An examination of sport fandom in the United Kingdom: a comparative analysis of fan behaviors, socialization processes, and team identification.
Parry, Keith D. ; Jones, Ian ; Wann, Daniel L. 等
Since Wann and Hamlet (1995) commented upon the lack of literature
focused upon the sport fan, there has been a substantial increase in
theoretical and empirical research on the subject, leading to
considerable advances in knowledge and understanding. There is still,
however, limited cross-cultural research, and according to Theodorakis
and Wann (2008), a need to explore cultural differences in fandom. This
is particularly true given that existing research indicates such
differences to be present in the motivations and behaviors of sport fans
and spectators from different ethnic groups (Bilyeu & Wann, 2002;
Wann, Bilyeu, Breenan, Osborn & Gambouras, 1999), as well as
different nationalities (Melnick & Wann, 2004, 2011; Theodorakis
& Wann, 2008). To date, a limited number of cross-cultural studies
have been undertaken. Wann, Melnick, Russell and Pease (2001) report
findings from data collected from North American fans, and further
empirical studies have tested Norwegian (Melnick & Wann, 2004),
Greek (Theodorakis & Wann, 2008), and Australian fans (Melnick &
Wann, 2011). The studies have all explored similar aspects of fandom: a)
fan related behavior (such as watching sport on television, b) the role
of various agents in the socialization process (such as parents or the
media), c) gender differences, and d) the extent of fan identification
with a favorite team. The following paragraphs highlight some of the
more pertinent findings from these research endeavors.
Fan Related Behavior
Findings on fan related behavior demonstrate--in most
cases--similarity across contexts, with males consistently demonstrating
greater frequencies than females for all types of behavior. Males from
all studies to date and Australian female fans demonstrate high levels
of television spectatorship, with 81% of Norwegian male fans, 86% of
Greek male fans, 97% of Australian male fans, and 86% of Australian
female fans watching sport at least once a week. Female television
viewing is lower for Norwegian (66%) and Greek female fans (34.8%).
Listening to sport on the radio is, in most cases, comparatively rare
when compared to television viewing. It is a much more popular activity
amongst Greek males, of whom 61% listen at least once a week, and the
entire Australian sample, where 59% of males and 38% of females listen
to sport at least once a week, compared to only 15.9% of Greek females,
16% of Norwegian males, and 11% of Norwegian females.
The increased frequency of fan behaviors among Australian fans is
also reflected in terms of live attendance at sporting events. Half of
male Australian fans (50%) and 31% of female fans attend sporting events
at least once a week according to Melnick and Wann's (2011) study,
compared with 31% and 10% respectively of Norwegian fans, and only 10.1%
of male and 4.5% of female Greek fans. All studies, however, demonstrate
the importance of discussing sport with others, with 97% of Australian
fans and 91% of Norwegian fans doing so at least once a week.
Theodorakis and Warm (2008) found that male Greek fans follow this
pattern, with 86.3% discussing sport once a week (however, only 54.7% of
Greek females did so). The final behavior explored was that of accessing
sport on the internet. This was only been undertaken by Melnick and
Wann's (2011) Australian study, which demonstrates relatively high
levels of internet use amongst Australian males, with 78% accessing
sport at least once a week, compared to 44% of females.
The Role of Various Agents in the Socialization Process
Each study highlights friends and peers as key socialization
agents. There is less consistency with regard to the role of parents,
school and community in the socialization process. The North American
data reported by Wann et al. (2001) highlights the importance of the
school, followed by parents, and finally community. Greek fans report
that parents and community are the second and third most important
agents. The variability between each, however, is low. Australian fans,
as with North American fans, also report the community as least
significant; however they rate all agents highly, with little variation
between them. Norwegian fans are shown by Melnick and Wann (2004) to be
most strongly influenced by the local community, and both Greek and
Norwegian fans cite the school as the least important socialization
agent reflecting, to some extent, the different sporting structures
within each context, such as the emphasis upon club sport in Greece and
Norway, as opposed to the emphasis upon school based sport in the United
States. In terms of the single most influential factor, all studies to
date highlight the importance of the father as a socialization agent.
The Greek and Australian studies both report 45% of respondents
suggesting that their father had been the most influential agent, with
30% of Norwegian fans citing this as the case.
The Extent of Identification with a Favorite Team
Previous cross-cultural research also targeted team identification,
which is the extent to which a fan feels a psychological connection to a
team (Wann et al., 2001). Levels of identification for Australian fans
have been found to be very high, with fans supporting an Australian team
scoring a mean of 40.11 (out of a possible 56) on the Sport Spectator
Identification Scale (Wann & Branscombe, 1993), and those following
a non-Australian team scoring 42.07. These are considerably higher than
Greek (M= 36.9) and Norwegian fans (M= 31). Australians also were more
likely to have a favorite team (92%), compared to 87.5% of Greek fans
and 65.2% of Norwegian fans. Most Greek fans supported a team based in
their homeland compared to 90% of Australian fans and only 64% of
Norwegian fans. As Melnick and Wann (2011) suggest, this may be due to
the absence of a professional sports industry in Norway, whereas both
Greece and Australia possess high profile professional sport teams.
The Current Investigation
Johnes (2005) suggests that the United Kingdom (UK) was the
birthplace of modern sport. Through the rationalization and codification
of folk games in the nineteenth century (a process Elias, 1986, terms
"sportization"), many of the sports that are played around the
world saw their birth in the UK. Yet despite this sporting tradition,
recent figures (Sport England, 2011) indicate that only slightly over
one third of the adult population participate in regular (once per week)
sporting activity. Although this percentage has risen since 2006, there
has been a decrease in the number of young people (aged 16-19) who
regularly participate in sport during the same period. Swimming,
association football, athletics, and cycling are the most participated
in sports as identified in the most recent (2010-11) survey of sports
participation rates amongst adults (Sport England, 2011). In July 2005,
London won the right to host the 2012 Olympic Games. The London 2012 bid
was backed by the Government as it was believed that hosting the Olympic
Games was one way to improve the physical and mental well-being of the
nation, with additional benefits such as urban regeneration and a
reduction in anti-social behavior (Hill, 2010).
However, although participation may be straggling, sport
spectatorship remains high in the UK. The sporting environment for fans
in the UK is dominated both in terms of attendance and media coverage by
the English Premier League (association football). The combined
attendance figures for top-flight English football teams were over
thirteen million in the 2011-12 season (ESPN, 2012). Elsewhere, crowds
at major sporting events appear to be increasing, with the flagship
tennis and motorsports events (Wimbledon and the British Formula One
Grand Prix) seeing record attendances in 2011 (PwC, 2011). Clearly,
sport fandom and spectating play an important role in the culture and
society of the UK and permeates many aspects of life.
The purpose of the current study was two-fold. First, we examined
sport fandom and spectating within the United Kingdom, replicating the
procedure used in the aforementioned cross-cultural examinations.
Second, we provide a comparison with existing studies based upon the
different cultures outlined above. Due to the lack of past research of
this type on fans in the UK, establishing specific hypotheses was not
appropriate (similar to the work by Melnick & Wann, 2004;
Theodorakis & Wann, 2008). Rather, the current investigation
attempted to examine the following research questions:
1. To what degree do male and female university students from the
United Kingdom engage in behaviors commonly associated with sport
fandom?
2. To what extent do parents, friends / peers, school and community
contribute to their socialization into the sport fan role?
3. Which one of the aforementioned socialization agents is most
influential?
4. How strongly do students identify with their favorite team?
Method
To facilitate comparison with previous studies, the methodology
adopted was based upon the previous cross-cultural studies discussed
above.
Participants
Participants were a convenience sample of 252 students based in the
Department of Sport at a provincial university in the UK. The gender
breakdown was 185 males (73.4%) and 67 females (26.6%). The mean age was
20.61 (SD = 3.26).
Measures
The questionnaire packet contained five sections. The first section
asked participants to provide demographic information regarding gender,
age, and year in school. In the second section participants indicated
how often they attended sporting events in person, watched sporting
events on television, listened to sporting events on the radio, and
discussed sport with friends and relatives. Response options to these
scale-items were: 1 = never, 2 = once a year, 3 = twice a year, 4 = once
a month, 5 = twice a month, 6 = once a week, 7 = twice a week, and 8 =
once a day.
The third section of the packet focused on fan socialization. Based
on and consistent with the work of McPherson (1975; 1976), Melnick and
Wann (2004) and Wann et al. (2001), four socialization agents were
identified and selected as having the greatest potential impact on the
sport fan socialization process: parents, friends, school and community.
Respondents expressed the impact of each agent on an 8-point Likert-type
scale anchored by 1 (the agent had no influence) to 8 (the agent had a
great deal of influence). Additionally, an open ended question was
included to allow participants to identify the most influential person
or entity in their decision to be a sport fan. This additional item was
justified due to the fact that forced-choice measures may be less
optimal in situations assessing sport fan socialization (see Spaaij
& Anderson, in press). Indeed, inconsistency between forced-choice
and open ended responses have been noted (e.g., Melnick & Wann,
2011).
The fourth section contained the Sport Fandom Questionnaire (SFQ;
Warm, 2002) to measure the degree of identification with the role of
sport fan. The SFQ is a reliable and valid unidimensional scale
comprised by five Likert-scale items. Response options ranged from 1
(strongly disagree) to 8 (strongly agree). Higher numbers represented
greater levels of sport fandom. The fifth and final section of the
packet focused on team identification. In this section, participants
completed the Sport Spectator Identification Scale (SSIS; Wann &
Branscombe, 1993). Respondents targeted their favorite team when
completing the SSIS. The SSIS contains seven Likert-scale items with
response options ranging from 1 (low identification) to 8 (high
identification).
Procedure
After receiving permission from class instructors, questionnaires
were completed by participants inside university classrooms. All
subjects were informed that their participation was completely
voluntary, participants could withdraw at any time, and the individual
responses would be held in strict confidence. Upon completion of the
study (approximately 20 minutes), the participants were debriefed and
excused from the testing session.
Results
Current Sample--Sport Fandom in the UK
Initial analyses. The five items on the SFQ were combined to form a
single measure of fandom (Cronbach's alpha = .90). Using a one-way
analysis of variance, males (M = 34.74, SD = 4.80) reported
significantly higher [F(l, 243) = 97.62,p < .001] levels of fandom
than females (M= 26.40, SD = 8.04). The seven items of the SSIS were
also combined (alpha = .92), with males (M= 44.01, SD = 8.91)
demonstrating significantly higher [F(1, 249) = 55.50, p < 0.001]
levels of identification than females (M = 33.90, SD = 11.00).
Sport fan related behavior. Males demonstrated greater frequency of
participation in behaviors than females, with the exception of attending
events in person (33% of females attended twice a week or more, compared
to 22% of males). Females were, however, also more likely to be less
regular attenders, with 36% attending less than once a month, compared
to 22% of males. Males tended to watch significantly more sport on
television, with over half (51%) watching every day, compared to only 8%
of females. This pattern was repeated for accessing sport on the
Internet, but not in terms of listening to sport on the radio (which was
less common for all). Gender differences were also evident in terms of
discussing sport, with 84% of males discussing sport every day, compared
to 33% of females.
The relationships among fandom behaviors, team identification and
sport fandom were examined using Spearman correlations. The findings
demonstrated significant positive correlations (all p < .01) between
team identification and all five fan behaviors (identification and
attendance r = .24; watching sport on television r = .47; listening to
sport on radio r = .36; accessing sport on Internet r = .43; discussing
sport r = .46). Similarly, significant positive correlations (all p <
.01) were found between sport fandom and the fan behaviors (fandom and
attendance r = .20; watching sport on television r = .49; listening to
sport on radio r = .36; accessing sport on Internet r = .54; discussing
sport r = .53).
Regression analyses were undertaken to further explore the
relationships among fandom, identification, and fan behavior. Each
regression used one of the behaviors as the dependent variable while
fandom and identification were predictor variables. Please see Table 1
for information regarding regression statistics for the predictor
variables. With respect to attendance at sporting events, the combined
effect of the two predictor variables was significant, F(2, 237) = 8.20,
p < .01 (R = 0.25; adjusted [R.sup.2] = 0.06). With respect to
independent contributions to attendance, team identification accounted
for a significant proportion of unique variance while sport fandom did
not. As for watching sport on television, the combined effect of the two
predictor variables was again significant, F(2, 240) = 64.13, p <
.001 (R = 0.59; adjusted [R.sup.2] = 0.34). With respect to independent
contributions to watching televised sport, both team identification and
sport fandom accounted for a significant proportion of unique variance.
The regression on listening to sport on the radio revealed that the
combined effect of the two predictor variables was significant, F(2,
240) = 24.51, p < .001 (R = 0.41; adjusted [R.sup.2] = 0.16). As for
independent contributions to sport radio listening, both identification
and fandom accounted for a significant proportion of unique variance.
With respect to accessing sport on the Internet, the combined effect of
the two predictor variables was significant, F(2, 241) = 64.80, p <
.001 (R = 0.59; adjusted [R.sup.2] = 0.34). With respect to independent
contributions to sport on the Internet, identification and fandom both
accounted for a significant proportion of unique variance. And finally,
for discussing sport, once again the combined effect of the two
predictor variables was significant, F(2, 241) = 75.16, p < .001 (p =
0.62; adjusted [R.sup.2] = 0.38). With respect to independent
contributions to sport discussions, both team identification and sport
fandom accounted for a significant proportion of unique variance.
Sport fan socialization. To examine the impact of the four
socialization agents, scores were analyzed using a 2 (Gender: male or
female) X 4 (Socialization Agent: parents, friends, school and
community) mixed factor analysis of variance. Means and standard
deviations appear in Table 2. Results indicated that the gender main
effect was significant (Wilks' Lambda = .934, F(4, 245) = 3.93,p
< .01). However, subsequent univariate tests indicated significant
gender differences only in terms of friends/peers, who were more
important for male fans F(1, 249)= 13.01, p < .001), with no gender
differences in terms of parents, school and local community. Table 3
reveals responses for the greatest influence on participants becoming a
sport fan. Fathers (49%) were, by some margin, the most significant
socialization agent, followed by friends (8%). Fewer females (33%)
identified their father as most influential, however.
Team identification, favorite team, and favorite sport. Wann et al.
(2001) suggest that a score of 35 or above on the SSIS can be indicative
of high identification. The mean score from the SSIS was 41.31, thus
demonstrating a very highly identified sample, especially among males (M
= 44.01). Females were, as noted earlier, significantly less identified
(M= 33.90). Most fans identified with an association football (soccer)
team (77%). Rugby union (10%), cricket (2%), basketball (2%) and
American football (1%) were also listed. Most respondents listed a
UK-based team (96% of those who listed a team), with only 4% listing a
non-UK team.
Comparison of British Sport Fans to Previous Samples
The results of the current study were compared with previous
cross-cultural studies (i.e., Melnick & Wann, 2004, 2011;
Theodorakis & Warm, 2008; Wann et al., 2001). Comparisons were drawn
between levels of sport fandom, team identification, sport fan
behaviors, and socialization agents (note that Wann et al., 2001, did
not examine levels of fandom and identification).
Sport fandom. An analysis of SFQ scores showed that British fans
appear to be more comparable with Australian fans rather than their
nearer neighbors in Europe (see Table 4). British fans had the highest
average total score, and the highest score from all groups was among
British male fans (the Norwegian sample reported the lowest total mean).
It is interesting to note, however, that female fans from Britain had a
mean score lower than those of Australian females (26.40 compared to
30.34). This finding may be linked to falling interest in sport in the
UK, with lower participation by females than males, a gender gap that
appears to become more pronounced with age (Cox, Coleman, & Roker,
2006). Further, a gender difference was found in each sample, with males
reporting higher levels of fandom than females.
Team identification. As shown in Table 4, British fans were highly
identified with their favorite teams. Although they identified more
strongly than their European counterparts, British female fans had a
lower mean score than Australian females, a finding consistent with the
aforementioned data on fandom. Also similar to fandom, British males had
the highest mean score on team identification and, once again, gender
differences were noted in each sample (although magnitude of the
difference was more pronounced in the British and Greek samples).
Sport fan behavior. British fans were more likely to watch sport in
person at least twice per week than their counterparts. British fans
were similar to most other samples in their high level of televised
sport consumption. Overall, 39% of British fans and 51% of males from
this sample reported watching sport on television on a daily basis.
These totals were the highest levels among all samples. With respect to
comparisons of listening to sport on the radio, subjects in the current
sample were less likely to do so on a weekly basis than those from
Greece and Australia. The proportion of British males who discussed
sport each day was considerably higher than the other cultures here with
71% of respondents indicating that they do so. Data for Internet use was
only available for Australian and British samples. It was clear that
British fans use the Internet to follow their teams to a greater extent
than fans in the Australian study.
Socialization into the sport fan role. With respect to category
rankings of the four primary socialization agents, an examination of
Tables 2 and 5 reveals that there were no exact matches between the
British sample and the other fan groups. However there were some
similarities between Australian and British fans for the importance of
parents and the importance of school for males. The importance of the
local community was lower for British and American fans.
In each sample, family members, and in particular fathers, were
most often listed as the primary socialization agent (see Table 3).
Generally, the results from this British study were more comparable to
those found in the Australian and United States samples than with the
other European studies. The key differences between the British sample
and those from Greece and Norway were that Greek fans reported more
friends as most influential while Norway had higher percentages from
friends and, especially, community.
Discussion
The current study reveals that British sport fans are highly
identified, typically with a British association football team, and that
they engage in fandom related behaviors more frequently than their North
American, European, and Australian counterparts. The data reflect the
strong sporting culture within the UK, particularly that in terms of the
cultural dominance of association football as a spectator sport. The
favorite sports selected by the UK sample is noteworthy as only one of
the four most popular participation sports for adults (association
football) provided the favourite sports team for the subjects. While
swimming, cycling, and athletics are popular participation sports they
did not feature in the list of favorite teams. However given the more
individual nature of these sports it may not be surprising that they
were not selected as a favorite.
It is interesting to note that, overall, British fans demonstrate
greater proximity to Australian fans than the two, geographically
closer, European nations. This may support the notion that a common
European culture remains more theory than practice (Schneider &
Barsoux, 2003). The proximity of behavior reflects Hofstede's
(1980) suggestion that British and Australian cultures are similar while
differing on a number of dimensions from both Norwegian and Greek
cultures. This seems reasonable given the colonial history of Australia
which, although Asian influences are becoming more dominant, is still
influential in Australian sport behavior (Cashman, 2010; Schirato, 2007;
Taylor et al. 2009). It has been documented that there are tangible
benefits from identifying with a sport team and that this identification
leads to greater levels of social interaction (Wann, 2006). As such, it
might be expected that this social comiection could result in increased
game attendance. However, sport fandom was not found to be a significant
predictor of attendance. This evidence reinforces the findings of Wann,
Brame, Clarkson, Brooks, and Waddill (2008) who, for the case of college
sports, found no interaction between game attendance and identification.
Higher levels of team identification for male fans was found to
result in increased indirect consumption through electronic media which
suggested that the social connections for highly identified male fans
may be formed through the communal watching of sport through electronic
media. The consumption of sport through the media was found to be more
common than attendance in person at games across all samples,
reinforcing Crawford's (2004) view that indirect sport consumption
is now the norm. The extent to which indirect consumption now dominates
the fan experience can be see through a comparison of the frequencies of
attending in person and watching sport on television. Across all samples
at least 70 % of participants watched sport at least once a week via
television, whereas, for attending a game this figure is below 50% and
it is in the American and Australian samples that this distinction is
most marked.
British fandom is also revealed to be somewhat male dominated.
Specifically, male fans were more highly identified, engaged in fan
related behaviors more often, and fathers were the most frequent
socializing agent into sport fandom. This perhaps reflects the dominance
of male football as a spectator sport within the UK (Jones, 2008).
However, comparison to previous samples reveals that the socialization
pattern is not confined to the UK. In all cultures, the father was the
most frequently cited socialization agent, indicating that gender
scripts [as discussed by Melnick and Wann (2004) in relation to their
Norwegian study] may be present across cultures and are witnessed in
Europe, North America, and Australia.
The apparent lack of importance placed upon community sport as a
socializing agent by UK participants is worthy of future consideration.
As was the case with American and Australian fans, community was ranked
as the least important of the four socialization agents by both males
and females (see Table 5). The lack of importance placed upon community
sport may be reflected in the sport structures of these countries where
large professional teams dominate the sporting landscape. The greater
emphasis placed on community sport (or the lesser emphasis on
professional sport) in Norway may be linked to their lower levels of
obesity (WHO, 2010) and may offer consideration for the UK in light of
their low sport participation figures (Sport England, 2011). The
frequency of attending sports events in person and watching sport on
television may also be of concern for the health of the UK population.
If the frequency of consuming sport is at the expense of sport
participation then, as this sample grow older (at present it is
relatively young with a mean of just over 20 years of age) it is likely
that their participation will decrease even further (Kokolakakis,
Lera-Lopez, & Panagouleas, 2011).
Summary and Limitations
The data have demonstrated both similarities and differences across
different cultures. Results revealed gender differences in behavior with
males reporting greater levels of participation than females. Both team
identification and fandom were significant predictors of fan behavior.
The importance of the father as a socialization agent was highlighted
throughout the sample, and levels of identification were generally high.
Cross-cultural analysis indicated that socialization agents for the UK
were more varied than other countries, UK fans were more likely to watch
sport live, and UK males were more likely to watch and discuss sport
daily. In general, it was demonstrated that UK fan behavior was closer
to that of American and, in particular, Australian fans, rather than
fans in European (Greek and Norwegian) samples.
Finally, a few limitations of the current research warrant mention.
First, as with several of the previous studies serving as comparisons
for the current investigation (e.g., Melnick & Warm, 2004, 2011;
Warm et al., 2001), the college student sample tested here was quite
homogeneous with respect to age and, perhaps, experience. That is,
although perhaps unlikely (Warm et al., 2001), it remains possible that
younger and older fans as well as fans without college experience may
respond in a different fashion (e.g., report a differential pattern of
fan behaviors and/or a different set of primary socialization agents).
Further, there were several important fan behaviors that were not
examined here, such as likelihood of purchasing team merchandise and
propensity to respond in an aggressive manner while watching sporting
events. Future investigators should add to the current work by expanding
the behaviors assessed. And finally, although the list of cross-cultural
analyses of fan behaviors and socialization processes continues to grow
(and now totals five countries), there are certainly many more locales
from which samples are needed to acquire a more complete picture of the
cultural differences and idiosyncrasies that impact sport fandom.
References
Bilyeu, J. K., & Wann, D. L. (2002). An investigation of racial
differences in sport fan motivation. International Sports Journal, 6,
93-106.
Cashman, R. (2010). Paradise of Sport: a history of Australian
sport. Petersham, N.S.W.: Walla Walla Press.
Cox, T., Coleman, L., & Roker, D. (2006). Understanding
participation in sport: What determines sports participation among 15-19
year old women? London: Sport England.
Crawford, G. (2004). Consuming sport: Fans, sport and culture.
London: Routledge.
Elias, N. (1986). An essay on sport and violence, In N. Elias &
E. Dunning, Quest for excitement: sport and leisure in the civilizing
process (pp. 150-174). Oxford: Blackwell.
ESPN, (2012). Barclays Premier League Stats - 2011-12. Available
from: http://soccemet. espn.go.com/stats/_/league/eng.
l/year/2011/barclays-premier-league?cc=3436
Hill, J. (2010). Sport in History: An Introduction, London:
Palgrave MacMillan.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's Consequences, London: Sage.
Johnes, M. (2005). United Kingdom. In D. Levinsen & K.
Christensen (eds.), Berkshire Encyclopaedia of World Sport Vol 4 (pp.
1653-1660). Great Barrington, USA: Berkshire Publishing
Jones, K. W. (2008). Female fandom: Identity, sexism, and
men's professional football in England. Sociology of Sport Journal,
25, 516-537.
Kokolakakis, T., Lera-Lopez, F. & Panagouleas, T. (2011).
Analysis of the determinants of sports participation in Spain and
England. Applied Economics, 44, 2785-98.
McPherson, B. (1975). Sport consumption and the economics of
consumerism. In D. W. Ball & J. W. Loy (Eds.) Sport and social
order: Contributions to the sociology of sport (pp. 243-275). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
McPherson, B. (1976). Socialization into the role of sport
consumer: A theory and causal model. Canadian Review of Sociology and
Anthropology, 13, 165-177.
Melnick, M. J., & Wann, D. L., (2004). Sports fandom
influences, interests, and behaviors among Norwegian university
students. International Sports Journal. 8, 1-13.
Melnick M. J., & Wann, D. L. (2011). An examination of sport
fandom in Australia: Socialization, team identification, and fan
behaviour. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 46, 456-470.
PwC, (2011). Attendances rise at UK's biggest annual sporting
events Available from http://
www.ukmediacentre.pwc.com/News-Releases/Attendances-rise-at-UK-s-biggest-annualsporting-events-lOdc.aspx.
Schirato, T. (2007). Understanding sports culture. London: Sage.
Schneider, S. C., & Barsoux, J. L. (2003). Managing across
cultures. Harlow: Pearson Education.
Spaaij, R., & Anderson, A. (in press). Parents or peers: Which
is it? Sport socialization and team identification in Australia: A
rejoinder to Melnick and Wann. International Review for the Sociology of
Sport.
Sport England, (2011). Once a week sport participation factsheet:
summary of results. Available from:
http://www.sportengland.org/research/active_people_survey/aps5.aspx.
Taylor, T., Lock, D., & Darcy, S. (2009). The Janus face of
diversity in Australian sport. Sport in Society 12, 861-875.
Theodorakis, N. D., & Wann, D. L. (2008). An examination of
sport fandom in Greece: influences, interests and behaviours.
International Journal of Sport Management and Marketing. 4, 356-374.
Wann, D. L. (2002). Preliminary validation of a measure for
assessing identification as a sport fan: the Sport Fandom Questionnaire.
International Journal of Sport Management, 3, 103-115.
Wann, D. L. (2006). Examining the Potential Causal Relationship
Between Sport Team Identification and Psychological Well-being. Journal
of Sport Behavior, 29, 79-95.
Wann, D. L., Brame, E., Clarkson, M., Brooks, D. & Waddill, P.
J. (2008). College Student Attendance at Sporting Events and the
Relationship Between Sport Team Identification and Social Psychological
Health. Journal of Intercollegiate Sports, 1, 242-254.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring
degree of identification with the team. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 24, 1-17.
Wann, D. L., Bilyeu, J. K., Breenan, K., Osborn, H. &
Gambouras, A. F. (1999). An exploratory investigation of the
relationship between sport fan motivation and race. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 88, 1081-1084.
Warm, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1995). Author and subject gender
in sports research. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26,
225-232.
Wann, D. L., Melnick, M. J., Russell, G. W. & Pease, D. G.
(2001). Sport Fans: The psychology and social impact of spectators. New
York: Routledge.
WHO, (2010). WHO Global Infobase. Available from:
https://apps.who.int/infobase/Comparisons.aspx.
Keith D. Parry
University of Western Sydney
Ian Jones
Bournemouth University
Daniel L. Wann
Murray State University
Address correspondence to: Keith Parry, School of Business,
University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith South DC, NSW
1797. Email:
[email protected]
Table 1
Regression Statistics for the Analyses Incorporating Sport
Fandom Behaviors as the Dependent Variables and Team
Identification (SSIS Scores) and Sport Fandom (SFQ Scores)
as the Predictor Variables.
Team
Identification Sport Fandom
Beta t Beta t
Attendance at events .19 2.4 * .07 0.8
Sport television viewing .31 4.5 *** .33 4.7 ***
Sport radio listening .27 3.5 *** .18 2.2 *
Sport on the Internet .24 3.6 *** .41 5.8 ***
Sport discussions .25 3.6 *** .43 6.3 ***
Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01., *** p < .001.
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for the Impact of the
Four Socialization Agents.
Socialization Sample
Agent British Greek Australian
Parents
All participants 5.67 (2.03) 4.36 (2.93) 5.72 (2.23)
Males 5.72 (2.00) 4.85 (2.94) 5.76 (2.18)
Females 5.54 (2.14) 3.16 (2.57) 5.66 (2.34)
Friends
All participants 5.67 (1.76) 4.47 (2.71) 6.01 (1.80)
Males 5.91 (1.69) 4.74 (2.74) 6.24 (1.58)
Females 5.01 (1.80) 3.81 (2.52) 5.65 (2.07)
Schools
All participants 5.33 (1.77) 4.26 (2.47) 5.52 (1.86)
Males 5.36 (1.72) 4.56 (2.46) 5.45 (1.79)
Females 5.27 (1.92) 3.55 (2.36) 5.63 (1.99)
Community
All participants 3.97 (1.89) 4.28 (2.57) 5.42 (2.00)
Males 4.09 (1.86) 4.53 (2.44) 5.65 (1.84)
Females 3.66 (1.94) 3.65 (2.78) 5.03 (2.19)
Socialization United
Agent Norwegian States *
Parents
All participants 4.67 (2.28) 4.69
Males 4.43 (2.30) 4.88
Females 4.84 (2.26) 4.52
Friends
All participants 4.97 (1.93) 5.47
Males 5.47 (1.89) 6.02
Females 4.62 (1.89) 4.99
Schools
All participants 3.82 (2.12) 5.14
Males 3.59 (1.98) 5.25
Females 3.99 (2.21) 5.04
Community
All participants 5.33 (2.01) 4.08
Males 5.45 (1.94) 4.30
Females 5.25 (2.15) 3.88
Notes: Response options ranged from 1 (the agent had no
influence) to 8 (the agent had a great deal of influence).
Standard deviations appear in parentheses next to each mean.
* Wann et al. (2001) did not report standard deviations for
the US sample.
Table 3
Percentages of Sport Socialization Agents Listed as Most Influential.
British Greek
Agent All M F All M F
Father 49 55 33 39 49 34
Mother 4 2 10 0 0 0
Both parents 2 2 5 0 0 0
Brother 6 5 8 6 5 6
Sister 2 1 5 0 0 0
Siblings 0 0 0 0 0 0
Grandfather <1 1 0 3 4 0
Grandmother 0 0 0 0 0 0
Husband 0 0 0 0 0 0
Uncle 2 3 0 5 6 4
Cousin <1 1 0 2 2 2
Family total 66 70 61 55 66 46
Friend 8 10 3 23 22 37
Boyfriend <1 0 2 0 0 0
Friend total 9 10 5 23 22 37
School 7 4 15 0 0 0
Teacher 2 2 2 0 0 0
School total 11 6 17 0 0 0
Community <1 1 0 0 0 0
Mass media 2 3 2 1 <1 1
Coach 2 1 3 2 1 4
Sport team 1 1 0 1 2 0
Famous player 6 8 4 0 0 0
Myself 0 0 0 4 3 9
Sample Norwegian
Australian
Agent All M F All M F
Father 45 53 34 30 28 33
Mother 4 1 10 3 0 5
Both parents 0 0 0 2 0 3
Brother 8 6 11 5 10 0
Sister 1 0 2 3 0 5
Siblings 0 0 0 1 0 2
Grandfather 3 2 5 0 0 0
Grandmother 1 1 0 0 0 0
Husband 0 0 0 1 0 2
Uncle 2 3 0 0 0 0
Cousin 1 0 2 1 2 0
Family total 65 66 64 46 50 50
Friend 7 6 8 13 14 12
Boyfriend 1 0 2 2 0 3
Friend total 8 6 10 15 14 15
School 7 5 10 9 6 10
Teacher 1 0 3 0 0 0
School total 8 5 13 9 6 10
Community 1 2 0 22 26 19
Mass media 3 3 2 7 12 3
Coach 1 1 0 1 2 0
Sport team 0 0 0 0 0 0
Famous player 10 14 5 0 0 0
Myself 1 0 2 0 0 0
United States
Agent All M F
Father 35 39 31
Mother 5 3 7
Both parents 1 0 4
Brother 8 11 5
Sister 2 1 2
Siblings <1 0 <1
Grandfather 1 2 1
Grandmother <1 <1 <1
Husband 1 0 2
Uncle 2 2 2
Cousin 1 <1 1
Family total 57 59 56
Friend 9 10 7
Boyfriend 4 0 7
Friend total 13 10 14
School 12 8 15
Teacher <1 0 <1
School total 12 8 16
Community 2 2 2
Mass media 5 7 4
Coach 2 3 1
Sport team 0 0 0
Famous player 1 1 <1
Myself 1 1 1
Notes: Numbers are percentages rounded to the nearest whole number.
<1 = .1% to .4%. Columns do not total 100% due to responses
classified as "other" and persons failing to provide a response.
Table 4
Means and Standard Deviations for Sport Fandom (SFQ Scores)
and Team Identification (SSIS Scores).
Sample
British Greek
Sport Fandom
All participants 32.53 (6.89) 27.66 (3.96)
Males 34.74 (4.80) 30.29 (6.77)
Females 26.40 (8.04) 21.75 (9.41)
Team Identification
All participants 41.31 (10.50) 36.91 (11.82)
Males 44.01 (8.91) 40.203 (10.18)
Females 33.90 (11.00) 29.91 (12.21)
Australian Norwegian
Sport Fandom
All participants 32.35 (7.82) 26.32 (N/A)
Males 33.58 (6.83) 28.50 (7.40)
Females 30.34 (8.90) 24.80 (8.90)
Team Identification
All participants 40.31 (8.52) 30.50 (N/A)
Males 41.19 (8.05) 33.80 (9.80)
Females 38.74 (9.17) 27.20 (10.20)
Notes: Wann et al. (2001) did not report fandom and
identification scores for their United States
sample. N/A = data not available.
Table 5
Rank Order of the Impact of the Four Socialization Agents.
Socialization Sample
Agent
British Greek
All M F All M F
Parents 1 2 1 2 1 4
Friends 2 1 3 1 2 1
Schools 3 3 2 4 3 3
Community 4 4 4 3 4 2
Socialization Sample
Agent
Australian Norwegian
All M F All M F
Parents 2 2 1 3 3 2
Friends 1 1 2 2 1 3
Schools 3 3 3 4 4 4
Community 4 4 4 1 2 1
Socialization Sample
Agent
United States
All M F
Parents 3 3 3
Friends 1 1 2
Schools 2 2 1
Community 4 4 4