Coping with unexpected loss of water supply among regular exercisers.
Devonport, Tracey J. ; Lane, Andrew M. ; Crone, Diane 等
On the 20th July 2007 severe flooding hit Gloucestershire, UK,
affecting many houses and businesses, including the local water
treatment works. For some individuals, the floods posed a natural
disaster in that homes were flooded, and unfortunately, there were
fatalities resulting directly from the floods (cited by BBC news,
http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/in_depth/629/629/6911778.stm. Accessed 18th
June 2009). For the vast majority, the floods posed an inconvenience,
and most notably water supply was unavailable to many areas for up to
two weeks after the water treatment works were flooded. When exploring
literature concerning natural disasters it becomes apparent that with
few exceptions (Reacher, McKenzie, Lane, et al., 2004), published
literature has primarily focused on individuals whose homes are damaged
by the elements (Beggs, Haines, & Hulbery, 1996; Blaikie, Cannon,
Davis, & Wisner, 1994; Cutter, Boruff, & Shirley, 2003;
Tunstall, Tapsell, Green, et al., 2006). However, high dependency on
natural resources such as water can leave individuals vulnerable to
changes in resource conditions. As such exploring the experiences of
individuals that have endured water loss presents a worthy line of
investigation.
Within the natural disaster literature, vulnerability has been
defined as 'the characteristics of a person or group in terms of
their capacity to anticipate, cope with, resist, and recover from the
impact of a natural hazard' (Blaikie et al., 1994, p. 9). This
definition takes into account the significance of behaviors during the
different phases of a natural disaster. This concurs with theoretical
proposals that the relevance and effectiveness of a particular coping
response varies with the phase of a stressful transaction (Lazarus &
Folkman, 1984; Lazarus, 1990, 2000).
Coping is regarded a complex, dynamic stress process that involves
the person, the environment, and the evolving relationship between them
(Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004). Coping efforts are initiated in
response to an appraisal that personally meaningful goals have been
harmed/lost (damage has already occurred), threatened (anticipated
future harm or loss), and/or challenged (a potential opportunity for
personal growth/gain) (Lazarus, 2001). As a stressful encounter
progresses, there is a continuous interaction between appraisals, coping
and emotional responses, each changing as the transaction unfolds
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
There was a clear rationale for selecting regular exercisers as the
focus of the present study. Previous research has demonstrated that
people utilize exercise as a self--regulatory behavior offering positive
outcomes for health and well--being. For example, it has been found that
exercise associates with an increases in pleasant affective states and
reduction in unpleasant ones (Berger & Motl, 2000; Biddle, 2000;
Scully, Kremer, Meade, et al., 1998). Exercise is also associated with
an increase in positive self--perceptions of body image (Hausenblas
& Fallon, 2006). Further, exercise appears to be an effective
strategy to help cope with daily hassles (Rostad & Long, 1996).
Given the perceptual nature of stress and coping, when placed under
stressful conditions, individuals might identify a number of barriers
that they believe prohibit exercise. Such barriers might exacerbate
stress symptoms by disrupting the use of exercise as a preferred
self--regulatory behavior. For example, one consequence of natural
disasters may be to affect the exercise strategies people use to
regulate their mood (Thayer, Newman, & McClain, 1994). Flooding and
water loss might affect the type of exercise available with activities
such as running and cycling being restricted. This is significant, as
individuals appear to accrue greater benefits when they can choose
activities by preference (Lane, Jackson, & Terry, 2005). Therefore,
an individual might not anticipate experiencing pleasant affective
states following exercise when they are unable to pursue their preferred
mode of exercise.
The present study should be set in the context of examining how
exercise might act as a coping strategy to assist normal functioning and
the extent to which sudden and unexpected loss of water supply presents
a series of challenges when seeking to maintain exercise (e.g.,
accessing sports facilities and washing post exercise). Findings from
the study have practical implications for sport and exercise
practitioners whose goals include enhancing enjoyment, performance and
well--being benefits associated with exercise. We suggest that given the
role of exercise as a self--regulatory strategy, there is a need to
consider the potential impact of unexpected stressors and develop
methods through which coping could be supported. In doing so, it might
be possible to enhance an individuals' ability to maintain their
exercise regimen and associated benefits. The present paper focuses on
unexpected water loss, but it should be noted that this represents an
example intended to prompt practitioners to consider the requirements
for successful adaptation to any unexpected stressors.
The present study used qualitative methods to explore personal
accounts of loss of water supply among regular exercisers. The aim was
to explore the impact of water loss on exercise and identify coping
strategies intended to manage the situation and consequences.
Qualitative methods were used as these facilitate an exploration of
coping with this unique situation.
Method
Research context
Approval for this study was attained from the first and second
authors University ethics committee. Participants signed informed
consent forms for participation prior to undertaking an interview, and
no incentives for participation were offered.
Participants
Nine volunteer participants (four male, five female; age: M = 39.10
years, SD = 10.89) were recruited via convenience sampling. Participants
were approached at sports facilities within the region affected by water
loss. Upon offering consent to take part in interviews inclusion
criteria were then utilized to attain individuals exercising at least
three times per week at moderate to vigorous intensity, and residing
within the affected areas over the duration of water loss. These
criteria were applied to ensure that all participants were habitual
exercisers and experienced water loss. The participants exercise
routines ranged from a minimum of one--hour three times per week to
daily exercise for 60 to 90 minutes.
Pseudonyms are used to maintain confidentiality and protect the
identity of all participants. Methods advocated by Creswell (1998, 2009)
were used to ensure trustworthiness including peer review and debriefing
wherein the authors coded the interviews both individually and during
group meetings. Emergent themes were subject to ongoing discussions
until such time that consensus was attained. Member checking occurred on
two occasions, firstly when producing interview transcripts and second,
when reviewing a final draft of the article intended for submission.
This was in order to ensure an accurate portrayal of participants'
perceptions. The participants and their experiences are described in
detail, this is done to add depth and context to the data and ultimately
enhance trustworthiness.
Interview procedure
Participants completed semi--structured interviews within a week of
the reintroduction of water supply to facilitate accurate recall. All
participants were provided with information regarding the nature and
purpose of the semi--structured interview. This included the interview
rationale, the use of interview data, issues of confidentiality, topics
to be discussed, and procedures for tape recording and transcribing the
interview. The schedule covered four themes of enquiry including: 1)
consequences of water loss; 2) exercise behaviors during water loss; 3)
coping behaviors intended to manage the situation and resultant
consequences and 4) temporal changes during the water loss encounter.
Open questions such as 'Can you tell me what your experiences have
been when faced with the recent lack of water supply?' and
'Can you describe the ways in which you sought to overcome problems
resulting from a lack of water supply?' were followed by
clarification and elaboration probes. At the end of the interview,
participants were asked whether there was anything else they could tell
the interviewer concerning their experience with the loss of water
supply. During interviews participants referred to the term "gym
use", which in the present study refers more to where the activity
takes place than what was done. Gym use refers to all activities that
can occur in health and fitness centers including weights,
cardiovascular equipment, gym classes (e.g., formal exercise classes
with an instructor) and a swimming pool. The authors collaborated in an
initial review of the data to determine thematic content and to
establish when data saturation had occurred, i.e., no new themes were
emerging within the interview data provided.
Data Analysis
Data preparation and analysis began shortly after each interview
was completed. QSR NVivo--2 (2002) was the software used as a tool to
host data analysis. Using inductive coding, quotes were extracted from
the interviews and categorized into raw themes that represented a
meaningful point.
Results
The perceptions of nine participants regarding the impact of water
loss on exercise behaviors are explored along with resultant
consequences. We acknowledge that interviews are co--constructions, and
as such, where possible we offer extended quotations so that readers
might consider alternative interpretations of the data.
Impact of water loss on exercise behaviors and resultant
consequences
During the six months leading up to the point of water loss, all
participants completed a minimum of three 30--minute sessions of
moderate to vigorous intensity exercise per week. All participants
stated that during the period of water loss, they modified their
exercise regimen.
There were many attributions offered in the narratives of
participants for changes in exercise behaviors following water loss.
These included; a) spending a greater amount of time planning where and
when each exercise session could take place; b) challenges of
maintaining hygiene; c) a sense of increased sensitivity to the
perceptions of other people (a concern that others might judge personal
hygiene and priority for water use as a result of exercising); d) closed
sports facilities; e) health and safety concerns; f) continued poor
weather; g) priorities for the use of water; h) pursuing other
priorities; and, i) curiosity to see flood sites. The personal
consequences of modified exercise regimens ranged from minimal
influences to considerable changes (gains and losses) in psychological
and possibly physiological well--being. The consequences experienced
appeared influenced by an individual's beliefs concerning exercise
behaviors in terms of exercise conditions (e.g., exercise form,
intensity, duration and location) and the benefits of exercise (e.g.,
mood regulation, body image).
When faced with sudden water loss, individuals holding fixed
exercise beliefs (which we explain shortly) were more likely to perceive
a reduction in exercise. As a consequence they were more predisposed to
experiencing increases in unpleasant emotions, concerns regarding weight
gain and loss of tone, and concerns regarding the ability to resume
exercise routines. An example of a fixed beliefs evidenced by
participants was that exercise must involve activities of a sufficient
intensity to elicit sweating. As will be demonstrated in the data
presented, although such individuals might have engaged in alternative
activities, they failed to recognize these as exercise as they were not
sweat--inducing. Consequently, their-perception was that their exercise
regimen had reduced along with the self--regulatory outcomes of
exercise. By contrast, individuals holding flexible exercise beliefs
appeared to pursue and/ or recognize alternative exercise conditions
more readily. These individuals were able to acknowledge the
contribution of alternative exercise regimens toward their
self--regulatory goals thereby maintaining the benefits associated with
exercise.
Individuals presenting evidence of fixed exercise beliefs included
Tony, June, Kate, Mary, and Jenny. With the exception of Jenny, these
individuals reported a reduced exercise regimen and experienced negative
outcomes of water loss. Those individuals evidencing flexible exercise
beliefs included Lucy, Nick, Cody, and Terry. Without exception, they
maintained, albeit modified, their exercise regimen and experienced
positive or minimal consequences following water loss. Findings of the
present study suggest that exercise beliefs influenced behavioral,
cognitive and emotional responses to water loss. The following
exploration by participant offers a deeper insight into these beliefs
and their influence on the impact of water loss on exercise behaviors
and resultant consequences.
Jenny (female)--perceived exercise as increased
Jenny is a 50 year--old female teaching assistant. At the time of
the water loss she was on annual leave from school. When the gym
re--opened she noted it was much quieter than usual. She indicated that
I probably exercised more because the gym was very quiet and there
was nobody here [gym] really so it was quite pleasant because it's
quite hot in there [gym] and it wasn't so warm ... it was something
to do rather than sitting at home and be depressed thinking about
the water, and exercise makes you feel a bit better when you've
done some exercise.
Jenny repeatedly expressed feelings of depression resulting from
the domestic inconvenience of water loss. For her, exercise was a means
of managing these negative mood states.
Cody (male)--perceived exercise as modified but maintained
Cody is a 30--year old male fitness instructor employed by a local
health and fitness centre that closed during the floods. He modified his
exercise habits during the period of water loss as a consequence of
having more free time, safety considerations and to indulge curiosity.
He surmised;
my bike's not particularly safe [a racing bicycle with thin tyres]
and I knew there was a lot of gravel washing up onto the surface,
but I did go out for a jog just to look around town, I actually
went out because of the floods because I wanted to go and see the
spectacle of it all. I went jogging into town and waded through
water to look around.
He felt he was not exercising any less, but pursued exercises that
were not reliant upon the gym. He noted an increase in exercise using
the natural environment, for example, hill walking. He felt that this
break in gym routine was a positive outcome. Cody made reference to
beliefs concerning the role of water in the ritual of exercise noting
I was interrogating someone about why it [water loss] was such a
big deal? He said he just doesn't want to exercise without having a
shower afterwards, its part of society I suppose, all these
facilities we have got access to and we expect them. When we
haven't got them, we have to think more about what we are doing.
People don't necessarily want to do that which I think is good in
that it challenges people but it is bad because people don' t
generally have answers of how to get around it.
Cody felt that a strip wash was sufficient to maintain hygiene
following exercise.
Nick (male)--perceived exercise as modified but maintained
Nick is a 54 year--old retired male, he felt his exercise regimen
was modified not because of the water loss, but due to the persistent
rain that occurred during this time. He noted that he walked and did the
gardening less so he used the rowing machine and bike at home also
noting that "I was coming in here [gym] anyway and probably
spending a bit more time in here than I was before 'cause of the
weather so you know, not a problem, it was good compensation". He
acknowledged these modifications as helping to maintain his exercise
outputs.
Lucy (female)--perceived exercise as modified but maintained
Lucy is a 41 year--old female solicitor on long--term sick leave at
the time of the water loss. She wished to continue her normal exercise
routine explaining that the gym was a safe exercise environment due to
her health condition. She commented that: "I'm recovering from
surgery that went wrong, if something happened physically and I was out
on my own on a hill ... that's my main reason I prefer the
gym". However, with the gym closed, she indicated that
I was just feeling a bit worded that I was going to put on weight
really ... I only usually go out on my bike if I've not been to the
gym or if I feel particularly well ... so it's actually been good
for me to break the routine ... I think maybe the gym is something
that can be integrated into these other activities rather than
instead of.
Lucy felt that the water loss had forced her to pursue alternative
exercises producing positive outcomes.
Terry (male)--perceived exercise as modified but maintained
Terry is a 46 year--old male employed by the air--force. He noted
that his normal exercise routine reduced because "it's just
harder to keep clean afterwards, strip washes instead of showers ...
you're trying to keep clean for longer rather than wearing clothes
and getting them sweaty because you have no washing machine".
However, Terry observed that the floods and water loss presented
alternative ways of exercising,
some of the houses in our street were flooded and everybody pitched
in helping to move sandbags, furniture, providing drinks, food so
they'd got something ... you do it for two or more hours and you
certainly knew about it the next day ... and you feel good about it
as well, you get that high from thinking I've achieved something
for somebody else.
Terry clearly expresses positive consequences accrued from helping
others whilst completing an alternative form of exercise.
Tony (male)--perceived exercise as reduced
Tony is a 46 year--old male whose occupation involved promoting and
supporting the uptake of physical activity. Tony felt his exercise
routine had reduced during the water loss because other priorities had
taken precedence. "Well I haven't run at all ... there was the
issue of not having water from the point of view of going running.... It
wasn't that I couldn't use the shower, it was the
preoccupation [with finding water]". Tony felt that the water loss
provided an alternative physical activity "the lugging the water up
and down, 15 litres of water from there to the car, going back doing it
again, ... carrying this thing is physical activity".
Despite acknowledging the physical demands of transporting water
Tony spoke of routines, particularly for running, and the disruption to
his routine produced psychological consequences "I do feel the buzz
you feel when you do a run so I would say that it [not running] was more
irritating and frustrating". He explained "I'd reached
the point where I was going to start running again before we were told
the water was coming on because you have to sort of adapt". For
Tony, he did not perceive the physically demanding act of carrying water
as meeting his self--regulatory goals. For example, in respect of weight
management he noted that, "I routinely weighed myself so you have
an awareness of where you are in and around your general routine, I mean
I don't count everything as I'm not that kind of person but
within the range you don't want to let yourself go". As the
water loss endured, he became increasingly frustrated by the lack of
perceived exercise and intended to regulate these feelings by exercising
despite the ongoing challenges presented.
June (female)--perceived exercise as reduced
June is a 22 year--old female who worked on the service desk for an
information technology company. She did not complete any of her usual
exercise regimen during the period of water loss because of perceived
difficulties in maintaining cleanliness and personal hygiene following
exercise. As a consequence of reduced exercise, she became more
conscious of energy intake, expenditure and weight. She indicated that:
because I wasn't exercising as much I was sort of thinking,
normally I have my big lunch at work and then I go to they gym and
I feel like I'm burning it off ... I was watching what I eat more
than I normally would ... looking back it's quite funny I didn't
realize it at the time, but normally I drive everywhere, whereas I
was walking more places, even when I could get around in my car
when the floods subsided I was still wanting to walk just because I
hadn't had any exercise that day.
However, June believed that the gym was the exercise forum in which
to bum off calories and as such did not acknowledge the contribution of
walking to her weight management goals. June consequently evidenced body
image concerns
I'm sure I've put weight on (laughs), and I probably have because
even though I was trying to watch what I eat I probably was eating
the same as I normally do ... my bodies used to having exercise and
stuff and I think when you're used to doing exercise when you don't
have it for a week you feel a bit sluggish as well.
Kate (female)--perceived exercise as reduced
Kate is a 30 year--old female who worked in information technology.
She felt her exercise routine had reduced. As a horse rider/owner, her
ten mile round trip to ride and maintain the horse became a seventy mile
round trip due to flooded roads. She commented that: "the time I
would spend riding was actually spent getting there" and
consequently, she did not ride during the duration of the water loss.
Furthermore, her running routine altered. She became more conscious of
factors associated with exercise including the practicality of
showering. She indicated that:
if you'd just had a shower you wouldn't do anything after it. You
needed to plan ahead what you were doing ... when I went out
running I'd be thinking people would be looking at me thinking I'd
be dirty and not having a shower (laughs) ... l just knew I'd be
able to have a shower when I got back which I had to plan rather
than spontaneously do.
Kate explained that a disrupted exercise routine reduced
automaticity and therefore increased the perceived effort required in
maintaining her exercise routine: "if I hadn't had a break
from running I'd still be running, and now I can't get back
into it".
Mary (female)--perceived exercise as reduced
Mary is a 30 year--old female exercise instructor who regularly
works one--to--one with clients. She did not exercise for seven days
during the period of water loss. She explained that: "I
couldn't face the thought of being that dirty, wet hair dripping
with sweat and everything and then not being able to have a proper
shower afterwards, so I didn't train for a week". Mary also
evidenced body image concerns,
it sounds ridiculous because I know it's not happening but you can
just feel yourself expanding, I just didn't feel so good about
myself and also with the situation you were just having a few more
bad things to eat than you would normally do. Everything was a bit
"topsy--turvey", your eating habits and your exercise habits so 1
just felt a bit bloated and not as good as I would normally.
Temporal coping responses during water loss
In order to explore vulnerability to water loss, and to try to
understand the challenges in maintaining exercise behavior, we
considered the significance of cognitive, affective and coping responses
during different phases of the natural disaster. We focused on the day
that the impending water loss was announced, during the first three days
of water loss, and, thereafter until running water was re--established.
The day water loss was announced
Table 1 presents those responses identified by participants
following the announcement that the Gloucestershire area would lose
water supply. It was consistently noted among participants that there
was a degree of uncertainty with local radio and television announcing
that water loss would occur within a 'couple of days' and last
7--10 days.
As Table 1 indicates, individuals felt uncertain about future
events when first hearing about impending water loss. This sentiment
expressed by all participants, reflected a perceived lack of control
over events, and was associated with affective states such as panic,
fear, anger and shock. At this point participants noted that in an
attempt to cope they utilized emotion--focused strategies (e.g., humor,
denial, social support for emotional reasons) and problem--focused
strategies (e.g., water/food stocking, problem solving, risk assessing).
Terry exemplified the confusion that quickly stimulated
problem--focused coping following the announcement of impending water
loss. He indicated
initially there was a bit of confusion about what to do and what
was going to happen, but very quickly people started to realize,
well, collect water in buckets, dustbins whatever just to conserve
it, there was a bit of a rush at first to get bottled water, doubts
whether it would be there tomorrow.
June described the panic she initially experienced, and in order to
help dissipate this intense emotion she became more practically focused.
When you heard water was going you started to really panic, and
then rather than panic let's do something about it ... which I
suppose gave us something to focus on as opposed to just sitting
there thinking well when's it going to go.
These illustrative quotes clearly highlight the uncertainty
experienced by all participants when hearing of impending water loss.
They also exemplify problem--focused coping (e.g., planning, resource
accumulation) evidenced by all participants. Whilst a range of
strategies evidenced were intended to be emotion--focused (e.g.,
distraction, social support and alcohol), for some individuals
problem--focused strategies also helped to manage emotions as they
became a distracter. At this stage of water loss, the clear priority for
individuals was to accumulate resources including informational (e.g.,
about the impending water loss) and tangible resources (e.g., water,
water storage facilities). Due to the preoccupation with the
accumulation of water resources, there was little consideration of
exercise including the pursuit of exercise, consequences of disrupted
routines and so on.
First three days of water loss
During the first three days of water loss participants focused
their efforts on learning to source and adapt their use of water. As
such, when exploring responses during the first three days of water loss
there was a shift evidenced in thinking and coping towards a greater
problem--focused orientation (see Table 2). Participants became
conscious of water in terms of quantities required, its collection, and
prioritizing how to use water. This was evidenced by Kate,
everything just takes longer, cooking you have to stop and think
and you still go to turn the tap on and there's no water there, or
you go to flush the loo and there's not water there so it's a case
of being mindful all the time that the situations a bit different.
Participants referred to the unprecedented situation they faced,
and all participants believed this communal challenge increased
community spirit. There was a great deal of informational and practical
support seeking and giving among participants, which included showering
and washing clothes at friends and family. Many participants described
helping to distribute water, clearing flooded houses, moving sandbags,
checking on neighbors and seeking to help those less fortunate. This
increased communal interaction was seen as a positive outcome resulting
from the water loss. Terry explained, "I think you meet more people
that you would have done normally going in your house at five
o'clock and shutting your door, I met people I'd never known
before that lived a few doors away".
The increased communal support also engaged participants in
activities recognized to varying extents as alternative forms of
exercise by participants. Nick and Terry both felt that activities such
as carrying large water bottles and moving sandbags provided alternative
exercise. Whilst Tony acknowledged such activities as alternative forms
of exercise, he observed that they did not provide the 'buzz'
he attained from running, an activity he ceased to undertake during the
water loss. As such, these activities failed to attain his
self--regulatory objectives.
During this stage of water loss, with one exception (Jenny),
participants holding fixed exercise beliefs identified perceived
barriers to exercise. For example, Mary made comment that a desired
characteristic of exercise was that the intensity should be sufficient
to be sweat--inducing. June felt the gym environment provided the forum
in which to bum off calories at the end of a working day. Tony believed
that he could only attain the 'buzz' he desired to enhance his
mood from running.
These fixed beliefs presented a barrier to pursuing and/or
recognizing alternative forms of exercise. For example, Mary did not
wish to get sweaty due to the lack of shower facilities and a reliance
on strip washes. Tony could not accommodate his running routine for
similar reasons. June's gym was closed and she failed to identify
an alternative venue for exercising. These individuals did not
acknowledge activities of lesser intensity as exercise (e.g., walking),
nor did they acknowledge the potential contribution of such activities
toward their self--regulatory objectives.
Those individuals holding flexible exercise beliefs sought to
resume their pre--water loss exercise regimen, and where this was not
possible pursued alternative exercise. This was evidenced by Lucy,
I packed my gym kit and I thought I'll just phone them [Gym] and
then I found out they were closed, so initially I thought, 'oh',
'cause I wanted to go up, but then 1 thought well of course it will
be closed. It was maybe a bit optimistic to think they would carry
on as Tewksbury went under you know (laughs) so initially there was
that disappointment and then I thought well I'll just try and go
out on my bike instead, which I don't normally do but I was just
feeling a bit worried that I was going to put on weight really,
worried that I was going to get out of shape, and I guess there was
probably some physiological element to it you know ... I think I
did need the exercise I really enjoyed it.
Here Lucy evidences adaptation to her exercise regimen that enabled
her to maintain her self--regulatory objectives for exercise.
As water loss endured
As the period of water--loss endured beyond three days there
appeared to be a further change in behavioral, cognitive, emotional and
coping responses. The narratives offered indicated a desire for
circumstances to return to normal and an increased consciousness
regarding factors such as water use, hygiene, community spirit and
exercise (see Table 3). Whilst participants indicated that community
spirit persisted, it was also noted that the novelty of circumstances
waned. The desire to see a resumption of water supply and the waning
resolve resulting from ongoing inconveniences was described succinctly
by Mary who said, "I was getting more and more frustrated with the
whole situation".
The ongoing challenges and associated thoughts resulted in an
increased unpleasant affect with terms such as fed--up, miserable,
irritated, and depressed being used to describe this shift. Jenny
presented one of the more intense emotional responses to the enduring
water loss. This is evident in the following quote;
by the Friday of the week that we lost the water I did get totally
fed up with it the fact that it was another day without water,
trying the taps to see if any came out and it was quite depressing
really, the rain as well that didn't help (laughs) ... I'd had
enough by then and wanted it all to be over.
As water loss continued providing extended circumstances beyond
their control, participants began to engage more emotion--focused
strategies including wishful thinking, positive thinking and social
support. Kate expressed a desire to remove herself from the situation
and observed that others did leave the affected areas.
If it hadn't have been for the horse [Kate owned a horse which
required daily care] and having to get around for that, and work
being open, then I would have just gone home to London for a week
or so. I know people who did flee the county just for convenience
sake, trying to carry on with their normal lives.
Participants' use of problem--focused coping continued to
focus on prioritizing activities and the use of resources. This was
attributed to ongoing challenges that would not abate until water supply
returned. As suggested by Kate "the time needed to do things and
the knock on effect to organize things like going to see people and
going to other places ... even mundane things like doing the washing up
take longer".
A further trend evidenced as the period of water loss endured was
an increasing awareness of reduced or modified exercise regimens and a
greater resolve to resume pre--water loss exercise routines,
particularly among those individuals with fixed exercise beliefs. Mary
indicated that she did not maintain her usual exercise schedule because
she wished to avoid getting hot and sweaty. Mary perceived exercise as
comprising of activities of an intensity that resulted in sweating. The
consequences of a lack of exercise became more evident as the period of
water loss continued.
I got to the Monday and I was like 'I don't care anymore, I'm
training today whether I'm dirty or not' so I did and I had a bath
that night at [friends] sisters ... I was getting more and more
frustrated with the whole situation, being in here [gym] and having
nobody here, and not being able to train.
When viewed collectively, the qualitative excerpts and Tables (1,
2, and 3) evidence more emotion--focused coping when anticipating
(uncertain, lack of control) the novel stressor and as the stressor
endured and resilience was challenged. By comparison, problem--focused
coping was evidenced throughout all stages of the water loss. This
largely centered on resource accumulation to manage the situation.
In summary of the results, we suggest that the rich narratives
offered by participants offer an insight into these coping trends. A
finding that appears to have emerged is that individuals holding
flexible exercise beliefs also tended to maintain an exercise regime.
Discussion
The present study explored the effects of unexpected loss of water
supply on coping strategies used among a sample of habitual exercisers.
Results indicate that the loss of water supply influenced the exercise
regimens of participants differently. The extent to which participants
continued or ceased exercise appeared related to the relative
flexibility of exercise beliefs. Participants holding flexible exercise
beliefs utilized problem--focused and appraisal focused coping to adapt
to the challenges presented by water loss thereby continuing to exercise
regularly albeit having to change their regimen. Examples of this
include exercising at a lower intensity to avoid sweating, completing
activities outside of the gym environment (i.e., walking, exercising in
the home), or pursuing a different activity altogether. Additionally,
participants holding flexible beliefs tended to recognize that daily
life in the absence of running water presented more opportunities to
engage in physical activity, with examples including carrying buckets of
water and moving sand bags. Conversely, participants with fixed exercise
beliefs typically failed to recognize alternative exercise behaviors
undertaken (e.g., increased walking, increased physical activity
associated with attaining and using water), or pursue exercise of a
lesser intensity in alternative venues. It should be noted that these
participants reported doing similar tasks, but importantly from a coping
perspective, did not recognize that such tasks provided exercise or
contributed to self--regulatory goals.
Of the participants investigated, Jenny was the only participant to
perceive an increase her exercise. Jenny evidenced fixed exercise
beliefs in that she perceived exercise as an activity undertaken in the
gym. She visited the gym more regularly during the period of water loss
as she found a quieter gym a more pleasant environment to exercise in,
she also felt it helped her manage negative moods resulting from the
water loss. Jenny's increase in exercise might have been
facilitated by the decision to re--open the gym the day following water
loss. By contrast, the sports facilities used by some participants
remained closed for the duration of water loss (due to flooding) and as
such participants who maintained exercise under such conditions adopted
a flexible approach toward exercise during water loss because of
constraints/considerations such as closed facilities and health and
safety concerns (e.g., poor road conditions for cycling).
Cody and Lucy offer examples of participants evidencing flexible
exercise beliefs. When faced with constraints to their usual exercise
regimen, they consciously pursued and/ or acknowledged other physical
activities. They both noted enhanced motivation for exercise resulting
from modified exercise routines. The positive impact of modifying
exercise regimens has been demonstrated in previous research
highlighting the potential benefits of this strategy as a motivational
tool (Jensen & Lorish, 1994). In this instance, the changes were
enforced, but both participants became cognizant of, and confident with
exercise alternatives, and the benefits of varying routines.
Results show participants holding fixed exercise beliefs were more
likely to perceive a reduced exercise regimen following water loss.
These individuals also reported consequential unpleasant emotions and
thoughts. For example, Kate expressed concerns that she would find it
difficult to resume her exercise regimen following disruptions. June,
Mary and Tony expressed body image concerns having perceived a reduced
exercise routine during the period of water loss. Of these participants
June and Mary were concerned that they had gained weight and used terms
including "bloated" and "sluggish" to describe their
corresponding perceptions. Tony expressed a concern that he might have
gained weight in the form of increased body fat, and lost muscle tone,
something he said was based on factual information as he weighed himself
daily during the water loss. It is proposed that these findings indicate
that exercise was used as a weight--management strategy and that sudden
cessation without a perceived effective replacement led to rapid changes
in emotions. Preoccupation with weight and fear of weight--gain has
become so pervasive that it has been termed 'normative
discontent' (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel--Moore, 1985; p.
267). Tony and June both acknowledged engaging in alternative forms of
exercising. June acknowledged an increase in time spent walking whilst
Tony noted that the daily task of carrying water was a physical
activity. We suggest that if these participants were to recognize these
activities as exercise contributing to self--regulatory goals, then they
might not have experienced these body image concerns.
The references made to body image concerns are consistent with
research indicating that people exercise to lose or control weight,
alter body shape and enhance perceived attractiveness, particularly
among females (Cash, Novy, & Grant, 1994; Furnham & Greaves,
1994; Hausenblas & Fallon, 2006). Results of the present study also
offer an insight into the difficulties experienced by individuals when
seeking to attain balance between exercise and diet in order to maintain
a positive sense of body image. Due to the sudden unexpected nature of
the water loss, the resulting changes to exercise behavior had the
potential to disturb the homeostasis between diet and exercise. We
propose that the sedentary nature of many modes of employment and
lifestyle in general can mean that formal exercise represents the only
time when people are active. It is possible that individuals became
conscious of the sedentary nature of their jobs when habitual exercise
completed during leisure time became problematic. Participants might
also have become more aware of extrinsic motives for exercise such as
body image manipulation.
June was certainly aware of this when describing how she
consciously monitored her dietary intake when she was no longer able to
exercise in the gym. June subconsciously pursued less vigorous, and
therefore less sweat inducing forms of exercise noting "looking
back I didn't realize [that she was walking more to compensate for
an inability to use the gym] it at the time". Having made this
observation, she then reflected further, suggesting that she believed
the intention of these behaviors was to address hygiene concerns whilst
also seeking to prevent weight gain and maintain positive moods. Had she
acknowledged the contribution of these behaviors towards her weight
management goals and emotion regulation goals at the time, positive
affect and body image might have been more effectively sustained and
unpleasant emotions and cognitions avoided (Carver & Scheier, 1990).
We suggest that exercise for many participants in the present study
was not only used to regulate body image, but also to regulate mood.
Those participants experiencing negative emotions and cognitions
resulting from exercise reduction attempted to cope by engaging in a
range of behaviors. Some of these behaviors appear to have been
undertaken subconsciously as indicated by June. Mary evidenced fixed
exercise beliefs in that she perceived the need to exercise at a higher
intensity that resulted in sweating. She perceived individuals working
at lesser intensities as not 'working hard', "those other
people weren't working so hard were like 'well yeah it's
okay' [difficulties in showering] and I was like 'yeah
that's 'cause you don't work very hard'". She
was not prepared to exercise at a lesser intensity, and as she did not
wish to become sweaty, she refrained from exercising. As the period of
water loss endured, she increasingly experienced the negative affective
consequences of abstinence. She eventually sought social support to
access showering and laundry facilities so that she could resume
exercise and alleviate the frustration and "sluggish" fee
lings she experienced. Tony also evidenced fixed beliefs in that he
perceived running as the only activity to provide the 'buzz'
he craved from exercise. He indicated that he had become so frustrated
with a lack of exercise that he reached a point where he was going to
resume running again irrespective of the difficulties faced in
maintaining hygiene. We surmise that these results support the
contention that many exercisers utilize exercise as a self--regulatory
behavior, and for those individuals holding fixed exercise beliefs, when
exercise routines cease abruptly or are impaired, their fixed exercise
beliefs hinder their ability to adapt quickly and efficiently. For such
individuals, unpleasant emotions are evidenced until such time that
resolution is attained. Within the present study, those individuals
holding fixed beliefs took longer to identify a solution to disrupted
exercise regimens and as such failed to maintain the self--regulatory
functions of exercise.
Lazarus (2000) advocated the use of idiographic and qualitative
research methods in order to develop a more complete understanding of
the transactional nature of coping. Although a retrospective approach
was utilized, collecting data within a week of water supply
reintroduction helped facilitate clarity of recollection among
participants, as evidenced by the vivid accounts provided. The data
provided helps develop an awareness of vulnerability to water loss
across the different stages of the stress transaction. Within the
interviews participants were asked to consider the stressors and coping
responses when hearing about impending water loss, during the first
three days of water loss and thereafter. When exploring responses it
appears that participants experienced a greater emotional response
during impending water loss and following the first three days of water
loss. This resulted in the use of more emotion--focused coping when
anticipating the novel stressor and as the stressor endured beyond three
days thereby challenging resilience. Problem--focused coping was
evidenced throughout all stages of the water loss and was primarily
concerned with water accumulation and priorities for use.
Personal accounts of the stages of water loss evidenced the
interaction between appraisals, coping and emotional responses as the
stress transaction unfolded (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). A coping
strategy used at one stage of water loss (e.g., hand holding or
prioritizing activities/reducing exercise when hearing of water loss)
might not be evidenced, or evidenced to a lesser extent in other stages.
Furthermore, findings offer support for research indicating that the
adaptive outcomes of specific coping strategies with acute stressors
might become less effective if the stressor becomes chronic stress
(Wethington & Kessler, 1991). Sacrificing exercise on the day water
loss was announced was perceived as helping to create time to accumulate
water resources and manage the increased demands resulting from water
loss. However, as water loss endured, those individuals who did not
perceive a resumption/ maintenance of exercise experienced undesired
psychological and physical consequences. This was particularly notable
among individuals holding fixed beliefs regarding exercise. As the
period of water loss endured, sacrificing exercise was no longer seen to
be an effective coping strategy. Those individuals still not exercising
evidenced an increasing resolve to resume exercise commonly noting the
negative impact of exercise reduction on their self--regulatory goals.
Limitations and Future Research
Limitations of the present study include the sample size and
sampling methods used. Caution should be exercised when interpreting and
generalizing the results of a small sample size. However, the ecological
validity and personal meaning captured within this study helped to
enhance the credibility of findings. Using convenience sampling,
participants were not recruited as a function of the frequency, duration
and intensity of their weekly exercise regimen. It is plausible that
those individuals with a higher frequency, intensity and duration of
exercise might be affected more by loss of water supply. Whilst a range
of weekly exercise regimens were described, low participant numbers mean
that it is not possible to make inferences regarding exercise habit
strength and the consequences of water loss.
In conclusion, results of the present study demonstrate the role of
water supply in facilitating exercise in western society, albeit in the
context of a specific population and location. When this resource is no
longer available, habitual exercisers engage a range of coping efforts
in an attempt to maintain or resume exercise. The narratives provided by
participants evidence the role of exercise beliefs in coping with
unexpected stressors. We suggest that these findings could be translated
to represent the beliefs regarding any behavior under threat from
unexpected stressors. Exploring the role of beliefs in effectively
coping with unexpected stressors represents a fruitful line of future
enquiry. This should include studies with sample sizes that facilitate
the statistical analysis of suggestions made within this manuscript.
References
Beggs, J., Haines, V., & Hulbery, J. S (1996). The effects of
personal network and local community contexts on the receipt of formal
help during disaster recovery. International Journal of Mass Emergencies
and Disasters, 14, 57--78.
Berger, B. G., & Motl, R. W. (2000). Exercise and mood: A
selective review and synthesis of research employing the Profile of Mood
States. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 69--92.
Biddle, S. J. H. (2000). Emotion, mood and physical activity. In S.
J. H. Biddle, K. R. Fox, & S. H. Boutcher (Eds.), Physical activity
and psychological well--being (pp. 63--87). London: Routledge.
Blaikie, P., Cannon, T., Davis, I., & Wisner, B. (1994). At
risk: Natural hazards, people's vulnerability, and disasters,
London: Routledge.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions
of positive and negative affect: A control--process view. Psychological
Review, 97, 19--35.
Cash, T. F., Novy, R L., & Grant, J. R. (1994). Why do women
exercise? Factor analysis and further validation of the Reasons for
Exercise Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 539--544.
Creswell, J. W. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design:
Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Creswell, J. W. (2009). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative,
and mixed methods approach (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cutter, S. L., Boruff, B. J., & Shirley, W. L. (2003). Social
vulnerability to environmental hazards. Social Science Quarterly,
84,242--261.
Furnham, A., & Greaves, N. (1994). Gender and locus of control
correlates of body image dissatisfaction. European Journal of
Personality, 8, 183--200.
Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2006). Exercise and body
image: A meta--analysis. Psychology and Health, 21, 33--47.
Jensen G. M., & Lorish C. D. (1994). Promoting patient
cooperation with exercise programs: linking research, theory, and
practice. Arthritis Care Research, 7, 181--9.
July floods : At a glance. Retrieved June 18th, 2009, from
http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/in_ depth/629/629/6911778.stm.
Lane, A. M., Jackson, A., & Terry, P. C. (2005). Preferred
modality influenced on exercise--induced mood changes. Journal of Sports
Science and Medicine, 4, 195--200.
Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Theory--based stress measurement.
Psychological lnquiry, 1, 3--13.
Lazarus, R. S. (2000). Cognitive--motivational--relational theory
of emotion. In Y. L. Hanin (Ed.), Emotions in sport (pp. 39--63).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress appraisal and
coping. New York: Springer.
Long, B. C., & Van Stavel, R. (1995). Effects of exercise
training on anxiety: A meta--analysis. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 7, 167--189.
McAuley, E., Elavsky, S., Motl, R. W., Konopack, J. F., Hu, L.,
& Marquez, D. X. (2005). Physical activity, self--efficacy, and
self--esteem: Longitudinal relationships in older adults. Journal of
Gerontology, 60, 268--275. QSR NVIVO--2 [Computer software]. (2002).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Reacher, M., McKenzie, K., Lane, C., Nichols, T., Iverson, A.,
Hepple, P., Walter, T., Laxton, C., & Simpson, J. (2004). Health
impacts of flooding in Lewes: A comparison of reported gastrointestinal
and other illness and mental health in flooded and non--flooded
households. Communicable Disease and Public Health, 7, 1--8.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel--Moore, R. (1985). Women
and weight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.),
Psychology and gender: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1985 (pp.
267--307). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Rostad, F., & Long, B.C. (1996). Exercise as a coping strategy
for stress: A review. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 27,
197--222.
Scully, D., Kremer J., Meade, M. M., Graham, R., & Dudgeon, K.
(1998). Physical exercise and psychological well--being: A critical
review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 32, 111--120.
Sweet, S. (1998). The effect of a natural disaster on social
cohesion: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Mass
Emergencies and Disasters, 16, 321--331.
Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., & McClain, T. M. (1994).
Self--regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising
energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67, 910--925.
Tunstall, S., Tapsell, S., Green, C., Floyd, P., & George, C.
(2006). The Health Effects of flooding: Social research results from
England and Wales. Journal of Water and Health, 4, 365--380.
Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (1991). Situations and
processes of coping. In J. Eckenrode (Ed.), The social context of coping
(pp. 13--29). New York: Plenum Press.
Tracey J. Devonport and Andrew M. Lane
University of Wolverham pton, UK
Diane Crone
University of Gloucestershire, UK
Table 1
Responses on the day impending water loss was announced
Cognition Affect Coping
Feeling helpless, Panic, Denial, Humor,
Confused, Anxious, Using alcohol, Venting,
Concerned for Anger, Support seeking (problem,
self and others, information and emotional
Intrigued by novelty Disbelief, support),
of situation,
Shock, Water/food stocking,
Uncertainty Focused, Risk assessment/ problem
regarding future, solving,
Compassion for others, Fear, Support giving,
Inconvenienced Frustrated Put in perspective
Table 2
Responses during the first three days of water loss
Cognition Affect Coping
Mindful/ conscious Focused, Problem solving, Planning,
of water use,
Compassion for others, Grateful, Prioritize activities,
Intrigued by Enjoyment, Prioritize use of water,
novelty of situation,
Conscious of intensified Frustration, Daily focus,
community spirit, Guilt--not Resource accumulation,
Feeling blessed, exercising Seeking social
support--practical,
Self--conscious Put in perspective,
of personal hygiene, Distraction,
Perceived catastrophizing Offering practical support,
More radical haircut,
Exercising
Table 3
Responses to the latter (4+ days) stages of water loss
Cognition Affect Coping
Mindful/ conscious Bored, Positive thinking,
of water use,
Compassion for others, Sluggish, Wishful thinking,
Blessed, Calm, Putting in perspective,
Conscious of intensified Enjoyment, Distraction,
community spirit, Frustration, Alternative routines,
Appreciative of Positive, Daily focus,
support services,
Self--conscious of Guilt----using Prioritize activities,
personal hygiene, bottled water Prioritize water use,
Unable to see the end, for toilets, Seek social support
Self--conscious Depressed, problem & emotion,
when exercising,
Perceived enduring Fed up, Planning,
challenge/strain, Miserable, Provide social
Community spirit, Tedious, support----practical,
Demands of partner to Irritated, Exercising
maintain hygiene, Guilt---not
Inconvenienced, exercising
Self--conscious
when exercising,
Self--conscious of
personal hygiene