The importance of financial literacy.
Lusardi, Annamaria
How much do individuals know?
Increasingly, individuals are in charge of securing their own
financial well-being after retirement. With the shift from defined
benefit to defined contribution pensions, today's workers must
decide both how much to save and how to allocate their retirement
wealth. Financial markets have become more complex and individuals are
faced with a proliferation of new investment products. Investment
opportunities have expanded beyond national borders, permitting
individuals to invest in a broad range of assets and currencies.
However, as the financial crisis has made clear, it is very hard to
navigate this new financial system, and the consequences of mistakes can
be devastating. How well equipped are individuals to make financial
decisions and how much do individuals know about economics and finance?
Very few datasets provide information about financial literacy, and
those that do often do not have any facts about saving and financial
decisionmaking. To address this, Olivia Mitchell and I designed a module
on financial literacy for the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), a
survey that provides information on people 50 and older. (1) We aimed to
assess their knowledge of basic concepts that lay at the basis of saving
and portfoliochoice decisions, such as interest compounding, inflation,
and risk diversification. The results from that initial module were
striking: only one-third of respondents could do simple inter est rate
calculations and appeared to understand the effects of inflation and the
workings of risk diversification. What is surprising is not that people
lack financial knowledge, but rather how little people know about basic
economic concepts. Financial illiteracy is not only widespread but is
particularly severe in certain demographic groups. Two groups that stand
out from our analysis are the elderly and women; both of them display
very low knowledge. (2)
These findings are not unique to this survey or to this particular
age group. We have confirmed these findings using different datasets,
different methods of data collection, and different age groups. For
example, we find low rates of numeracy among younger individuals (Early
Baby Boomers) (3) and in the entire U.S. population. (4) Moreover, such
results are not limited to the United States. With several co-authors, I
examined financial literacy in the Netherlands using the same questions
that I used in the U.S. HRS. (5) Like American households, Dutch
households also exhibit fairly low levels of financial knowledge. (6)
Together with Peter Tufano, I also have assessed financial
knowledge that is specifically related to debt, or debt literacy. (7)
Our aim was to evaluate respondents' knowledge of the workings of
interest compounding and credit cards and their ability to compare
borrowing options and choose those with lower rates. We found that even
though most individuals deal frequently with credit cards and other
forms of borrowing, only a minority of individuals in the United States
possess basic financial knowledge relating to debt. For example, only
one-third of respondents in a representative sample of the U.S. pop
ulation know that they cannot eliminate credit card debt by paying a
minimum amount equivalent to the interest payments.
In this survey, we were able to go one step further in our analysis
to compare actual financial knowledge (as determined by responses to our
debt-literacy questions) to self-assessed knowledge, which was
determined by asking respondents to rate their own financial knowledge.
In stark contrast to responses to the questions measuring actual
knowledge, the majority of individuals give themselves high knowledge
ratings, pointing to a gulf between how much people actually know and
how much they think they know. Two other findings stand out in our
analysis: we find that women display low debt literacy, based on
responses to our questions measuring actual financial knowledge, and
that they give themselves low ratings when assessing their own financial
knowledge. In contrast, elderly respondents rank the lowest in terms of
actual financial knowledge but the highest in terms of self-assessed
knowledge. This may explain the prevalence of financial scams
perpetrated against the elderly.
Does financial knowledge matter?
Even with little personal knowledge, individuals can avoid making
mistakes by consulting with those who are more knowledgeable, including
financial professionals. It is not enough to recognize that financial
knowledge is low; we must also understand whether financial literacy
matters in decisionmaking. Addressing this question is particularly
difficult because financial literacy is not distributed randomly in the
population: those who possess high levels of literacy are likely to
possess characteristics, such as high talents and ability, or patience,
which also are correlated with financial decisionmaking. Moreover,
individuals may choose to invest in acquiring financial knowledge; thus,
financial literacy itself can be a choice variable. And, it may be that
those who have high wealth, rich pensions, or investments in financial
markets care more about improving their financial knowledge.
In my work, I have examined a set of outcomes that are related to
wealth accumulation. In many of my papers, I documented that significant
numbers of workers do not plan for retirement, even when they are not
far away from it. (8) Yet planning for retirement pays off: those who
plan end up with twice as much wealth as those who do not. Looking at
retirement planning data thus represents an easy and direct way to test
the predictions of a simple version of the life-cycle model. According
to this model, people should be forward-looking: they should look ahead,
anticipate the drop in income after retirement, and calculate how much
they need to save in order to maintain a constant stream of consumption
over their lifetime. However, these calculations are not easy. They
require the ability to make projections about future variables (such as
income growth, inflation, and pension benefits) as well as the ability
to determine present discounted values. In my work, I have examined
whether individuals do make these calculations and whether financial
literacy affects their ability to do so.
In the module we designed for the 2004 HRS, Olivia Mitchell and I
added a question that asked whether individuals had ever tried to
calculate how much they need to save for retirement. While respondents
to the HRS are only five to ten years away from retirement, only around
30 percent of them had calculated how much they needed to save.
Moreover, we found a strong link between retirement planning behavior
and financial literacy: it is disproportionately those who can make
interest rate calculations and who possess some sophisticated knowledge
who reported having calculated how much they need to save for
retirement. However, as mentioned above, one cannot rule out the
possibility that it is the desire to plan for retirement that results in
individuals making an effort to increase their financial knowledge,
rather than financial knowledge causing individuals to plan for
retirement.
To be able to disentangle this nexus of causality, we re-examined
the link between financial literacy and planning. In another paper using
data from the Rand American Life Panel, a survey in which we inserted
the same questions that we designed for the HRS, (9) we use the fact
that several U.S. states mandated financial literacy education in high
school to measure respondents' exogenous exposure to financial
education. The decision to mandate financial education is mostly the
result of a political process. Moreover, states differed in what they
mandated and the year the mandate went into effect. We also considered
the amount of resources that were devoted to education across states,
because mandates can be ineffective if few resources are allocated to
training teachers and implementing new courses. We found that
individuals who were born in states that mandated financial education in
high school were more likely to display higher financial knowledge later
in life. Moreover, respondents who received their education in states
that not only mandated financial education but also had higher per pupil
education spending had higher levels of financial knowledge. Most
importantly, after instrumenting financial literacy with mandates across
states, interacted with the amount of education expenses per pupil, we
found a strong positive relationship between financial literacy and
retirement planning, even stronger than the simple estimates based on
HRS data. This finding is consistent with another important paper in
this area of research: Bernheim, Garrett, and Maki (2001) showed that
individuals who were exposed to financial education in high school had
higher savings later in life. (10) Given that retirement planning is a
powerful proxy for wealth, there is now a body of evidence supporting
the effects of financial literacy on wealth holdings.
In the debt literacy paper, I demonstrated that debt literacy can
be linked to a variety of financial experiences, from borrowing on
credit cards to using payday lending or pawn shops, to investing in
stocks and mutual funds, or simply having a checking account. That paper
emphasizes the fact that individuals make many financial decisions and
that those decisions are highly interrelated. For example, those who
always pay credit card bills in full are less likely to use other
highcost means of borrowing, such as payday loans. Conversely, those who
only pay the minimum amount due on their credit cards are more likely to
use other costly forms of borrowing. This means that financial literacy
can have an effect above and beyond the single financial decisionmaking
variable--for example, wealth accumulation, participation in the stock
market, having a checking account--which we normally study when
assessing the impact of financial literacy on behavior. Thus, to fully
capture the effect of literacy on financial behavior, it is important to
look at a rich set of financial experiences.
How to increase the effectiveness of financial education
Having shown that financial literacy is very low and that it
affects financial behavior, we naturally arrive at the question of what
can be done to raise financial knowledge and which programs can
influence saving and wealth accumulation. This is the topic I pursued in
my newly published book, Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase
the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs (University
of Chicago Press, 2008). There are two ideal venues for the delivery of
financial education: schools and the workplace. The book provides an
overview of financial knowledge among high school students and shows
that it is not only the adult population but also the young who lack
basic financial knowledge. Given the benefits that financial literacy
brings, there may be advantages to introducing financial literacy into
high school curricula. The book also offers an evaluation of
employer-provided financial education programs. The evidence, so far, is
mixed, but as the book argues, we cannot necessarily learn much from
existing programs. Workplace programs commonly offer very limited
interventions, such as a one-time retirement seminar or one benefit
fair. It is hard to imagine that such interventions can do much to
combat widespread financial illiteracy; a one-time, one-size-fits-all
seminar can hardly be an adequate response to the problem of widespread
financial illiteracy among U.S. workers. The book provides evidence that
programs that offer multiple financial education sessions have been
effective in stimulating saving among lowincome workers, who are
normally those least likely to save. It also shows that women are
particularly receptive to financial education programs. Given that women
tend to display low levels of literacy, these findings suggest that
targeted education programs can raise financial literacy among the
population groups that are most in need of improved financial literacy.
The book also shows that one way to promote saving is to facilitate
and simplify the decisionmaking process, including helping workers to
implement saving plans. A recurrent result of financial education
programs is that, although they seem to affect the intentions of
workers, many of whom report plans to modify their saving or investment
behavior after attending a seminar, these intentions do not always
translate into actions. This provides some explanation for why
retirement planning seems to have such a large effect on saving. As some
psychologists have argued, devising a plan of action makes it more
likely for individuals to follow through on intentions. If this
mechanism is important, it may be possible to devise cost-effective ways
to stimulate saving. The book describes a program that I have
implemented at Dartmouth College in collaboration with a professor of
marketing from the Tuck School of Business and the vice president for
Finance and Administration at Dartmouth. 11 We provided new hires
(non-faculty employees) with a planning aid. This is simply a
double-sided sheet that describes the steps that new employees have to
take to enroll in a supplementary retirement account (SRA). It also
provides information that employees would otherwise have to collect in
order to open an SRA. Thus the planning aid simplifies decisionmaking,
which can be particularly useful for those with low financial literacy.
Having clear guidelines on what needs to be done in order to open an SRA
also makes it easier to translate intentions into actions. Finally, the
program provides information when it is needed, that is, when decisions
about pensions have to be made. Preliminary evidence shows that this
simple intervention doubled the enrollment into SRAs at Dartmouth. It
also shows that, by recognizing the many difficulties that people face
when making saving decisions--from limited financial literacy to
barriers to implementing saving plans- we may hope to increase the
effectiveness of financial education programs.
In a world of increased individual financial responsibility, where
workers are in charge of their financial well-being and where financial
markets offer new and complex financial products, financial literacy is
essential. Just as it has proven to be impossible to succeed in the
modern world without the ability to read and write (literacy), so it
will be impossible to succeed in the present-day financial system
without knowing the abc's of economics and finance (financial
literacy).
(1) See the review and discussion of these questions in A. Lusardi,
"Financial Literacy: An Essential Tool for Informed Consumer
Choice?" NBER Working Paper No. 14084, June 2008.
(2) See A. Lusardi and O. Mitchell, "Planning and Financial
Literacy: How Do Women Fare?" NBER Working Paper No. 13750, January
2008, and American Economic Review 98(2), 2008, pp. 413-17.
(3) See A. Lusardi and O. Mitchell, "Baby Boomer Retirement
Security: the Roles of Planning, Financial Literacy and Housing
Wealth," NBER Working Paper No. 12585, October 2006, and Journal of
Monetary Economics 54, 2007, pp. 205-24.
(4) See A. Lusardi and O. Mitchell, "How Ordinary Consumers
Make Complex Economic Decisions: Financial Literacy and Retirement
Readiness," unpublished paper, March 2009.
(5) See M. van Rooij, A. Lusardi, and R. Alessie, "Financial
Literacy and Stock Market Participation," NBER Working Paper No.
13565, October 2007.
(6) The questions we have designed for the US. HRS have now been
added to several national surveys. In addition to the Dutch DNB Household Survey, they have been added to the German SA HE, the Italian
Household Income and Wealth, the World Bank Russia Survey, a survey of
pension providers in Mexico, and a survey of entrepreneurs in Sri Lanka.
It is therefore possible to perform international comparisons.
(7) See A. Lusardi and P. Tufano, "Debt Literacy, Financial
Experiences, and Overindebtedness," NBER Working Paper No. 14808,
March 2009.
(8) See A. Lusardi, "Household Saving Behavior: The Role of
Financial Literacy, Information, and Financial Education Programs,"
NBER Working Paper No. 13824, February 2008, and forthcoming in
Implications of Behavioral Economics for Economic Policy.
(9) See A. Lusardi and O. Mitchell, "How Ordinary Consumers
Make Complex Economic Decisions: Financial Literacy and Retirement
Readiness," unpublished paper, March 2009.
(10) D. Bernheim, D. Garrett, and D. Maki, "Education and
Saving: The Long-term Effects of High School Financial Curriculum
Mandates," Journal of Public Economics 85, 2001, pp. 435-565.
(11) See A. Lusardi, P. Keller, and A. Keller, "New Ways to
Make People Save: A Social Marketing Approach," NBER Working Paper
No. 14715, February 2009, and chapter 7 of the book, Overcoming the
Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education
and Saving Programs, University of Chicago Press,
Lusardi is a professor of economics at Dartmouth College and a
Research Associate in the NBER s Program on Aging. Her profile appears
later in this issue. 2008, pp. 209-36.