurement issues
associated with coping effectiveness, such as the distinction between
short- and long-term coping effectiveness (Folkman & Moskowitz,
2004). Although coping effectiveness is a challenge to measure, advances
in our understanding of coping effectiveness have the potential to make
significant differences in applied settings.
Although no coping studies solely on adolescents participating in
team sports are currently available, there is a small body of research
which has assessed team sport adult athletes. Holt and Hogg (2002)
examined the stress and coping experiences among a sample of
international female soccer players participating at the 1999 world cup.
Stress sources were classified as coaches, demands of international
soccer, competitive stressors, and distractions. To cope with these
stressors the participants reported strategies within problem-focused,
emotion-focused, appraisal-reappraisal (e.g., restructuring perceptions
of stress), and avoidance coping dimensions. Holt and Dunn (2004b)
examined stress and coping longitudinally among four high-level female
soccer players. The players experienced a variety of stressors including
concerns about team performance, selection, team-mates, and fitness. The
coping strategies used by the participants included avoidance,
additional training, thought control strategies, and seeking social
support. Additionally, Park (2000) examined the coping responses among
180 Korean athletes participating in 41 different sports. Park concluded
that team sport athletes require a greater variety coping strategies
than either individual or dual-port athletes, perhaps due to the
different stressors they experience. At the present time little is known
about the stress and coping responses used by male team sport athletes
or indeed adolescent team sport athletes.
The aim of this paper was to address the existing gaps in the sport
psychology literature, and build upon the Nicholls, Holt, Polman, and
James (2005) paper. The first purpose of this study was to examine the
stressors experienced by international adolescent rugby union players
during the 31-day study. The second purpose was to examine the coping
strategies used by the participants to manage these stressors. The third
purpose of this paper was to examine the perceived coping effectiveness
of the coping strategies deployed by the rugby union players.
Method
Participants
Participants were 11 international rugby union players (Mage = 17.9
years, SD = 0.3) who were affiliated with the England Rugby Football
Union talent identification program and were members of the England
under-18 team. The competitive playing experience of the sample ranged
between 7 to 12 years (M playing experience = 9.7 years, SD = 2.1). All
participants attended high school. Participants were recruited by letter
and written informed consent was provided by all participants and a
parent (where the participant was under 18 years old).
The 31-day Competitive Period
Data were collected between February 2005 to March 2005. During the
31-day study period the players attended two, two day training camps
with the England under-18 squad. The participants played in three
competitive games for the England under-18 side against Loughborough
University students, Sale Academy, and an international match versus
Scotland. The participants did not play any club games during the 31
days of the study. During days 1-5 there was no competitive match, but a
two day training camp for all of the players in preparation for the
first match. The first match of the study took place during days 6-10
and was against Loughborough University students. During days 11-15 and
16-20 there were no competitive matches, but a training camp during days
16-20. During days 21-25 there was a match against Sale Academy, with
the final match taking place during days 26-31 against the Scotland
under-18 team. It should be noted that scouts from professional rugby
union clubs attended the training camps and matches. This month was
critical to the rugby careers of the participants and represented a
period of transition where they would either join a club as a
professional player or attend university. As such, the participants were
trying to impress the scouts and secure professional contracts during
the month of the study. In addition to the players' international
commitments, all of the players were affiliated to professional clubs
and so took part in two training sessions per week with their club
during the 31-day diary study.
Data Collection
Daily diary. The daily diary used followed the format of Nicholls,
Holt, Polman, and James (2005), except a Likert-type scale was added to
measure coping effectiveness (Kim & Duda, 2003). The diary comprised
of three discrete sections: (a) stressor checklist and open-nded stress
boxes, (b) open-ended coping responses section, and (c) perceived coping
effectiveness Likert-type scale. The stressor checklist was adapted from
the checklist used by Anshel (1996), who reported a goodness-of-fit
index of .87, with alphas for each stressor ranging from .81 to .92. The
checklist included the following categories: making a physical or mental
error; being criticized by coach; observing an opponent cheat;
sustaining an injury; receiving a wrong call from an official; observing
an opponent perform well; difficult weather conditions; and being
distracted by the crowd or someone watching. In addition to the nine
checklist stressor categories there were two additional blank boxes
where the players could report any stressors they experienced that were
not on the list. The open-ended coping response section required the
participants to write what they did to manage each of the stressors they
had marked on the checklist, or in the open-ended stressor boxes. The
third section of the diary sheet required the participants to rate how
effective each of their coping responses were in managing the reported
stressors on a 5-point Likert-type scale, with I being not effective and
5 being very effective coping (Kim & Duda).
All participants received a package of 31 diary sheets (dated
February 11th 2005 through March 13th 2005), instructions, and one
example of a completed diary sheet. They were asked to complete the
appropriately dated diary sheet on the evening of each day they played
rugby (either competitively or in practice). They were not required to
complete the diary on days when they did not play (i.e., rest days or
travel days). All participants were telephoned by the first author two
days before the study commenced, on the evening of the first day of the
study, and every five days hence to answer any questions. Originally, 30
rugby players consented to participate in the study and were sent diary
packages, but fully completed diaries where data reported for every day
of rugby played in either competition or practice were returned by I 1
participants.
Data Analysis
Stressors. Data from the checklists were tallied to identify the
five most-frequently reported stressors. Data were analyzed longitudinally to illustrate longitudinal patterns within in the data,
and thus provide a more accurate insight into the stress process (e.g.,
Nicholls, Holt, Polman, & James, 2005; Udry, 1997), by creating five
time periods of five days, and a period with six days (days 1-5, 6-10,
11-15, 16-20, 21-25, 26-31). The five most-frequently reported stressors
in each time period were then tallied and divided by 11 to produce mean
scores and standard deviations were calculated.
Coping. The open-ended coping responses data were transcribed
verbatim and subjected to an inductive (Maykut & Morehouse, 1994)
and deductive (Patton, 2002) content analysis procedure. The first phase
of the data analysis was inductive. Similar coping strategies were
grouped together as first-order themes and assigned a descriptive label.
A rule of inclusion was written for each theme. Similar first-order
themes were grouped under more abstract labels as second-order themes
(e.g., 'increased effort' was assigned to the second-order
theme of 'Behavior Technique Coping' whereas
'visualization' was assigned to the second-order theme of
'Relaxation').
The second phase of the data analysis involved a deductive content
analysis procedure which served to verify the authenticity and
appropriateness of the second-order themes (Patton, 2003). This involved
a discussion between the first and second author. Following the
modification of the coding scheme there was 99.3% agreement.
Second-order themes were then classified according to the coping
function that they were apparently intended to serve using the
dimensions of problem-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidance coping
that have been recommended in the literature (Kowalski & Crocker,
2001). To produce mean scores for each time period the number of coping
first-order themes in the three coping functions were tallied across the
sample during each time period and then divided by the number of
participants in the study (11) and standard deviations were calculated.
To produce longitudinal mean coping function scores reported by
each participant for the six time periods, the number of coping
first-order themes in the three coping functions were tallied across the
sample during each time period and then divided by the number of
participants in the study (11). Standard deviations were calculated
across the participants for each specific time period.
Coping effectiveness. The final analytic technique was designed to
provide an indication of the effectiveness of coping strategies deployed
to manage the most frequently-cited stressors. The number of times each
coping theme was used to manage making a physical error, coach/parent
criticism, making a mental error, sustaining an injury, and observing an
opponent play well were tallied for the sample over 31-days. The coping
effectiveness of the strategy in relation to each specific stressor was
tallied and then divided by the frequency of coping themes reported for
managing the particular stressor. This process generated a mean coping
effectiveness score for each coping strategy in relation to the stressor
it was used to manage. For instance, increasing concentration to manage
physical error stressors was reported on 17 occasions and had a mean
coping effectiveness of 3.8. To establish coping effectiveness
longitudinally, the perceived coping effectiveness rating of each coping
strategy used by all of the participants were tallied and divided by the
number of deployed coping strategies during each period. This process
generated a mean effectiveness of all the coping strategies across all
of the participants in each of the six time periods.
Results
Participants reported that they played rugby on 171 of the 341
total available days. On average, each participant played rugby on 15.4
days (SD = 4.4). From the 171 daily diaries received, participants
reported 328 stressors and 410 coping responses.
Stressors. The five most reported stressors were physical error
(92), criticism from coach or parent (47), mental error (39), sustaining
an injury (32), and observing an opponent perform well (31). The five
most cited stressors comprised of 73% of all the reported stressors
(Table 1). Figure 1 indicates that the mean number of stressors remained
consistent, apart from days 1115 and 16-20 where the participants
reported fewer stressors.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Coping. The most reported first order theme was blocking (63),
increased effort (52), increased concentration (38), and took advice
(22). Of the three coping dimensions, problem-focused coping strategies
were used 270 times, this accounted for 66% of all reported coping
strategies. Figure 2 shows that problem-focused coping strategies were
used the most in each time period. Emotion-focused coping strategies
were used 72 times, 17.5% of total reported coping strategies. Finally
avoidance coping strategies were used 68 times, yielding 16.5% of total
reported coping strategies (Table 2).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Coping effectiveness. Figure 2 shows that mean coping effectiveness
started at 3.66 during days 1-5, and steadily decreased during days
6-10, 11-15, 16-20, and 21-25. Mean coping effectiveness rapidly
increased during days 26-31 to its highest value of 3.91. Overall, the
participants had a perceived mean effectiveness rating of 3.29. When
looking at the mean-effectiveness of the coping strategies reported, the
coping effectiveness scores ranged from 1-5 (see Table 3). The most
frequently-cited coping strategies were not rated as being the most
effective responses. For example, to cope with making a physical error
the most frequently-reported coping strategy was blocking (20), which
was rated with a mean perceived effectiveness of 3.8. However, the most
effective coping strategy for managing a physical error was going
through a routine (cited 4 times, M effectiveness = 4.8) followed by
practicing (cited 11 times, M effectiveness = 4.6).
Discussion
This paper examined stressors, coping strategies, and the perceived
coping effectiveness of these coping strategies among a sample of
international adolescent rugby union players. Similar to previous
research, the findings revealed that a small number of stressors recur
over time. The five most reported stressors (e.g., making a physical
error, criticism from coach/parent, making a mental error, injury, and
observing an opponent play well) accounted for 73% of all stressors.
This longitudinal finding is similar to Nicholls, Holt, Polman, and
James (2005) who found that the four most reported stressors for golfers
comprised of 75% of the whole sample. These results suggest that on the
whole, the players' coping attempts were directed towards managing
a limited number of stressor.
Making a physical error stressor was cited the most during each
period of the study. The other stressors fluctuated during the month of
the study, but receiving criticism from a coach or parent was the second
most reported stressor during five of the six time periods.
Coaches' communication was a source of stress among the soccer
players in the Holt and Hogg (2002) paper. The number of players citing
this stressor was not reported. Coach related stressors did not feature
prominently in the study by Nicholls, Holt, Polman, and James (2005), or
other research (e.g., Gould, Eklund, & Jackson, 1993). Perhaps this
stressor may be experienced more by athletes participating in team
sports due to the different roles of coaches in team and individual
sports. Another stressor that featured prominently in this study, but
not the Nicholls, Holt, Polman, and James study was injury. This is
because rugby is a much more physically demanding sport than golf, where
players are at constant risk from a variety of injuries (e.g., Garraway,
Lee, Hutton, Russell, & McLeod, 2000). From an applied perspective,
sport psychology consultants should take into account the nature of
sport in which their client competes. Applied consultants should also
consider the dynamic relationship between the coaches/ parents and the
athlete, especially when working with adolescent athletes to prevent
this stressor from inhibiting performance.
It is also interesting to note that the participants reported more
stressors during the periods of days when matches were played. The
number of stressors declined during days 11-15 and 16-20 which is when
the players did not have a match to play. Although there were no matches
between days 1-5, the level of stressors reported were still high. This
may have been due to the fact that the squad was coming together in
preparation for a crucial part of the players' rugby career which
could determine team selection for the England under-18 team and
subsequently whether they would be offered a professional club contract.
The rugby players reported a vast number of coping strategies
during the month of the study (see Table 2). Blocking was the most
frequently reported coping strategy used by the sample, and included
attempts by the participants to shut out thoughts, or mentally withdraw
from stress they were experiencing. This timing is congruent with other
adolescent sport coping research with golfers (e.g., Nicholls 2005a;
Nicholls, Holt, & Polman, 2005; Nicholls, Holt, Polman, & James,
2005). Although this type of coping strategy may be effective in the
short term, researchers have issued caution in athletes using blocking
for long-term stressors (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1998; Pensgaard &
Duda, 2003) as it can have many undesirable effects, which could include
emotional, behavioral, and social problems (Ayers, Sandler, West, &
Roosa, 1996; Compas, Malcarne, & Fondacaro, 1998; Compas et al.,
2001).
Coping strategies that were classified within the problem-focused
dimension were the most frequently cited compared to strategies within
either the emotion-focused or avoidance classifications in congruence with previous research (e.g., Crocker & Isaak, 1997; Gaudreau et
al., 2001; 2002; Nicholls, Holt, Polman, & James, 2005). More
problem-focused coping strategies were reported when the rugby players
reported more physical error stressors.
The mean coping effectiveness of the strategies deployed by the
participants fluctuated during the study. Coping effectiveness appeared
to decline from days 1-5 to days 21-25. It may have been due to the
number of coping strategies deployed by the participants. The general
decrease in coping effectiveness coincided with the deployment of fewer
coping strategies. During days 26-31 there was an increase in both
coping effectiveness and the mean number of deployed coping strategies.
This may indicate that coping effectiveness could be related to the
number of coping strategies deployed, although further research is
required to support this hypothesis. There were also considerable
fluctuations in the mean effectiveness of individual coping strategies.
This finding supports the notion that coping effectiveness is related to
the choice of the coping strategy (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995; Crocker
et al., 1998), suggesting that adolescent rugby players should be
encouraged to engage in adaptive coping strategies and refrain from
using the maladaptive coping strategies identified in this study.
A weakness of this study is that perceptions of control of the
stressors were not examined. Although evidence was found to support the
notion that coping effectiveness is related to choice (c.f., Bolger
& Zuckerman, 1995) other models of coping effectiveness, such as the
goodness-of-fit approach and the automacity theory could not be examined
within this study. This should be addressed in the future, and will
provide researchers with a more comprehensive understanding of coping
effectiveness in a sport setting (Nicholls & Polman, 2007).
Every effort was made to instruct the participants to rate the
effectiveness of each coping strategy by including three Likert-type
coping effectiveness scales in the diary. However, the participants
tended to rate the overall coping effectiveness of the coping strategies
they used, and used only one of Likert-type scales. This is why it was
not possible to provide a mean coping effectiveness scale for problem-,
emotion-focused, and avoidance coping.
A general limitation of daily approaches to collect stress and
coping data is that participants only report concrete and discrete
events and fail to report ongoing and complex problems (Folkman &
Moskowitz, 2004) which could have included concerns about future
careers. A further issue with collecting data longitudinally is the drop
out rate among participants. Shaughnessy and Zechmeister (1994) reported
that this one of the obstacles of collecting longitudinal research.
Originally 30 participants agreed to take part in the study but complete
diaries were only returned by 11 participants. Although the authors were
in contact with the rugby players, future research should develop and
implement additional techniques to try and increase the number of
returned diary studies. One way to do this could be to have face to face
meetings with players, or have electronic daily diaries which are
electronically mailed back to the researcher on a daily basis
immediately after completion.
From an applied perspective this paper has provided a unique
insight into the demands placed on international adolescent rugby union
players. Although this group experience a wide range of stressors there
are five stressors which international adolescent rugby union players
experience much more regularly such as making a physical error,
receiving criticism from their coach/parents, making a mental error,
injury, and observing an opponent play well. As such, applied
practitioners should spend more time teaching adolescent rugby players
to manage these stressors.
This paper has found evidence to suggest which coping strategies
are likely to be the most effective strategies in managing these salient
stressors. For instance when rugby union players experience the stress
of making a physical error they should go through their routine,
practice, and re-focus on the task at hand. After experiencing the
stress of making a mental error, rugby union players should think
through their movement and visualize the intended action without an
error. After receiving criticism from their coach, rugby union players
should listen and learn from their mistakes, and increase their
concentration. With regards to opponent playing well stressors, rugby
union players should increase their concentration. These findings
provide a basis for the creation of coping interventions to enable
international adolescent rugby union players to cope more effectively
with stressors, but coping intervention studies are required (c.f.,
Nicholls, 2005b) to empirically examine any recommendations.
References
Anshel, M. H. (1996). Coping styles among adolescent competitive
athletes. Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 311-323.
Ayers, T. S., Sandler, I. N., West, S. G., & Roosa, M. W.
(1996). A dispositional and situational assessment of children's
coping: Testing alternative models of coping. Journal of Personality 64,
923-958.
Bloom, B.S. (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York:
Ballantine.
Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying
personality in the stress process. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69, 890-902.
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Harding Thomsen,
A., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood
and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and
research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87-127.
Compas, B. E., Malcarne, V. L., & Fondacaro, K. M. (1988).
Coping with stressful events in older children and young adolescents.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 405-411.
Cote, J., & Hay, J. (2002). Children's involvement in
sport: A developmental perspective. In J. M. Silva and D. E. Stevens
(Eds.) Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 484-502). Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
Crocker, P. R. E., & Isaak, K. (1997). Coping during
competitions and training sessions: Are youth swimmers consistent?
International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 355-369.
Crocker, P. R. E., Kowalski, K. C., & Graham, T. R. (1998).
Measurement of coping strategies in sport. In J. L. Duda (Ed.) Advances
in sport and exercise psychology measurement edited by (pp. 149-161).
Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: Pitfalls and
promise. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 745-774. Garraway, W. M. Lee,
A. J., Hutton, S. J., Russell, E. B.A. W., & McLeod, D. A. D.
(2000). Impact of professionalism on injuries in rugby. British Journal
of Sports Medicine, 34, 348-351.
Gaudreau, P., Blondin, J. P., & Lapierre, A. M. (2002).
Athletes' coping during a competition: Relationship of coping
strategies with positive affect, negative affect, and performance-goal
discrepancy. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3, 125-150.
Gaudreau, P., Lapierre, A. M., & Blondin, J. P. (2001). Coping
at three phases of competition: Comparison between pre-competitive,
competitive, and post-competitive utilization of the same strategy.
International Journal of Sport Psychology, 32, 369-385.
Gould, D., Eklund, R. C., & Jackson, S. A. (1993). Coping
strategies used by US Olympic wrestlers. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 64, 83-93.
Holt, N. L., & Dunn, J.G. H. (2004a). Grounded theory of the
psychosocial competencies and environmental conditions associated with
soccer success. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 199-219.
Holt, N. L., & Dunn, J. G. H. (2004b). Longitudinal idiographic analyses of appraisal and coping responses in sport. Psychology of Sport
and Exercise, 5, 213-222.
Holt, N. L., & Hogg, J. M. (2002). Perceptions of stress and
coping during preparations for the 1999 women's soccer world cup
finals. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 251-271.
Kim, M. S., and Duda, J. L. (2003). The coping process: Cognitive
appraisals of stress, coping strategies, and coping effectiveness. The
Sport Psychologist, 17, 406-425.
Kolt, G. S., Kirkby, R. J., & Lindner, H. (1995). Coping
processes in competitive gymnasts: Gender differences. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 81, 1139-1145.
Kowalski, K. C., & Crocker, P. R. E. (2001). The development
and validation of the Coping Function Questionnaire for adolescents in
sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23, 136-155.
Krohne, H. W. (1993). Vigilance and cognitive avoidance as concepts
in coping research. In Attention and avoidance: Strategies in coping
with aversiveness. Edited by H. W. Krohne (pp. 19-50). Seattle, WA:
Hogrefe and Huber.
Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New
York: Springer.
Lazarus, R. S. (2000). How emotions influence performance in
competitive sports. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 229-252.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and
coping. New York: Springer.
Maykut, P., & Morehouse, R. (1994). Beginning qualitative
research: A philosophic and practical guide. Philadelphia: Falmer Press.
Nicholls, A. R. (2005a). Longitudinal analyses of stress, coping,
and coping effectiveness among Scottish international adolescent golfers
during a 28-day diary study. Journal of Sport Sciences, 23, 1265-1266.
Nicholls, A. R. (2005b). Can athletes be taught to cope more
effectively? A longitudinal analysis of a coping case study with an
international adolescent golfer. Journal of Sport Sciences, 23,
1263-1264.
Nicholls, A. R., Holt, N. L., & Polman, R. C. J. (2005). A
Phenomenological Analysis of Coping Effectiveness in Golf. The Sport
Psychologist, 19, 111-130.
Nicholls, A. R., Holt, N. L., Polman, R. C. J., & James, D. W.
G (2005). Stress and coping among international adolescent golfers.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 333-340.
Nicholls, A. R., & Polman, R. C. J. (2007). Coping in sport: A
systematic review. Journal of Sport Sciences, 25, 11-31.
Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1998). The relationship of
coping and its perceived effectiveness to positive and negative affect
in sport. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 773-778.
Park, J-K. (2000). Coping strategies used by Korean national
athletes. The Sport-Psychologist, 14, 63-80.
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pensgaard, A. M., & Duda, J. L. (2003). Sydney 2000: The
interplay between emotions, coping, and the performance of Olympic-level
athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 17, 253-267.
Scanlan T. K., & Lewthwaite, R. (1984). Social psychological
aspects of competition for male youth sport participants: Predictors of
competitive stress. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 208-226.
Seiffe-Krenke, I. (1995). Stress, coping, and relationships in
adolescence. Mahwah, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Shauglmessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1994). Research
methods in psychology (3rd ed). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Smith, R. E., Leffingwell, T. R., & Ptacek, J. T. (1999). Can
people remember how they coped? Factors associated with discordance between same-day and retrospective reports. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 76, 1050-1061.
Smith, R. E., Ptacek, J. T., & Smoll, F. L. (1992). Sensation
seeking, stress, and adolescent injuries: A test of stress-buffering,
risk-taking, and coping skills hypotheses. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 62, 1016-1024.
Udry, E. (1997). Coping and social support among injured athletes
following surgery. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 71-90.
Adam R. Nicholls
Leeds Metropolitan University
Remco C. J. Polman
The University of Hull
Address Correspondence To: Adam R Nicholls, Carnegie Research
Institute, Fairfax Hall, Headingley Campus, Leeds, LS6 3QS, UK. E-mail
[email protected]
Table 1
Total Stressor Frequencies for 31-Day Period
Stressor Frequency
Making a physical error 92
Suffering criticism from a coach/parent 47
Making a mental error 39
Sustaining an injury 32
Observing an opponent perform better than themselves 31
Playing badly due to poor weather/course conditions 22
Being distracted by spectators 17
Receiving a wrong call from an official 12
Observing an opponent cheat 7
Not being selected 7
Tiredness 4
Nerves at the start of the match 2
Given responsibility within team 2
Lack of time during the match 2
Didn't want to let team-mates down 2
Having to take control 1
Improve skill level 1
Singing national anthem 1
Being played out of position 1
Kicking practice 1
Impress in first game 1
Observing video of themselves 1
Gym session 1
Low motivation 1
Had wrong kit 1
Table 2. Classification and Frequencies of Coping Responses Over 31
Days
Coping Function Second order First order theme
theme
Problem-focused Technique Changed technique (17)
orientated Practiced (12)
coping
Cognitive Thought movement through (5)
technique
coping
Behavioral Increased effort (52)
coping Reduced injury symptoms (15)
Adapted game (7)
Tried to outplay opponent (6)
Went through routine (5)
Tried to prove self (5)
Improved performance (3)
Played for team (1)
Slapped thighs (1)
Carried on same way (1)
Took control (1)
Made quicker decisions (1)
Planning Changed tactics (2)
Rescheduled (1)
Communication Praised opponent (8)
Argued (3)
Confronted opponent (2)
Defended self from criticism (1)
Offered vocal support (1)
Concentration Increased concentration (38)
Refocused on task (19)
Increased self-focus (13)
Focused on opponent (4)
Goal setting Reminded self of goals (1)
Cognitive Self-talk(15)
coping Dealt with stressor (9)
Stayed positive (4)
Put things into perspective (4)
Motivated self (3)
Got annoyed (2)
Paid too much attention (2)
Learned from mistakes (2)
Imagined people watching (1)
Counted in head (1)
Stuck to plan (1)
Emotion-focused Relaxation Visualization (19)
Physical relaxation (6)
Breathing exercises (2)
Acceptance Positive appraisal (4)
Accepted others (3)
Accepted situation (2)
Reassured self (1)
Social Took advice (22)
Informed significant other (5)
Listened (4)
Sought advice (1)
Did not take advice (1)
Blame Blamed the weather
Got angry (1)
Avoidance Cognitive Blocking (63)
avoidance Dwelled on stressor (3)
Did not let it worry me (2)
Laughed (1)
Left it (1)
Table 3. Coping Strategies, Frequencies, and Mean Effectiveness in
Managing the Three Most frequently Cited Stressors
Mean
Stressor Coping strategy Frequency effectiveness
Physical Blocking 20 3.8
error Increased concentration 17 3.3
Re-focused on task 16 3.8
Visualization 12 3.4
Technical adjustments 12 3.9
Practised 11 4.6
Increased effort 10 3.6
Self-talk 7 3.3
Went through routine 4 4.8
Physical relaxation 3 4
Took advice 2 4.5
Thought movement through 1 4
Vocal support 1 4.5
Laughed 1 4
Dwelled on stressor 1 4
Slapped thighs 1 2
Left it 1 1
Blamed weather 1 4
Dealt with it 1 3
Stayed positive 1 4
Coach/
Parent Took advice 14 4
criticism Increased effort 7 3.1
Blocking 6 2.7
Proved self 4 3.8
Listened 3 4.7
Increased concentration 2 4.0
Argued 2 4.5
Perspective 2 3
Visualization 2 3
Motivated self 2 2.5
Informed significant 1 5
others
Reassured self 1 4
Carried on same way 1 4
Technical adjustments 1 4
Self-talk 1 4
Refocused on task 1 4
Increased self-focus 1 4
Didn't take advice 1 1
Mental Increased effort 8 3.8
error Took advice 6 3.5
Blocking 6 2
Increased concentration 5 3.4
Thought through movement 4 4.5
Visualization 4 4.3
Self-talk 3 3.7
Learned from mistake 1 5
Breathing exercise 1 4
Reassured self 1 4
Focused on task 1 4
Dealt with it 1 4
Informed significant 1 4
others
Technical adjustments 1 4
Physical relaxation 1 4
Counted in head 1 4
Stayed positive 1 3
Dwelled on stressor 1 2