首页    期刊浏览 2024年12月04日 星期三
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Exploring the shame coping experiences of youth sport parents.
  • 作者:Partridge, Julie A. ; Wann, Daniel L.
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Sport Behavior
  • 印刷版ISSN:0162-7341
  • 出版年度:2015
  • 期号:August
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:University of South Alabama
  • 摘要:The definition of shame has been extensively debated in the psychological literature (Tangney& Dearing, 2002). While shame is universally considered to be a negative emotion, views on its cause, characteristic intensity, and the degree to which it is adaptive or maladaptive vary. Tangney and Dearing (2002), following H. B. Lewis (1971), believe that shame is elicited by a focus on the self as "bad", and is an apparently global, stable attribution. Due to the nature of shame attributions (i.e., the entire self is bad), shame is viewed as an intensely negative experience and shame-proneness is viewed as quite maladaptive.
  • 关键词:Coping (Psychology);Parent and child;Parent-child relations;Teenage athletes;Teenagers;Youth

Exploring the shame coping experiences of youth sport parents.


Partridge, Julie A. ; Wann, Daniel L.


Shame is conceptualized as a negative, self-conscious family of emotions that results from feelings of devaluation (Elison, 2005; Nathanson, 1992; Tomkins, 1963). When an individual perceives being seen as "less than" by others (whether this perception is accurate or not), the emotions of shame, embarrassment, or humiliation can result. Youth sport is a public (and widely valued) forum through which children are provided opportunities to compare their physical skills and abilities with others and is thus a likely site at which shame and embarrassment may be experienced.

The definition of shame has been extensively debated in the psychological literature (Tangney& Dearing, 2002). While shame is universally considered to be a negative emotion, views on its cause, characteristic intensity, and the degree to which it is adaptive or maladaptive vary. Tangney and Dearing (2002), following H. B. Lewis (1971), believe that shame is elicited by a focus on the self as "bad", and is an apparently global, stable attribution. Due to the nature of shame attributions (i.e., the entire self is bad), shame is viewed as an intensely negative experience and shame-proneness is viewed as quite maladaptive.

How Do We Cope with Shame?

All people experience feelings of shame and embarrassment at one time or another, but may cope with shame very differently. A constructive coping style is to attend to the antecedent cause of the shame, evaluate whether or not one cares to address it, and then take steps to make changes necessary to prevent the shame from reoccurring. For example, a person who is ashamed of a poor grade in a class might choose to work harder on their assignments to perform better in the future. However, Nathanson (1992) suggested that people do not typically respond to shame in an adaptive manner and thus identified four distinct maladaptive coping styles commonly used in response to shame. Each family of coping styles is associated with different motivations, affects, cognitions, behaviors, and outcomes and characterizes the styles by which an individual attempts to reduce or ignore the negative emotional experience of shame without actually addressing its source. Nathanson's (1992) model describes four families of maladaptive shame-focused coping styles, labeled Withdrawal, Attack Self, Attack Other, and Avoidance.

At the Withdrawal pole, the person acknowledges the experience as negative, acknowledges that the shame is a valid (i.e., deserved) emotion, and tries to withdraw or hide from the situation (Elison, Pulos, & Lennon, 2006; Nathanson, 1992). For instance, a young athlete might ask to quit the basketball team after a shame-inducing performance. At the Attack Self pole, the person acknowledges the experience as negative and also accepts the shame as a valid experience, but in this case, turns anger inward. For example, a young baseball player might feel self-directed rage for being a "loser" after dropping a simple fly ball. Withdrawal and Attack Self share two important aspects, recognition of the negative experience and conscious acceptance (i.e., internalization) of shame's message that the "self' is inadequate (Partridge & Elison, 2009). An important distinction between Withdrawal and Attack Self is the motivation behind each strategy. Withdrawal leads people to pull away from others in order to reduce their discomfort and shame experiences, while individuals using Attack Self often endure shame to preserve relationships with others (i.e., acknowledge their own deficiencies to maintain important social connections).

The other two coping styles described by Nathanson (1992) utilize more extemalizations of the negative shame experience. In Avoidance, a person typically does not acknowledge the negative experience of the self, typically does not accept shame's message as valid (i.e., denies its existence), and attempts to distract the self (and others) from the painful feeling. For example, an athlete might joke about missing important free throws or disavow interest in basketball. Finally, at the Attack Other pole, the person may or may not acknowledge the negative experience of self, typically does not accept shame's message, and attempts to make someone else feel worse (e.g., by teasing someone else or externalize the shame by blaming an opponent for cheating). Support for the coping styles proposed by Nathanson have been found in college-age students (Elison, Lennon, et al., 2006; Elison, Pulos, & Lennon, 2006), as well as sport fans (Partridge, Warm, & Elison, 2010) and collegiate athletes (Elison & Partridge, 2012).

Vicarious Shame

Shame is considered to be a self-directed emotional response; however, there is growing evidence to suggest that shame can also be experienced vicariously through association with others (Lickel, Steele, & Schmader, 2011). The actions of an identified in-group may have an impact on the emotional responses of individuals who identify with that social group, and groups with which an individual identifies most strongly are more likely to result in stronger emotional reactions (Branscombe & Doojse, 2004; Johns, Schmader, & Lickel, 2005). Johns and colleagues (2005) found that stronger levels of identification as an American predicted higher levels of shame and higher desire to distance oneself from the group when reacting to a very negative event (i.e., prejudice toward people of Middle Eastern descent following the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001). Similarly, Lickel, Schmader, Curtis, Seamier, and Ames (2005) found that level of vicarious shame was predicted by a shared identity with a wrongdoer, an appraisal of a self-image threat, and a motivation to distance oneself from the event.

One important form of identification that exists in sport is the relationships between parents and children. Parents have been found to be susceptible to experiencing levels of vicarious shame in response to their children's negative or incorrect behaviors. Furthermore, parents have been found to exhibit specific responses to experiencing vicarious shame. Seamier, Schmader, and Lickel (2009) found that parents reported higher levels of shame response to vignettes involving a child's wrongdoing when a critical observer was present compared to the absence of this critical observer.

Sport Parent Shame

Parents have been found to exert a significant amount of influence over youth sport participants, particularly in terms of the child's perceptions of their own competence, and therefore beliefs about their likelihood of future success in sport (Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois, Sarrazin, Brustad, Trouilloud, & Cury, 2002; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Horn & Weiss, 1991), enjoyment of the activity (Babkes Stellino, Partridge, & Moore, 2012; Bois, Lalanne, & Delforge, 2009; McCarthy, Jones, & Clark-Carter, 2008), and feelings of anxiety (Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois et al., 2009; Brustad, 1988). The majority of this research on parental influence has only focused on how parents impact their children, while largely ignoring the reciprocal effect that children may have on the parent. Dorsch, Smith, and McDonough (2009) examined the reciprocal nature of these relationships and found that youth sport parents are socialized in a number of ways (behaviorally, cognitively, affectively, and in relationships). From an emotional standpoint, parents reported feelings of emotional connection to sport, utilizing emotional management strategies, and experiencing reactive emotional experiences in the youth sport setting (including feelings of shame, embarrassment, and guilt over a child's performance). Moreover, behavioral, cognitive, affective and relationships issues were reported to be highly interrelated, with experiences in one area frequently impacting their responses in another (e.g., reactive emotional experiences could lead to specific behaviors).

It seems clear that more investigation of parental emotional responses to their child's participation is needed to understand this complex relationship. Such an investigation was the focus of the work research. However, because of insufficient prior work in the area of shame coping among sport parents, the development of specific hypotheses was not warranted. That is, although past research had certainly investigated the process of shame coping generally (Elison et al., 2006; Nathanson, 1992) and more specifically within sport settings (Elison & Partridge, 2012; Partridge et al., 2010) investigators had yet to examine shame coping strategies among youth sport parents. Parents often report particularly high levels of identification with their child athletes and this identification can facilitate a number of responses, including those of an abusive or hostile nature (Wann, 2014). There is reason to believe that the reactions of sport parents may be different from other sport groups (e.g., athletes, fans without a familial relationship with the player). In addition, prior work on shame coping among fans targeted shame reactions to team performances (Partridge et al., 2010) while the current work was focused on reactions to a single player. Therefore, past research, although relevant, was not appropriate for developing specific hypotheses due to the critical differences in the participants (parents versus other sport participants) and the shame target (player versus team). Consequently, the current study examined two research questions, 1) What maladaptive shame coping styles are most frequently endorsed by youth sport parents, and 2) What variables account for the greatest amount of unique variance in maladaptive shame coping styles of youth sport parents. We are not suggesting that shame is the only (or even primary) emotional response exhibited by highly identified parents in response to viewing their child in competition. Rather, research indicates that these persons report many different affective responses (Sloan & Van Camp, 2008). This includes both positive (e.g., happiness, satisfaction, and confidence) and negative emotions (e.g., anger, discouragement, and tension) (Sloan, 1989; Wann, Dolan, McGeorge, & Allison, 1994). However, shame as an emotional response had yet to be empirically examined and thus, this research was warranted.

Method

Participants

Participants were 218 youth sport parents (M age = 40.2 years; SD =8.14) who volunteered to take part in the study. There were 49 (22.7%) males and 167 (77.3%) females (two participants did not report gender and thus were not included in the gender analyses). The sample was obtained from the Midwest region of the United States and included parents whose children participated in a total of 15 different sports (swimming, gymnastics, baseball, softball, soccer, tennis, basketball, dance, volleyball, golf, cheerleading, football, track, martial arts, and equestrian). The parents had an average of 2.4 children (SD = .76) participating in sport, however, participants were asked to consider the child with the most experience in sport when completing the surveys.

Materials

Compass of shame scale-parent. The Compass of Shame Scale (CoSS; Elison, Lennon et al., 2006; Elison et al., 2006) was originally designed to assess the four distinct maladaptive shame coping styles described by Nathanson (1992) and has been found to exhibit good psychometric properties. It was previously modified for sport situations (CoSS-Sport; Elison & Partridge, 2012). For this study, the CoSS-Sport was adapted to target the responses of parents to their child's sporting events. The CoSS-Sport consists of eight situational scenarios (i.e., items) with four corresponding response statements that are aligned with each pole of Nathanson's model: Attack Other (AO), Attack Self (AS), Avoidance (AV), and Withdrawal (WD). A fifth response, Attack Self-Child (AS-C) was created for this study to address the possibility that parents might Attack Self through a negative response directed at themselves in terms of their child's behaviors or abilities. For instance, a parent might observe their child making an error in the field during a game and respond to any resulting embarrassment by berating him or herself for not working with the child more on their fielding. Each of the five response options to each of the eight items utilizes a Likert-type response scale of 1 = never, 2 = seldom, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, and 5 = almost always. A sample CoSS-Parent item reads, "When someone points out my child's weaknesses...". Response options to this item are, "I just want to be by myself' (Withdrawal), "I want to point out other players' faults" (Attack Other), "I deny there is any reason for me to feel bad" (Avoidance), "I tend to obsess over these weaknesses" (Attack Self), and "It makes me feel like my child is less worthy" (Attack Self-Child). Total possible subscale scores range from 8 (low use of the coping style) to 40 (high use of the coping style). Internal consistency using Cronbach's alpha reliability procedure revealed that each scale possessed adequate reliability (AS = .79; AO = .80; WD = .83; AV = .68; AS-Child = .80).

It is important to note that the CoSS was designed to measure only maladaptive responses to shame. An individual who copes adaptively with shame can report low scores on each of the CoSS subscales, thus the questionnaire does not imply that any individual is maladaptively coping with shame. Furthermore, the responses to the stem statements do not necessarily require acknowledgement of shame, which means that individuals are not assumed to have any inherent level of shame present (Borg et al., 1988). Rather than inquiring about shame directly, responses describe related feelings and behaviors such as getting angry with others, being self-critical, or making jokes.

Sport spectator identification scale. The Sport Spectator Identification Scale (SSIS; Warm & Branscombe, 1993) is a 7-item, Likert-type scale measuring the degree to which an individual identifies with a specific team or athlete. Participants were asked to identify their favorite sport team by writing it on a line at the top of the SSIS. Participants could identify any team from any sport at any level, or an individual in an individual sport such as auto racing or figure skating. All questions on the subsequent questionnaires pertained to the identified team or athlete. A sample item from the SSIS is, "How important is it to YOU that the team listed above wins?" Possible responses ranged from 1 (low identification) to 8 (high identification). Therefore, higher numbers indicate greater levels of spectator identification. The SSIS has been found to be a highly valid and reliable measure (see Wann et al. 2001, for a review). Internal consistency for the SSIS was [alpha] = .76.

Dysfunctional fandom scale. The Dysfunctional Fandom Scale (DFS; Wakefield & Wann, 2006) is a 5-item, Likert-type scale designed to measure an individual's level of dysfunctional fandom. A sample item from the DFS is, "I have had confrontations with others while watching the team listed above when I voiced my opinion." Responses range from 1 (inaccurate) to 8 (accurate). Higher numbers on the DFS indicate higher levels of dysfunctional fandom. The internal consistency of the DFS was [alpha] = .79.

Sport fandom questionnaire. The Sport Fandom Questionnaire (SFQ; Wann, 2002) is a 5-item, Likert-type scale questionnaire that assesses the level of general sport fandom that an individual possesses. An example item from the SFQ is: "I consider myself to be a sport fan." Statement responses range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 8 (strongly agree). Higher scores on the SFQ reflect greater levels of fandom. The internal consistency for the SFQ was [alpha] = .92.

Procedure

Approval for the study was obtained from the Human Subjects Committee at the first author's university. Youth sport league administrators gave approval for the researchers to approach parents about taking part in the study. Individuals who agreed to participate provided signed consent and then completed a questionnaire packet containing a demographics questionnaire, a measure of trait shame coping (Compass of Shame Scale-Parent), and three measures of fandom (Sport Spectator Identification Scale, Dysfunctional Fandom Scale, Sport Fandom Questionnaire). The measures were given to participants in a random order to avoid any ordering effects. Upon completion of the questionnaire packet (approximately 20 minutes), participants were thanked for their cooperation, debriefed, and released from the testing session.

Results

Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. To examine Research Question 1 ("What maladaptive shame coping styles are most frequently endorsed by youth sport parents?"), a series of paired-sample t-tests (df = 185) were used to compare the average shame coping style scores among sport parents (see Table 1). A Bonferroni test was used to adjust for inflated Type I error. Results indicated that the Avoidance subscale was significantly higher than the other four coping subscales (Attack Self, Attack Other, Withdrawal, and Attack Self-Child). The Attack Self and Attack Other subscales did not differ from each, but both were significantly higher than Withdrawal.

Correlations were calculated using a Pearson product moment correlation and are shown in Table 2 (gender was coded as males = 1; females = 2). Because gender was found to be significantly correlated with certain variables (i.e., team identification and AS), gender was incorporated into the regression analyses reported below.

To examine Research Question 2 ("What variables account for the greatest amount of unique variance in maladaptive shame coping styles of youth sport parents"?), five separate linear regression analyses were performed, one each for the five shame coping subscales (AS, AO, AV, WD, and AS-Child). Team identification, dysfunctional fandom, sport fandom, parent gender, and child's perceived ability in their primary sport were identified as predictor variables while the shame coping strategies were the dependent variables.

Attack Other (AO)

The combined effect of the five predictor variables was significant, F(4, 165) = 6.09, [R.sup.2] = .14, p < .001. With respect to independent contributions to AO scores, dysfunctional fandom (t = 3.62, p < .01, p = .28) and team identification (t = 2.07, p < .05, [beta] = .18) accounted for a significant amount of unique variance. Therefore higher scores on dysfunctional fandom and team identification predicted higher AO scores. The other variables (i.e., sport fandom, gender, and child's perceived ability) did not account for a significant proportion of unique variance in AO scores (sport fandom t = -.30, p > .05, [beta] = -.03; parent gender t = .79, p > .05, P = .06; child's perceived ability t = -1.09, p > .05, [beta] = -.09).

Attack Self (AS)

The combined effect of the five predictor variables was significant F(5, 164) = 2.41, [R.sup.2] = .07, p < .05. With respect to independent contributions to AS scores, dysfunctional fandom (t = 2.45, p < .05, [beta] = .20) and parent gender (t = -2.43, p < .05, [beta] = -.19) accounted for a significant amount of unique variance. This indicates that higher levels of dysfunctional fandom and being a male were predictive of higher AS scores. Team identification (t = .41, (3 = .04), sport fandom (t = -.42, [beta] = -.04), and child's perceived ability (t = -.38, [beta] = -.32) did not account for significant amounts of unique variance.

Attack Self-Child (AS-Child)

The combined effect of the five predictor variables was significant, F(5, 164) = 4.41, [R.sup.2] = .12, p < .01. With respect to independent contributions to AS-Child scores, dysfunctional fandom (t = 3.96, p < .01, [beta] = .31) and child's perceived ability (t = -1.88., p < .05, [beta] = -. 15) accounted for a significant amount of unique variance. Therefore, higher levels of dysfunctional fandom and lower perceptions of the child's ability were predictive of higher AS-Child scores. Team identification (t = .51, [beta] = .05), sport fandom (t = -.43, [beta] = -.04), and parent gender (t = -.75, [beta] = -.54) did not account for significant amounts of unique variance.

Avoidance (AV)

The combined effect of the five predictor variables was not significant, F(5, 163) = 1.25, [R.sup.2] = .04, p > .05. Team identification (t = .98, [beta] = .09), dysfunctional fandom (t = 1.72, [beta] = .14), sport fandom (t = -.47, [beta] = -.04), parent gender (t = .67, [beta] = .05), and child's perceived ability (r = -.17, [beta] = -.01) were not significant predictors of AV shame coping strategies.

Withdrawal (WD)

The combined effect of the five predictor variables was significant F(5, 162) = 1.93, [R.sup.2] = .06, p < .10. With respect to independent contributions to WD scores, only dysfunctional fandom (t = 2.29, p < .05, [beta] = .19) accounted for a significant amount of unique variance, and indicated that higher levels of dysfunctional fandom predicted higher WD scores. Team identification (t = -.20, [beta] = -.02), sport fandom (t = .37, [beta] = .03), parent gender (t = -.93, [beta] = .07), and child's perceived ability (t = 1.53, [beta] = .13), were not significant predictors of WD shame coping strategies.

Discussion

Shame is an emotion that is typically conceptualized as an experience that occurs when one is devalued, or seen as "less than" in front of others (Elison, 2005), and may occur vicariously through identification with a secondary group or individual (e.g., a child). The current study sought to expand our understanding of the experience of vicarious shame in sport parents, including the most frequently used parental coping styles, as well as factors that may contribute to the use of these coping mechanisms in youth sport parents. There were two research questions in this study: 1) What maladaptive shame coping styles are most frequently endorsed by youth sport parents, and 2) What variables account for the greatest amount of unique variance in maladaptive shame coping styles of youth sport parents?

Paired samples t-tests indicated that all five measured coping styles were significantly different from one another (AV > AS > AO > AS-C > WD). Avoidance is an externalization technique that is used to protect the individual against several negative psychological symptoms (e.g., depression, hostility, anger and psychopathy) in dealing with shame (Campbell & Elison, 2005; Elison, Lennon, et al., 2006; Elison, Pulos, et al., 2006; Partridge & Elison, 2009; Yelsma, Brown, & Elison, 2002). Furthermore, the motivation in Avoidance behaviors is to minimize the conscious experience of shame, or to show oneself to be above the experience of being ashamed. Avoidance behaviors would serve such an externalizing, protective function, although none of the measured variables were found to be significant predictors of this coping style within the sample. This finding is also consistent with previous research on vicarious fan shame (Partridge et al., 2010). Similarly, a Withdrawal coping style has been found in previous samples to be the least commonly endorsed style overall, which is consistent with results of the current study.

Results from the t-tests also indicated that there were significant differences between scores on each of the other coping styles (i.e., Attack Self, Attack Other, and Attack Self-Child). Each of these "attacking" coping styles contain a common anger component, although they have different targets (i.e., internal or external). Seamier et al. (2009) found that the presence of others elevated levels of vicarious parental shame stemming from a child's behavior. Furthermore, shame and guilt were predictive of maladaptive disciplinary responses (i.e., overreactivity). Given the visibility of sport, even at the youth levels, parents may be more likely to react with anger if their child's performance causes shames and is seen as a reflection of their own behavior. Unfortunately, this anger can have significant implications on psychological health. Attack Self has been found to have a significant relationship with negative psychological symptoms such as depression, while Attack Other was significantly correlated with hostility, anger and psychopathy (Campbell & Elison, 2005; Elison, Lennon, et al., 2006; Elison, Pulos, et al., 2006; Yelsma, Brown, & Elison, 2002).

Consistent with previous research (Massey & Partridge, 2010; Partridge et al., 2010), the least commonly utilized coping style was Withdrawal, which is an internalization technique in which an individual is aware of the shameful experience and attempts to avoid other individuals as well as discussion/coverage of the shameful event. Withdrawal behaviors in the context of a child's sport experience could include refusal to discuss the sporting event following a particularly embarrassing loss, or refusing to talk about the game with other parents or coaches. However, the highly public and social nature of youth sport (i.e., expectations placed upon parents to attend children's games and to discuss the games with others), may be a contributing factor in why Withdrawal coping mechanisms are more difficult to engage in than the other forms of shame coping.

Results from the regression analyses indicated that a significant amount of variance in four of the five coping subscales (Attack Other, Attack Self, Withdrawal, and Attack Self-Child) was accounted for by the predictor variables. Specifically, dysfunctional fandom accounted for a significant amount of variance in each of those four subscales, and in fact, was the only significant predictor of Withdrawal coping. Dysfunctional fandom has been associated with a variety of negative behaviors. For example, dysfunction fans are more likely to consume large amounts of alcohol and to call into sport talk radio to voice their complaints (Wakefield & Wann, 2006). Dysfunctional fans often engaged in bullying behaviors as children (Courtney & Wann, 2010) and tend to view physical and verbal fan violence as appropriate (Donahue & Wann, 2009). It is perhaps not surprising, then, that a higher level of fan dysfunctional correlates with greater tendencies to be verbally and physically aggressive at sporting events (Wakefield & Wann, 2006; Wann & Waddill, in press; Wann, Waddill, Thomas, Scheuchner, & Ruga, 2012). Given that fan dysfunction includes a confrontational element, it seems reasonable that persons with high levels of fan dysfunction would cope with shame in a more dysfunctional manner, particularly those who would choose to internalize a child's embarrassing or shameful performance in sport.

In addition to the influence of dysfunctional fandom, identification with a child's sport team also had an impact on Attack Other scores. Identification with a sport team has been found to influence AO scores in a fan population (Partridge et al., 2010) and it seems logical that more highly identified parent fans would be more likely to "lash out" against others (e.g., referees, coaches, other parents) when vicarious shame is experienced.

The Attack Self scores were predicted by both fan dysfunction and parental gender. Attack Self is an internalization technique where anger associated with shame is directed inward, and has been found previously to occur significantly more frequently in females (Elison & Partridge, 2012; Nathanson, 1992). The sample for the current study was primarily female. If mothers experience vicarious shame in response to something that their child does in a sport setting, they may be more likely to experience self-directed anger or an awareness of one's own faults (e.g., "If I were better at sports, then maybe my child wouldn't make so many mistakes"). Sport-related stressors, including stress over a child's level of performance have been identified previously as influencing a parent's sport experience (Harwood, Drew, & Knight, 2010). Results from the current study support the potentially negative impact of a vicariously experienced emotion resulting from a child's sport performance.

The Attack Self-Child subscale was moderated by both dysfunction and parent's perception of their child's ability. Parents who have higher expectations for their child in an achievement domain such as sport may respond differently to their performance in that domain (Eccles & Harold, 1991). Several studies have identified that a parent's competence belief for their child in sport may influence the child's own perceptions of competence (Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois et al., 2002; Fredericks & Eccles, 2002), and may also result in positive or negative emotional outcomes for the child (Bois et al., 2009). The results from this study suggest that parents who have higher expectations for their children in sport and a higher dysfunctional fan nature would be more likely to direct anger inwardly in response to a shaming experience (i.e., feel inadequate for a child's poor performance).

Limitations, Implications, and Future Directions

There are limitations to this study that should inform future research on this topic.

The current study examined parental identification at only one discrete time, and parents were asked to respond to the questionnaires regarding a child who was currently in season for their sport. It is possible that a parent could have higher identification with a child in one sport compared to others, or that their identification level with a child could fluctuate depending on the sport being played. Furthermore, future research should assess overall levels of shame proneness as well as trait shame coping styles to determine if higher or lower total levels of shame influence the specific coping style utilized in order to better identify whether it is the mere experience of shame, or the relative amount of shame that might lead to a specific reaction in a parent. If differing overall levels of shame predict different coping styles, interventions to help parents cope more effectively could be developed to improve emotional reactions to a child's performance in sport.

Parental behavior at youth sport events, particularly negative actions such as fighting or berating a child or an official, has become more important as youth sport has continued to become more popular, expensive and competitive. Youth sport organizations should utilize the information presented in this research to train coaches, officials and administrators more effectively by increasing awareness of the different types of emotional reactions in youth sport parents, as well as what may trigger these specific responses.

The often inappropriate behaviors of parents (and other adults) at youth sport organizations has gained the attention of the media and organizations dedicated to improving youth sports have begun to confront the issues of poor sportsmanship from parents. For instance, the National Alliance for Youth Sports publishes its National Standards for Youth Sports containing suggestions for youth sports administrators to assist leagues in decreasing the number and intensity of abusive behaviors among parents (National Standards, 2008). While exceptionally valuable, standards such as these are typically designed to focus situational antecedents of problematic parental behavior (e.g., a league's culture, modeling of inappropriate behaviors among coaches, etc.). However, research on hostility and aggression indicates that, although situational factors are important, so too are personality variables (Anderson, 1997; Anderson, Anderson, & Deuser, 1996). Given the significant impact of fan dysfunction for many of the shame coping styles, and in particular Attack Self-Child, it is evident that the current work supports the notion that personality traits are important antecedents of hostility. Youth sport administrators should be aware of this when developing strategies to improve the culture surrounding youth sports. That is, while focusing on situational determinants is important, they should also implement strategies designed to reduce the negative reactions of parents possessing traits associated with poor behaviors, such as high levels of dysfunctional fandom. While it would not be reasonable to "test" parents to determine those which are most at risk (i.e., to determine those parents who are highly dysfunctional), it could be quite useful to inform parents (and league officials) about the relationship between fan dysfunction and tendencies to cope through Attack Self-Child. By arming parents with this knowledge, they may become more aware of their own traits and, consequently, begin to implement strategies designed to reduce the corresponding reaction behaviors.

References

Anderson, C. A. (1997). Effects of violent movies and trait hostility on hostile feelings and aggressive thoughts. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 161-178.

Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., & Deuser, W. E. (1996). Examining an affective aggression framework: Weapon and temperature affects on aggressive thoughts, affect, and attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 366-376.

Babkes, M. L., & Weiss, M. R. (1999). Parental influence on children's cognitive and affective responses to competitive soccer participation. Pediatric Exercise Science, 11, 44-62.

Babkes Stellino, M., Partridge, J. A., & Moore, K. (2012). Social influence on emotion and coping. In J. Thatcher, M. Jones, & D. Lavallee (Eds.) Coping and emotion in sport (2nd ed., pp. 145-166). New York: Nova Science Publishers.

Bois, J. E., Lalanne, J., & Delforge, C. (2009). The influence of parenting practices and parental pressure on children's and adolescents' pre-competitive anxiety. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 995-1005.

Bois, J. E., Sarrazin, P. G., Brustad, R. J., Trouilloud, D. O., & Curry, F. (2002). Mothers' expectancies and young adolescents' perceived physical competence: A year long study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 384-406.

Branscombe, N. R., & Doojse, B. (2004). Collective guilt: International perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Brustad, R. J. (1988). Affective responses in competitive youth sport: The influence of intrapersonal and socialization factors. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 10, 307-321.

Campbell, J. S., & Elison, J. (2005). Shame coping styles and psychopathic personality traits. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 96-104.

Courtney, J. J., & Warm, D. L. (2010). The relationship between sport fan dysfunction and bullying behaviors. North American Journal of Psychology, 12, 191-198.

Donahue, T., & Warm, D. L. (2009). Perceptions of the appropriateness of sport fan physical and verbal aggression: Potential influences of team identification and fan dysfunction. North American Journal of Psychology, 11, 419-428.

Dorsch, T. E., Smith, A. L., & McDonough, M. H. (2009). Parents' perceptions of child-to-parent socialization in organized youth sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 444-468.

Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. (1991). Gender differences in sport involvement: Applying the Eccles' expectancy-value model. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 7-35.

Elison, J. (2005). Shame and guilt: A hundred years of apples and oranges. New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 5-32.

Elison, J., Lennon, R., and Pulos, S. (2006). Investigating the compass of shame: The development of the Compass of Shame Scale. Social Behavior and Personality, 34, 221-238.

Elison, J., & Partridge, J. A. (2012). Relationships between shame-coping, fear of failure and perfectionism in college athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 35, 19-39.

Elison, J., Pulos, S., & Lennon, R. (2006). Shame-focused coping: An empirical study of the compass of shame. Social Behavior and Personality, 34, 161-168.

Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children's competence and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth trajectories in two male sex-typed domains. Developmental Psychology, 38, 519-533.

Harwood, C., Drew, A., & Knight, C. J. (2010). Parental stressors in professional youth football academies: A qualitative investigation of specializing stage parents. Qualitative Research in Sport and Exercise, 2, 39-55.

Horn, T. S., & Weiss, M. R. (1991). A developmental analysis of children's self-ability judgments in the physical domain. Pediatric Exercise Science, 3, 310-326.

Johns, M., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2005). Ashamed to be an American? The role of identification in predicting vicarious shame for anti-Arab prejudice after 9-11. Self and Identity, 4, 331-348.

Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press.

Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Seamier, M., & Ames, D. R. (2005). Vicarious shame and guilt. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8(2), 145-157.

Lickel, B., Steele, R. R., & Schmader, T. (2011). Group-based shame and guilt: Emerging directions in research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 153-163.

Massey, W. V., & Partridge, J. A. (2010). Patterns of shame coping styles in high school athletes. Journal of Youth Sports, 6,1-12.

McCarthy, P. J., Jones, M. V., & Clark-Carter, D. (2008). Understanding enjoyment in youth sport: A developmental perspective. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 142-156.

Nathanson, D. L. (1992). Shame and pride. New York: Norton.

National Standards. (2008). National standards for youth sports. West Palm Beach, FL: National Alliance for Youth Sports.

Partridge, J. A., & Elison, J. (2009). Shame in sport and exercise: Issues and directions. Journal of Contemporary Athletics, 4(3), 197-210.

Partridge, J. A., Wann, D. L., & Elison, J. (2010). Understanding college sport fans' experiences of and attempts to cope with shame. Journal of Sport Behavior, 33(2), 160-175.

Seamier, M., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2009). Parental shame and guilt: Distinguishing emotional responses to a child's wrongdoings. Personal Relationships, 16, 205-220.

Sloan, L. R. (1989). The motives of sports fans. In J. D. Goldstein (Ed.), Sports, games and play: Social and psychosocial viewpoints (2nd ed.) (pp. 175-240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Sloan, L. R., & Van Camp, D. (2008). Advances in theories of sport fan motives: Fan personal motives and the emotional impacts of games and their outcomes. In L. W. Hugenberg, P. M. Haridakis, & A. C. Earnheardt (Eds.), Sports mania: Essays on fandom and the media in the 21st century (pp. 129-157). Jefferson, NC: McFarland.

Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford Press. Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect/imagery/consciousness. Vol. 2. New York: Springer.

Wakefield, K. L., & Wann, D. L. (2006). An examination of dysfunctional sport fans: Method of classification and relationships with problem behaviors. Journal of Leisure Research, 38, 168-186.

Wann, D. L. (2014, February). Investigating the impact of team identification on the verbal and physical aggression of youth baseball parents. Paper presented at the annual Western Kentucky University Sport Psychology Forum, Bowling Green, KY.

Wann, D. L., Dolan, T. J., McGeorge, K. K., & Allison, J. A. (1994). Relationships between spectator identification and spectators' perceptions of influence, spectators' emotions, and competition outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 16, 347-364.

Wann, D. L., & Waddill, P. J. (in press). Predicting sport fans' willingness to consider anonymous acts of aggression: Importance of team identification and fan dysfunction. In C. Mohiyeddini (Ed.), Psychology of sports. Hauppauge NY: Nova.

Wann, D. L., Waddill, P. J., Thomas, D., Scheuchner, H., & Ruga, K. (2012). Sport spectator verbal aggression: The impact of team identification and fan dysfunction and fans 'verbal abuse of opponents and officials. Unpublished manuscript.

Yelsma, P., Brown, N. M., & Elison, J. (2002). Shame-focused coping styles and their associations with self-esteem. Psychological Reports, 90, 1179-1189.

Julie A. Partridge

Southern Illinois University Carbondale

Daniel L. Wann

Murray State University

Address correspondence to: Julie A. Partridge, Ph.D., Southern Illinois University Carbondale, Department of Kinesiology, 1075 S. Normal Ave., Carbondale, IL 62901-4310. Email: [email protected].
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for the fan and shame coping variables.

                              Mean     SD    Minimum   Maximum

Predictor Variables
  Team Identification         40.39   7.63    9.00      53.00
  Dysfunctional Fandom        13.93   8.17    5.00      41.00
  Sport Fandom                27.60   9.99    5.00      40.00
  Child's Perceived Ability    4.00    .84    1.00       5.00
Shame Coping Skills
  Attack Others               12.70   4.52    8.00      28.00
  Attack Self                 14.79   5.04    8.00      30.00
  Attack Self-Child           11.87   4.22    8.00      27.00
  Avoidance                   18.86   5.29    8.00      31.00
  Withdrawal                  11.19   4.25    8.00      25.00

Note: For the shame coping subscales, all means were significantly
different (paired samples t-tests, p < .05).

Table 2
Correlations among the variables

                              1       2       3       4        5
Child's Perceived Ability
(1)

Team Identification (2)

Sport Fandom (3)            99 **   43 **

Dysfunctional Fandom (4)     .05    .25 **   .12

Attack Other (5)            -.03    .19 *    .03    .32 **

Attack Self (6)             -.03     .03     .01    .17 *    44 **

Attack Self-Child (7)       -.12     .06     .04    .27 **   .56 **

Avoidance (8)                .01     .11     -.02   .15 *    .50 **

Withdrawal (9)               .13     .06     .07    .17 *    .63 **

Gender (10)                  .04    19 **    -.07    .06      .08

                              6        7        8       9      10
Child's Perceived Ability
(1)

Team Identification (2)

Sport Fandom (3)

Dysfunctional Fandom (4)

Attack Other (5)

Attack Self (6)

Attack Self-Child (7)       .64 **

Avoidance (8)               .42 **   34 **

Withdrawal (9)              59 **    .59 **   .35 **

Gender (10)                 -.16 *    -.07     .03     -.01

Note: * = p < .01; ** = p < .001; gender was coded as males = 1,
females = 2
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有