Student motivation in physical education and engagement in physical activity.
Bryan, Charity Leigh ; Solmon, Melinda A.
The mediating processes paradigm provides a lens for understanding
and interpreting how motivational constructs mediate teacher behaviors
and student learning. This framework is a "response-oriented"
approach that grew out of the unidirectional process-product paradigm of
the 1960's (Doyle, p. 170, 1977). Mediating constructs, such as
student interest, social background, prior knowledge and beliefs, and
the classroom context have been explored in attempts to explain more of
the "why," or in what context, learning occurs. Student
learning characteristics and the instructional setting are the essential
features of the mediating process paradigm.
The cognitive mediational paradigm provides a framework for the
study of student attitudes and perceptions (Solmon, 2003). Students are
recognized as active and controlling agents in the learning process.
They enter with varying backgrounds, knowledge, and experiences that
serve as filters through which they interpret instruction and make
meaning of classroom experiences. Based on prior experiences, students
form attitudes that are theorized to be powerful influences on decisions
they make about engaging in activity. Perceptions and attitudes are
important mediators between teacher actions and what students learn and
do. Positive attitudes are associated with adaptive motivational
behaviors and exerting effort. Negative attitudes are expected to result
in a reluctance to engage in activities (Solmon, 2003).
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) provides a
theoretical framework to study student motivation. Motivation is
conceptualized as a continuum ranging from amotivation, or a lack of
motivation to intrinsic motivation, defined as engaging in an activity
as an end in itself. In between a motivation and intrinsic motivation,
varying levels of extrinsic motivation reflect increasing levels of
self-determination. When individuals take part only due to the threat of
punishment or for a reward of some kind, they are at a level of external
regulation (Standage, Treasure, Duda, & Prusak, 2003). Introjected
regulation is characterized by recognizing some level of value in an
activity, but feeling as though one "ought" to participate out
of guilt or obligation. Identified regulation is the level at which
participants see the outcome as beneficial and they begin to participate
because they want to. Integrated regulation represents a level where the
activity is part of the individual's identity and is relevant to
higher goals, though it may still be somewhat extrinsically motivated
(Biddle, 1999). Intrinsic motivation is the highest level of
self-determination whereby an individual participates in an activity
simply for the sake of the activity itself (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Unfortunately, much of the research in the area of motivation finds that
participation in physical activity is often not intrinsically motivated
(Ryan, Frederick, Lepes, Rubio, & Sheldon, 1997). Higher levels of
self-determination and autonomy are more likely to elicit long-term
motivated behavior.
Attitude is a factor that should be examined when investigating
motivation levels because of its potential link to participation, or
lack thereof, in physical activity. Attitudes are often based on
experiences and events from childhood (Brustad, 1991) and attitude
formation is largely shaped by the fundamental beliefs that an
individual holds (Ajzen, 1988, 1993). Essentially, individuals'
beliefs influence their attitudes toward certain things. Perceptions and
attitudes are important mediators between teacher actions and what
students learn and do (Solmon, 2003).
Used extensively throughout the literature, the term attitude is
often banal and its definition, in psychometric terms, is often unclear.
Frequently in attitude-related studies only one component of attitude is
examined. However, this construct includes more than just one single
aspect (Subramaniam & Silverman, 2000). Silverman and Subramaniam
(1999) conducted a review of measurement issues on student attitudes in
physical education and physical activity. They concluded that the
research up to that time had yielded mixed results, and recommended
additional research be conducted to recognize the ways in which student
attitudes affect participation in physical activity. Parish and Treasure
(2003) observed that young people's failure to meet current
recommendations for moderate to vigorous physical activity may be partly
related to lack of motivation. The investigation of the influence that
attitude has on motivation has the potential to provide a clearer
understanding of students' decisions about being physically active.
Perceived climate is an important variable in the investigation of
motivation in the physical education class context. The seminal work
related to motivational climate was conducted by Ames and her colleagues
(Ames, 1992; Ames & Archer, 1988). A task or mastery-involved
environment is one where students demonstrate their ability by mastering
a task and comparisons are self-referenced. In this environment,
students in physical education are more likely to be intrinsically
motivated, believe that there are no gender disparities, and believe
that success is the result of effort (Treasure, 1997). An ego-involved
climate, on the other hand, Implies that children demonstrate their
ability by having a superior performance over another individual
(Nicholls, 1984). In this ego or performance-oriented climate, levels of
boredom increase while intrinsic motivation decreases, students attempt
to win or succeed through deception or cheating, and ability, not
effort, is emphasized (Treasure, 1997). Regardless of the student
perceptions of the climate, competence alone is not sufficient for
engagement. Recognizing the value of the task is also necessary in that
individuals must find significance in the activity and believe they can
be competent before they will willfully engage (Wigfield & Eccles,
2002).
Student engagement is cultivated when perceptions of a learning
climate are salient. Further, there is evidence that a task or mastery
climate contributes to student learning (Biddle, 2001). In physical
education, Parish and Treasure (2003) reported that physical activity
levels were positively correlated with perceptions of a mastery-oriented
climate. In addition, Ferrer-Caja and Weiss (2000) found that students
who perceived that learning and participation were promoted in their
physical education classes were more likely to engage in the activities,
exert effort, and focus on learning the task or activity. A link between
a mastery-oriented climate and levels of self-determination has also
been established (Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Papaioannou, 1994; Parish
& Treasure, 2003). Research (Parish & Treasure, 2003; Treasure
& Roberts, 2001) demonstrates the necessity and importance of
structuring mastery climates in physical education as a means of getting
children in physical education to be as active as possible. Ntoumanis
(2002), however, points out that physical education teachers are often
unsuccessful in constructing adaptive motivational climates in their
classes.
One goal of physical education is to promote physical activity and
it is important to explore ways to accomplish that goal. The middle
school years represent a critical period in the development of physical
activity patterns that extend to adulthood. Participation in physical
activity declines as children progress through adolescence, and the
decline is more pronounced for girls than boys (Bauman et al., 2009;
Parish & Treasure, 2003). The relationships among levels of
motivation, attitudes, and perceptions of the learning climate, and how
these factors relate to engagement in physical activity, have not been
thoroughly investigated. Employing the mediating processes paradigm to
investigate student motivation in physical education classes by
examining relationships between these variables can provide valuable
insight into designing class environments that foster engagement in
physical activity.
Using self-determination theory as a framework, the purpose of this
study was to investigative relationships between perceptions of the
learning climate, attitudes, levels of self-determination, and
engagement in physical activity in order to determine the role each
plays in student motivation toward physical education. Specific research
questions addressed were (a) How do students' perceptions of the
motivational climate relate to student attitudes and levels of
self-determination? (b) How do measures of student attitude,
self-determination, and perception of the climate relate to student
levels of activity as measured by pedometer counts? (c) How do
students' motivation and activity levels vary by grade level and
gender?
Methods
Participants
Participants in this study were 114 sixth (n = 30; 16 boys, 14
girls), seventh (n = 30; 18 boys, 12 girls), and eighth (n = 54; 23
boys, 31 girls) graders enrolled in a suburban public school. Students
participated in 90-minute physical education classes every other day on
a block schedule. They received instruction from certified physical
education specialists. The middle school physical education curriculum
focused on team sports, organized games, and fitness activities. One
class of sixth graders, one class of seventh graders and two classes of
eighth graders participated in the study. Prior to data collection,
child assent and parental permission forms were completed. Institutional
review board (IRB) approval was also obtained from the institution prior
to data collection.
Instrumentation
Motivational climate. The Learning and Performance Orientations in
Physical Education Classes Questionnaire (LAPOPECQ) is a 27-item
questionnaire used to measure perceptions of classroom goal structure in
physical education (Papaioannou, 1994). Participants are to think about
their physical education class and respond to a 5-point Likert scale
with one being strong disagreement and five being strong agreement. The
LAPOPECQ assesses perceptions of a learning and performance climate and
contains five factors: (a)teacher-initiated learning orientation; (b)
students' learning orientation; (c) students' competitive
orientation; (d) students' worries about mistakes; and (e) outcome
orientation without effort. These factors load on two higher order
factors: learning and performance.
The learning orientation subscale addresses perceptions of the
teachers' behaviors and the students' satisfaction with
learning. A sample item from the teacher-initiated learning orientation
is: "The PE teacher is completely satisfied when every
student's skills are improving." The student's learning
orientation is assessed through questions such as, "I feel very
satisfied when I learn new skills and games." Perceptions of a
performance climate were assessed using questions regarding performance
compared to peers, ability levels (performance with little effort), and
how much the student is concerned with making mistakes in physical
education class. The students' competitive orientation was measured
by response to statements such as: "Successful students are thought
to be those who perform skills better than their classmates."
Students' worries about mistakes, also part of the performance
orientation subscale, are represented by questions such as,
"Students worry about failure in performing skills because it would
lead to the disapproval of others." The third performance-oriented
factor is outcome orientation without effort, which is measured by
questions such as, "The PE teacher looks completely satisfied with
those students who manage to win with little effort." Using
confirmatory factor analysis, construct validity for the LAPOPECQ
questionnaire was also established by Papaioannou (1994). In this study,
data were analyzed using the two higher order factors, learning and
performance climates.
Attitude. A scale developed by Subramaniam and Silverman (2000) was
used to assess student attitudes toward physical education. The
instrument consists of 20 items with a 5point Likert scale used for
scoring. A score of one indicates strong disagreement while a score of
five indicates strong agreement. The attitude scale measures two
components of attitude: enjoyment and usefulness as it relates to the
current physical education program and the physical education teacher.
Students respond to 10 items related to enjoyment, such as the
following: "The games I learn in my physical education class get me
excited about physical education." The remaining 10 items represent
the usefulness factor, assessed through items like, "I feel my
physical education teacher makes learning in my physical education class
valuable for me." Content validity for the attitude instrument was
ascertained through a panel of experts. The content validity for
attitude enjoyment was .94 and attitude usefulness was .99. Subramaniam
and Silverman (2000) also established construct validity through
confirmatory factor analysis. Negative items are included in the scale
and were reverse coded prior to data analysis.
SIMS Motivation Scale. Levels of self-determined motivation were
assessed using the Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS), a 16-item
self-report inventory that assesses intrinsic motivation, identified
regulation, external regulation and amotivation (Guay, Vallerand, &
Blanchard, 2000). Participants are asked to use the scale and indicate
the answer that best describes the reason why they are currently engaged
in an activity. A seven-point Likert scale is used for all responses. A
score of one does not correspond at all and a score of seven corresponds
exactly (Standage et al., 2003). Sample statements from the SIMS
representing each of the subscales are: "Because I think that this
activity is interesting"(intrinsic motivation); "Because I
think this activity is good for me" (identified regulation);
"Because I feel like I have to do it (external regulation); and
"There may be good reasons to do this activity, but personally I
don't see any" (amotivation).
The SIMS assessment exhibits internal consistency using
Cronbach's alpha levels (Guay et al., 2000). Further, the SIMS is
able to discern motivational constructs adequately between genders and
across a wide variety of activities. Previous research (Standage &
Treasure, 2002) demonstrated the ability of the SIMS to delineate
between intrinsic motivation, identified regulation, external regulation
and amotivation. The SIMS has consistently demonstrated both reliability
and construct validity in previous research studies (Guay et al., 2000;
Standage et al., 2003).
Self report of physical activity. The Physical Activity
Questionnaire for Children (PAQC) was used as a measure of self reported
levels of physical activity. Designed for students in grades four and
above, the PAQ-C measures levels of moderate to vigorous physical
activity across different settings (Kowalski, Crocker, & Faulker
1997). Students report how many times in the previous week they
participated in a wide range of physical activity behaviors (Crocker,
Bailey, Faulkner, Kowalski & McGrath, 1997; Kowalski et al., 1997).
Other physical activity bouts related to physical education, free time,
recess, extracurricular sports, weekend activities, and evening
activities are also addressed within this instrument. The PAQ-C for
example, asks, "In the last 7 days, on how many days right after
school, did you do sports, or play games in which you were very
active?" Another question asks, "This past weekend, how many
times did you play sports, dance or play games in which you were very
active?" The PAQ-C has many advantages, such as its ease of use,
the expedient manner in which it is explained and administered, and the
fact that the instrument is of minimal cost. Kowalski et al. (1997), in
their validation study on children in grades four through eight,
reported a one-week test-retest reliability of .75 for boys and .82 for
girls.
Physical activity. Pedometers were used as an objective measure of
physical activity on three physical education days and once for a
24-hour count. Pedometers are a widely acceptable tool for measuring
student levels of physical activity (Graser, Pangrazi, & Vincent,
2009; Scruggs et al., 2003), and Vincent and Pangrazi (2002) established
that three to four days of pedometer counts could predict habitual
physical activity levels. In order to meet the Surgeon General's
recommendation of 30 minutes of activity a day, individuals would need
approximately 3,200 to 4,000 steps to meet this goal (Le Masurier,
2004). Pedometers provide an instantaneous response in a quantifiable
way regarding the activity, which can be quite encouraging for
individuals (Le Masurier, 2004). A disadvantage of pedometer use is that
they are unable to quantify the intensity, frequency or duration of
activity (Beighle, Pangrazi, & Vincent, 2001); however, in field
research and in physical education classes, they are an extremely
straightforward and efficient mechanism for assessing activity levels.
Students in this study were accustomed to the use of pedometers in
physical education, which greatly facilitated the management aspect of
this research. For the purposes of this study, during physical
education, students participated in a variety of activities such as
bowling, soccer, walking, capture the flag, and fitness testing.
Students were monitored throughout the activity time to ensure that
pedometers were not manipulated or lost during movement. At the end of
each physical education class, pedometers were removed and the
researchers recorded the pedometer count. Reliability for the
Digi-Walker pedometer has been established through correlations of step
counts, energy expenditure, and engagement time in physical activity
(Welk et al., 2000).
Procedures
Data were collected over five consecutive physical education days.
Due to the time constraints of block scheduling, researchers met with
two classes (sixth and eighth grade) on one day and seventh graders on
the following day.
Data Analysis
All data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social
Sciences (SPSS). Relationships among variables were evaluated using
simple correlations. To ensure the reliability of the SIMS, the attitude
scale, and the LAPOPECQ, Cronbach's alphas were computed for all
questionnaire items. A series of four 2 (gender) by 3 (grade)
multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) with univariate follow-ups
were used determine if perceptions of the motivational climate,
attitudes, self-determination, and physical activity varied by gender
and grade level.
Results
Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach's alphas for the
variables in the study are reported in Table 1. From the descriptive
data, the physical education class climate tended to be perceived as
learning as opposed to performance. Analogous to the climate scores,
attitudes regarding the physical education classes were also relatively
positive. Inspection of the means suggests that intrinsic motivation and
identified regulation are more influential in terms of self-regulation
than external regulation and amotivation.
Relationships among Motivational Constructs
Relationships between variables were assessed using Pearson's
correlation coefficients. The correlation matrix for all variables is
reported in Table 2. Three sets of relationships were of interest in the
analysis of the data: perceptions of the motivational climate, levels of
self-determination, and attitude. Examination of the interrelationships
among these constructs provides a background for interpretation of the
relationships between them. Perceptions of a learning climate, with a
focus on learning and improvement, were unrelated to perceptions of a
performance climate, with a focus on outperforming others. The lack of a
relationship between these two variables suggests that students did not
necessarily perceive one type of climate in physical education over the
other. Within the levels of self-determination, intrinsic motivation,
and identified regulation were positively related, while amotivation was
negatively related to both intrinsic motivation and identified
regulation. In addition, amotivation was positively associated with
external regulation. These relationships are consistent with the
theoretical assumptions of self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan,
1985). There was also a strong positive correlation between enjoyment
and usefulness (r = .803), the two components of attitude.
How perceptions of the motivational climate relate to levels of
self-determination and attitudes was a primary research question for
this study. Perceptions of a learning climate were related to the
components of self-determination and attitude. Intrinsic motivation and
identified regulation were positively associated with perceptions of a
learning climate, while amotivation was negatively related to
perceptions of a learning climate. Though statistically significant, the
strength of these relationships was relatively weak. Perceptions of a
learning climate were also positively related to both the enjoyment (r =
.484) and usefulness (r = .516) constructs of attitude, and these
relationships were of moderate strength. Perceptions of a performance
climate were not related to levels of self-determination or attitude.
The examination of the relationships between self-determination and
attitude were also of interest. Intrinsic motivation and identified
regulation were both positively correlated with attitude enjoyment and
usefulness, while amotivation was negatively related to both attitude
constructs. Though statistically significant, the strength of these
relationships was relatively weak.
Activity Levels and Motivational Variables
Perceptions of the motivational climate, levels of
self-determination, and attitudes were generally not related to the
measures of physical activity. Specifically, attitude enjoyment had a
weak, but statistically significant positive association with the
pedometer count in physical education classes, while external regulation
had a weak, negative relationship with that measure. No other
significant relationships emerged. The data also revealed a positive
weak correlation between the physical education pedometer count and the
24-hour pedometer count. However, neither of the pedometer counts was
related to the self reported levels of physical activity as assessed by
the PAQ-C.
Gender and Grade Effects
Means by grade and gender are reported in Table 1. The MANOVA for
motivational climate yielded a significant main effect for grade
[Wilks' [lambda] = .911, F(4, 212) = 2.53, p = .041]. The main
effect for gender [Wilks' [lambda] = .963, F(2, 106) = 2.02, p =
.137] and the grade by gender interaction [Wilks' [lambda] = .969,
F(4, 212) = .85, p = .497] were not significant. Univariate follow-ups
revealed that 8th graders had lower perceptions of a learning climate
than 6th and 7th graders IF(2, 107) = 5.00, p = .008, ES = .54], who did
not differ from one another. Perceptions of a performance climate did
not differ by grade level.
The MANOVA for attitude also yielded a significant main effect for
grade level [Wilks' [lambda] = .900, F(4, 214) = 2.86,p = .026].
The main effect for gender [Wilks' [lambda] = .977, F(2, 106) =
1.26,p = .287] and the grade by gender interaction [Wilks' [lambda]
= .937, F(4, 212) = 1.75, p = .141] were not significant. Univariate
follow-ups revealed that sixth graders rated physical education as more
enjoyable [F(2, 107) = 5.55, p = .005, ES = .70], and useful [F(2, 107)
= 4.14, p = .014, ES =.60] than seventh and eighth graders, who did not
differ from one another.
It is important to highlight the differences in children who are in
6th, 7th and 8th grades from an age perspective. First, we know that
children ages 12-21 do not regularly participate in vigorous activity
and their participation rate in physical activity decreases by 50 to 75%
from kindergarten to 12th grade (Sallis, 1995). Trost, et al., (2002)
also found significant inverse relationships with both moderate and
vigorous physical activity and grade level. Sadly, age related changes
appear to be exacerbated in girls in that their physical activity
declines are even more drastic, even in structured settings such as
physical education classes (McKenzie, 2003). Much of the research that
attempts to explain these declines has focused on motivation issues in
physical activity (Parish & Treasure, 2003), while other research
explains the differences as a result of significant physiological and
psycho-social changes during this age (Ruffin, 2009).
For levels of self-determination, there were no significant effects
for grade [Wilks' [lambda] = .864, F(8, 208) = 1.97, p = .052],
gender [Wilks' [lambda] = .962, F(4, 104) = 1.04, p = .393], or the
grade by gender interaction [Wilks' [lambda] = .917, F(8, 208) =
1.146, p = .334], although the main effect for grade approaches
significance. Inspection of the means suggest there is a trend for 6th
graders to display lower levels of amotivation and external regulation
than 7th and 8th graders, but the differences between the groups do not
reach statistical significance.
The MANOVA for physical activity levels revealed significant main
effects for both grade [Wilks' [lambda] = .639, F(6, 194) = 8.13, p
< .000] and gender [Wilks' [lambda] = .852, F(3, 97) = 5.64, p =
.001 ]. The gender by grade interaction was not significant [Wilks'
[lambda]= .931, F (6, 194) = 1.17, p = .325]. Univariate follow-ups
revealed that while self-reported physical activity did not differ for
grade [F(2, 99) = .20, p = .823] or gender [F(1, 99) = 1.53, p = .219],
pedometer counts yielded both grade and gender effects. During physical
education classes [F(2, 99) = 26.88, p <.000], 6th graders took more
steps than 7th graders (ES = .91), who took more steps than 8th graders
(ES = .54). The 24-hour counts did not differ by grade [F(2, 99) = .182,
p =.834]. Girls took fewer steps than boys both during physical
education classes [F(1, 99) = 15.16, p < .000, ES = .69] and during
the 24-hour count [F(1, 99) = 5.13, p =.03, ES = .20] Girls averaged
2701 steps while boys averaged 3310 steps during physical education
classes. Over the 24-hour period, girls and boys took 10,095 and 11,988
steps respectively during physical education classes.
Discussion
The focus of this study was to explore relationships among
perceptions of the motivational climate, attitude, and
self-determination, and investigate how those factors interrelated to
affect student engagement in physical activity. The first research
question focused on how students' perceptions of the motivational
climate related to their attitudes and levels of self-determination.
Perceptions of a learning (task-oriented) climate were related to
positive attitudes regarding both the usefulness and enjoyment of
physical education and higher levels of self determined motivation. This
suggests that positive attitudes about engaging in activity in physical
education classes are fostered when a task-involved environment is
salient. When opportunities for students to progress at their own rate
are provided, student attitudes and motivation toward physical education
may improve.
One important finding in this study was that the perception of the
climate appeared to be a stronger influence on students' attitudes
than on their levels of self-determination. This finding is unique, as
other studies (Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Papaioannou, 1994; Parish
& Treasure, 2003) have reported a link between a mastery-oriented
climate and levels of self-determination, but have not considered how
attitudes are related to the climate.
The second research question examined how measures of perceptions
of the motivational climate, student attitudes, and self-determination
related to student levels of activity as measured by pedometer counts.
It is unclear why stronger relationships did not emerge for the physical
education and 24-hour pedometer counts in regard to the attitude,
climate and self-determination variables. Additional research with these
variables could provide valuable information in this regard.
An intriguing finding is that the pedometer counts have no
relationship to the self-report measure of physical activity. This poses
an interesting scenario and may lead one to question whether or not the
pedometer is a good measure of physical activity both in and outside of
physical education time. It is important to keep in mind that pedometer
counts in this study were compared across classes and activities. One
logical conclusion is that pedometer counts need to be obtained in a
similar environment for successful comparisons to be made.
The final research question was related to the ways in which
students' motivation and activity levels vary by grade level and
gender. The results with regard to grade level are consistent with
previous research documenting decreases in physical activity levels and
motivation during adolescence (Bauman et al., 2009; Parish &
Treasure, 2003). Perception of an emphasis on learning in the
motivational climate and students' attitudes exhibited a marked
decline from the sixth grade to the eighth grade. This was accompanied
by a decrease in the average number of steps taken during physical
education classes. Taken together, these findings suggest that this is a
critical period in the downward trend in both motivational constructs
and physical activity levels. Efforts to foster positive attitudes
toward physical education and to emphasize a focus on learning and
individual improvement in the motivational climate seem to be especially
important during the middle school years if teachers are to slow the
decline in physical activity levels during adolescence.
According to the pedometer counts, boys in this study were
significantly more active than girls. There are two potential
explanations for this finding. First, it is well documented (Bauman et
al., 2009) that activity levels decline faster and at an earlier age for
females than males and this finding is consistent with that
conceptualization. Second, it is possible that many of the physical
education activities at the middle school were stereotypically
"male" oriented activities. Soccer and capture the flag (using
a football) may have piqued the interest of males more so than females.
This finding is consistent with other studies that have used pedometers
to measure physical activity levels in children (Vincent & Pangrazi,
2002). Previous research using the PAQ-C (Kowalski et al., 1997) also
found boys to be more active than girls using the self-report measure,
but in this study those differences did not emerge.
The professional implications resulting from this study are
two-fold. First, students who feel and/or perceive that they are
involved in a task-oriented learning environment where the needs of all
children are addressed are more likely to have more positive attitudes
about physical education and physical activity. The importance of the
link between a climate that fosters task involvement and intrinsic
motivation cannot be overemphasized, especially in settings such as
physical education and physical activity where individuals are likely to
continue participation if they are intrinsically motivated to do so.
Characteristics of a task-oriented environment include emphasis on
mastering the task at hand, self-referenced evaluation (i.e.
improvement), emphasis on effort and zero gender inequity (i.e.
equipment, activities, etc.).
Second, it is critical that physical educators offer activities
that are appealing to both genders or, provide a choice of activities to
perhaps increase the likelihood of all students being more active during
physical education time. Providing choice in the physical education
class, whether in terms of activity selection, difficulty of task, or
other alternatives, fosters a sense of autonomy, one of the critical
components of self-determination, and decreases the sense of working or
learning in a controlled environment (Tessier, Sarrazin, &
Ntoumanis, 2008).
Applying Research to Practice
This study provides new evidence to support relationships between
self regulation, attitudes, and learning climates which has previously
been largely unexplored. Previously, there have been measurement issues
related to attitude instruments in physical education. To date, this is
the only study to use this instrument in conjunction with others that
are valid and reliable.
Researchers and teachers have begun to use pedometers as a measure
of engagement. The previous results were not strong and neither were the
results of this study in relation to pedometer counts. This raises an
issue regarding self-report data. Since the pedometers do not relate to
self-report data, this calls into question how well students can
reliably recall their levels of physical activity. Second, the weak
relationships between pedometer data and other variables contributes to
a growing body of evidence that other devices, such as accelerometers,
may provide greater insight and more valid and reliable data in future
research. Another possible limitation is that, since pedometer counts
had only low correlations with the other variables, one may question the
selection of activities used in the study. Bowling and fitness testing
are potentially low-step activities and may require a greater amount of
time "waiting" for the student.
While there were disparate types of activities offered in the
physical education classes (i.e. bowling and fitness testing), it is
critical that practitioners understand how to structure class activities
in order to engage all students. Most individuals seek activities in
which the feel competent and where they can experience success (Harter,
1978). Unfortunately, the traditional sports-based curriculums used in
many programs are too narrowly focused (Bryan, Johnson, & Solmon,
2004; Ennis, 1999; Santina, Solmon, Cothran, Loftus, &
Stockin-Davidson, 1998) and do not provide a motivational climate that
is appropriate for students who may not like sports, who do not feel
competent or successful, or those who are physically inactive and/ or
unfit. Further, an overemphasis on competition may especially alienate
females who often participate at a lower intensity in competitive
situations, thus exacerbating their inability to meet the physical
activity guidelines (Scruggs, Beveridge, & Watson, 2003). One of the
best strategies for ensuring student engagement is to provide a variety
of physical activities and allowing some level of autonomy where
students make choices regarding the activities in which they wish to
participate. Providing a wider selection of activities increases the
likelihood that students will find something they like that will keep
them physically engaged.
The ways in which physical educators structure their classes and
attempt to influence the climate of their class has a tremendous impact
on student response, attitude, and levels of participation. Those who
are responsible for teaching young people skills and knowledge in
physical education, as well as the fitness concepts important for a
lifetime must be cognizant of how their activity selection and
approaches may influence students toward or away from greater levels of
physical activity.
Specifically, the influence of the physical education climate on
students' attitudes is of paramount importance. The results of this
study, along with extending this line of inquiry for future research
will contribute to a growing body of research that may help guide
programs and physical educators to design classes which will elicit more
active, healthier students. Indeed, this is a promising area of
research, but much work remains to be done.
References
Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago:
Dorsey Press.
Ajzen, I. (1993). Attitude theory and the attitude behavior
relation. In D. Krebs & P. Schmidt (Eds.), New directions in
attitude measurement (pp. 41-57). New York: de Gruyter.
Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and
motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and
exercise (pp. 161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the
classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivation processes.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267.
Bauman, A., Bull, F., Chey, T., Craig, C. L., Ainsworth, B. E.,
Sallis, J. F., Bowles, H. R., Hagstromer, M., Sjostrom, M., & Pratt,
M. (2009). The international prevalence study on physical activity:
Results from 20 countries. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition
and Physical Activity, 6, 1-11.
Beighle, A., Pangrazi, R.P. & Vincent, S.D. (2001). Pedometers,
physical activity, and accountability. Journal of Physical Education,
Recreation and Dance, 72, 16-19.
Biddle, S.J.H. (1999). Motivation and perceptions of control:
Tracing its development and plotting its future in exercise and sport
psychology. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 1-23.
Biddle, S.J.H. (2001). Enhancing motivation in physical education.
In G. C. Roberts (Ed.) Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (2nd
ed., pp. 101-127). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Brustad, R.J. (1991). Children's perspectives on exercise and
physical activity: Measurement issues and concerns. Journal of School
Health, 61, 228-230.
Bryan, C. L., Johnson, L. J., & Solmon, M. A. (2004, April).
Children's attitudes toward physical activity and their perceptions
of fitness activities. Paper accepted for presentation at the annual
meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
Crocker, P. R. E., Bailey, D. A., Faulkner, R. A., Kowalski, K. C.,
& McGrath, R. (1997). Measuring general levels of physical activity:
Preliminary evidence for the Physical Activity Questionnaire for Older
Children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 29, 1344-1349.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Doyle, W. (1977). Paradigms for research on teacher effectiveness.
In L. S. Shulman fed.), Review of research in education (Vol. 5, pp.
163-198). Itasca, IL: Peacock.
Ennis, C. D. (1999). Creating a culturally relevant curriculum for
disengaged girls. Sport, Education & Society, 4, 31-49.
Ferrer-Caja, E., & Weiss, M. R. (2000). Predictors of intrinsic
motivation among adolescent students in physical education. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 267-279.
Goudas, M., & Biddle, S.J.H. (1994). Perceived motivational
climate and intrinsic motivation in school physical education classes.
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 241-250.
Graser, S. V., Pangrazi, R. P., & Vincent, W. J. (2009). Step
it up: Activity intensity using pedometers. Journal of Physical
Education, Recreation and Dance, 80, 22-25.
Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., & Blanchard, C. (2000). On the
assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The
situational motivation scale (SIMS). Motivation and Emotion, 24,
175-213.
Harter, S. (1978). Pleasure derived from optimal challenge and the
effects of extrinsic rewards on children's difficulty level
choices. Child Development, 49, 788-799.
Kowalski, K. C., Crocker, P. R. E., & Faulkner, R. A. (1997).
Validation of the physical activity questionnaire for older children.
Pediatric Exercise Science, 9, 174-186.
Le Masurier, G. C. (2004). Walk which way? ACSM's Health &
Fitness Journal, 8, 7-10.
McKenzie, T. L. (2003). Health-related physical education: Physical
activity, fitness, and wellness. In Silverman, S. J., & Ennis, C.D.
(Eds.) Student learning in physical education (2nd ed., pp. 207-226).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement
motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in
education: Volume 1. Student motivation (pp. 39-73). New York: Academic
Press.
Ntoumanis, N. (2002). Motivational clusters in a sample of British
physical education classes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3,
177-194.
Papaioannou, A. (1994). The development of a questionnaire to
measure achievement orientations in physical education. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 11-20.
Parish, L.E., & Treasure, D.C. (2003). Physical activity and
situational motivation in physical education: Influence of the
motivational climate and perceived ability. Research Quarterly for
Exercise and Sport, 74, 173-182.
Ruffin, N. (2009). Adolescent Growth and Development. Retrieved
June 2, 2011 from http://pubs.ext.vt.edu/350/350-850/350-850.html.
Ryan, R. M., Frederick, C. M., Lepes, D., Rubio, N., & Sheldon,
K. M. (1997). Intrinsic motivation and exercise adherence. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 335-354.
Sallis, J. F. (1995). A behavioral perspective on children's
physical activity. In L. W. Y Cheung & J. B. Richmond (Eds.), Child
health, nutrition, and physical activity (pp. 125-152). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Santina, B., Solmon, M. A., Cothran, D. J., Loftus, S. J., &
Stockin-Davidson, K. (1998). Patriarchal consciousness: Middle school
students' and teachers' perspectives of motivational
practices. Sport, Education & Society, 3, 181-200.
Scruggs, P. W., Beveridge, S. K., Eisenman, P. A., Watson, D. L.,
Shultz, B. B., & Ransdell, L. B. (2003). Quantifying physical
activity via pedometry in elementary physical education. Medicine &
Science in Sports and Exercise, 35, 1065-1071.
Scruggs, P. W., Beveridge, S. K., & Watson, D. L. (2003).
Increasing children's school time physical activity using
structured fitness breaks. Pediatric Exercise Science, 15, 156-169.
Silverman, S., & Subramaniam, P. R. (1999). Student attitude
toward physical education and physical activity: A review of measurement
issues and outcomes. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 19,
97-125.
Solmon, M. A. (2003). Student issues in physical education classes:
Attitude, cognition, and motivation. In Silverman, S. J., & Ennis,
C. D. (Eds) Student Learning in Physical Education (2nd ed., pp.
147-163). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Standage, M., & Treasure, D. C. (2002). Relationship among
achievement goal orientations and multidimensional situational
motivation in physical education. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 72, 87-103.
Standage, M., Treasure, D.C., Duda, J.L., & Prusak, K.A.
(2003). Validity, reliability, and invariance of the situational
motivation scale (SIMS) across diverse physical activity contexts.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 19-43.
Subramaniam, P. R., & Silverman, S. (2000). Validation of
scores from an instrument assessing student attitude toward physical
education. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 4,
29-43.
Tessier, D., Sarrazin, P., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). The effects
of an experimental programme to support students' autonomy on the
overt behaviours of physical education teachers. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 23, 239-253.
Treasure, D. C. (1997). Perceptions of the motivational climate and
elementary school children's cognitive and affective response.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 278-290.
Treasure, D. C. & Roberts, G. C. (2001). Students'
perceptions of the motivational climate, achievement beliefs and
satisfaction in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 72, 165-175.
Trost, S. G., Pate, R. R., Sallis, J. F., Freedson, P.S., Taylor,
W. C., Dowda, M. & Sirad, J. (2002). Age and gender differences in
objectively measured physical activity in youth. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 34, (2), 350-355.
Vincent, S.D., & Pangrazi, R.P. (2002). An examination of the
activity patterns of elementary school children. Pediatric Exercise
Science, 14, 432-441.
Welk, G. J., Differding, J. A., Thompson, R. W., Blair, S. N.,
Dziura, J., & Hart, P. (2000). The utility of the Digi-Walker step
counter to assess daily physical activity patterns. Medicine and Science
in Sports and Exercise, 32, S481-488.
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of
competence beliefs, expectancies for success, and achievement values
from childhood through adolescence. In A.
Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.) The Development of Achievement
Motivation (pp. 91120). New York: Academic Press.
Charity Leigh Bryan
University of Louisiana at Lafayette
Melinda A. Solmon
Louisiana State University
Address correspondence to: Charity Bryan, University of
Louisiana-Lafayette, Dept. of Kinesiology. 225 Cajundrome Blvd.
Lafayette, LA. 70506. Work: (337) 482-6282, Cell: (225) 241-3883, Email:
[email protected]
Table 1.
Means, Standard Deviations (in parentheses) and Cronbach's Alpha
Coefficients for Motivational Climate, Attitude, Level of
Motivation, and Physical Activity Level by Gender
Variable Boys Girls 6th 7th
n =57 n =57 n =30 n =30
LAPOPECQ, (1- 5 Likert)
Learning 3.29 3.34 3.58 (a) 3.40 (a)
climate (.69) (.69) (.67) (.44)
Performance 2.94 3.08 3.02 3.06
climate (.51) (.52) (.55) (.52)
Attitude (1-5 Likert)
Enjoyment 3.52 3.21 3.81 (a) 3.21 (b)
(.77) (.92) (.72) (.79)
Usefulness 3.45 3.16 3.65 (a) 2.24 (b)
(.66) (.86) (.66) (.70)
SIMS (1-7 Liken)
Intrinsic 4.21 4.20 4.60 3.92
Motivation (1.33) (1.43) (1.18) (1.34)
Identified 4.19 3.94 4.03 3.91
Regulation (1.41) (1.21) (1.30) (1.22)
External 3.83 4.05 3.40 4.18
Regulation (1.28) (1.43) (138) (1.06)
Amotivation 3.14 3.39 2.78 3.48
(1.29) (1.17) (1.37) (1.21)
Physical Activity
PE 3310.65 (1) 27012 (2) 3830.00 (a) 3019.46 (b)
pedometer (846.61) (823.04) (744.13) (563.44)
24-hour 11988.30 (1) 10095.48 (2) 11352.63 11229.62
pedometer (4735.60) (4106.73) (4587.20) (4563.13)
PAQ-C 1.17 1.11 1.13 1.21
composite (.46) (.46) (.40) (.55)
Variable 8th Total Cronbach
n =54 n =114 Alpha
LAPOPECQ, (1- 5 Likert)
Learning 3.12 (b) 3.32 .87
climate (.75) (.68)
Performance 2.99 3.01 .72
climate (.54) (.51)
Attitude (1-5 Likert)
Enjoyment 3.19 (b) 3.36 .91
(.89) (.86)
Usefulness 3.12 (b) 3.30 .87
(.83) (.78)
SIMS (1-7 Liken)
Intrinsic 4.19 4.20 .81
Motivation (1.44) (1.37)
Identified 4.16 4.06 .75
Regulation (1.38) (1.31)
External 4.10 3.93 .76
Regulation (1.42) (1.35)
Amotivation 3.42 3.27 .72
(1.10) (1.24)
Physical Activity
PE 2540.60 (c) 3005.93
pedometer (768.76) (885.77)
24-hour 10778.88 110050.9
pedometer (4525.81) (4515.27)
PAQ-C 1.10 1.14
composite (0.43) (0.46)
Mean in the same row with differing superscripts are
significantly different at the p <.05 level.
Table 2.
Correlations for Motivational Climate, Attitude, Level of
Motivation, and Physical Activity level
Variable 1 2 3 4 5
LAPOPEC
l. Learning
2. Performance .l0
Attitude
3. Enjoyment .48 * -.10
4. Usefulness .52 * -.14 .80 *
SIMS
5. Intrinsic Motivation .27 * .03 .37 * .30 *
6. Identified Regulation .25 * -.01 .21 * .22 * .66 *
7. External Regulation .14 .14 .01 -.05 -.07
8. Amotivation -.23 * .09 -.29 * -.33 * -.40 *
Physical Activity
9. PE count .07 .10 .18 * .07 .13
10. 24-hour count .03 .05 -.03 -.03 .13
11. PAQ-C .04 .10 .01 .09 .16
Variable 6 7 8 9 10
LAPOPEC
l. Learning
2. Performance
Attitude
3. Enjoyment
4. Usefulness
SIMS
5. Intrinsic Motivation
6. Identified Regulation
7. External Regulation -.06
8. Amotivation -.33 * .45 *
Physical Activity
9. PE count .01 -.19 * .01
10. 24-hour count .15 -.01 -.01 .28
11. PAQ-C .14 .00 -.09 .15 .16
* p <.05