Living on the boundary: scriptural authority and psychology.
Hill, Peter C.
For Christian psychologists to move from their marginalized
position with mainstream psychology, they must be able to substantively
demonstrate the unique insights that the integration of psychology with
Christian theology offers to the discipline. To do this, Christian
psychologists must be able to show, not just claim, the authority of
Scripture by demonstrating its explanatory power on psychology's
terms. Three factors in psychology's new zeitgeist provide both
opportunities and challenges to demonstrating Scriptural authority: a
growing cultural interest in spirituality, postmodernism, and novel
approaches to cognitive science. Cognitive-Experiential Self Theory
(CEST) is provided as a concrete example where Christian thinking
provides greater understanding of an emerging psychological theory, thus
demonstrating explanatory power and providing Scripture a more
authoritative position.
**********
The theologian Paul Tillich often described himself as living
"on the boundary." For Tillich, the boundary was between
philosophy and theology, between essence and existence, between Europe
and America, and between the findings of science and theological
revelation (Rogers, 1977). Psychologists who are Christians also find
themselves on the boundary, sharing some of Tillich's boundaries
but also adding a few more: between commitments to empirical research and commitments to other forms of knowledge (including revealed
knowledge), between modernism and postmodernism, and between basic
tenets of the Christian faith and contemporary spirituality. As one of
these Christian psychologists, I find that living on the boundary is
sometimes uncomfortable, perhaps because the boundary itself seems
narrow and delicate. At other times, living on the boundary holds
unusual promise and is therefore exciting, especially when the boundary
begins to fade and only one field of vision slowly appears to emerge.
The very nature of the boundary itself seems to be shifting between
a psychology that has treated religion, including Christianity, with
little regard, and the signs of a new-breed psychology that is at least
willing to consider religion worthy of study. The boundary thus appears
to be less sharply defined than in the past and evangelicals, though
still on psychology's periphery, may have a greater opportunity for
impact on the discipline. Nevertheless, living on the boundary of
psychology and Christianity remains precarious, for it is a narrow
boundary and, for the evangelical, much is at stake. Those evangelicals
who take seriously our calling as professionals in the field of
psychology must be careful not to fall into either the abyss of a narrow
psychology that according to David Myers (1992, p. 12) "shields
itself from deep understandings" nor a narrowly conceived theology
that "shields itself from new understandings." Evangelical
Christian psychologists feel a pull in both directions because the
arguments on both sides of the boundary are compelling.
What often makes this such an uncomfortable boundary for the
evangelical is the desire to maintain the authority of scripture yet
remain committed to a discipline that utilizes a radically different
epistemology. One can make the case that if Christians are going to have
any substantial impact on psychology, it will have to be done, despite
serious presuppositional implications, on psychology's
epistemological terms. This suggests to some that the authority of
scripture will be undermined. My answer to such concerns is that our
understanding of scriptural authority must be broadened beyond typical
evangelical pronouncements and that boundary living, in one sense, calls
for an even greater commitment to scriptural authority-a commitment that
does not negate a belief in the sufficiency and primacy of scripture,
but actually complements this understanding.
Scriptural Authority
Evangelicals are rightly concerned about biblical authority. At the
heart of all other doctrine, the evangelical holds dear the prominent
position of scripture as a source of God's self-disclosure, that
is, indeed, a special revelation. Given the specialness of special
revelation, it is for the evangelical granted authority. For those who
live on the boundary of psychology and Christianity, there are at least
two meanings to the notion of scriptural authority: (a) a "high
view" of scripture, and (b) the "explanatory power" of
scripture. What I propose here is that evangelicals have emphasized the
first of the two conceptualizations, largely from a defensive posture
(perhaps justifiably so as a reaction to the late 19th Century's
sting of "higher criticism"), at the expense of the second.
But for those of us living on the boundary, both conceptions of
authority are crucial. And, furthermore, it is only the latter
conception that will allow evangelicals to have a greater impact on
their professional discipline.
A High View of Scripture. The first conception of scriptural
authority is the common one that we hear from evangelical theologians;
the high view of scripture argues that scripture is authoritative
because it holds regulatory force (i.e., it regulates my belief system
and is thus a primary if not solely sufficient source of my
"control beliefs") resulting from its inerrancy as a product
of divine inspiration found in its human authors. As Clark Pinnock (1971) points out:
The authority of Scripture is the watershed of theological conviction,
and its importance to a sound methodology is incalculable ... the
central problem for theology is its own epistemological base. From what
fountain head does theology acquire the information from which she forms
her doctrinal models and tests her hypothesis? What is the principium
theologiae which measures and authenticates the subject matter for
theology and preaching? No endeavor in theology can begin until some
kind of answer is given. The sheer weight of this crucial question has
kept and will continue to keep, the debate over biblical authority at
the center of the theological arena in our era. All issues pale before
this one. It is the continental divide in Christian theology. Everything
hangs on our solution to it. (p. 11)
From an evangelical perspective, there is no doubt truth in what
Pinnock has to say. And, indeed, a high view of scripture is
foundational to evangelicalism. Theology has good intradisciplinary
reasons that we are all familiar with why such a high view must be
preserved. Furthermore, to do good integration, we must first do good
theology (and good psychology for that matter). Otherwise, we are just
increasing the odds of mistakenly identifying conflict, when indeed no
conflict may exist. It appears to exist only because of poor
hermeneutics or poor science. Good theology and good psychology will
bring us closer to the truth about the objects of our respective domains
of study, scripture and human nature/behavior (generically defined)--and
the objects of our study are not in conflict.
At the heart of a good education in either discipline will be
courses on good methodology--hermeneutics in theology and research
methods and statistics in psychology (though unpopular those courses may
be). Pinnock (1971) instructs that an evangelical biblical hermeneutic
is a process of meaning-extraction, of bringing out the sense of the
Bible by means of principles Scripture itself supplies ... when we
approach Scripture as Christ did, in an attitude of total trust in all
that it teaches, a sound hermeneutic results. We strive to discern the
truth of each particular scripture in light of the whole of Scripture,
regardless of what extrabiblical fact or idea may be bearing upon the
text. Divine authorship implies complete reliability and assures us the
Bible will not ultimately contradict itself. (pp. 209-210)
The traditional hermeneutical approach subscribed to by Pinnock
(1971) is a model of doing theology proper and thus involves working
primarily with the claims of the text itself. But such an intratextual
declaration is not, in itself, totally sufficient as an authoritative
claim for those Christians who reside as ambassadors to another
discipline. The authoritative veracity of scripture, to be truly
persuasive to boundary sitters, requires both faith and sight, not
unlike the biblical examples of Gideon and Thomas. While a high view of
scripture may suffice for an understanding of scriptural authority for
the evangelical theologian, as a Christian integrationist living on an
interdisciplinary boundary I must find an additional criterion by which
I can declare the authority of scripture. The second criterion will not
contradict nor replace the high view of scripture as a foundation for
scriptural authority; rather it will supplement the first criterion and
act in concert with it.
Authority as Explanatory Power. "If evangelical Protestants do
not overcome their pre-occupation with negative criticism of
contemporary theological deviation at the expense of the construction of
preferable alternatives to these, they will not be much of a doctrinal force in the decade ahead" (Henry, 1966, p. 9). This concern of
Carl Henry's four decades ago about the evangelical role in the
theological arena applies well to the integration enterprise today. The
integrationist must show that, indeed, a Christian understanding adds
overall explanatory power to the phenomenon under investigation--in our
case, the understanding of the human person. In fact, one reason why the
evolutionary model is so alive and well in psychology (as well as in all
life sciences) today is that it holds immense explanatory power. If only
integrationists could develop a system with such explanatory power!
My concern is similar to Henry's. By assuming a defensive
posture that somehow has had to "prove" scriptural authority
(e.g., the endless theological debates centered around scriptural
minutia), the evangelical has frequently lost sight of the true
specialness of special revelation; that is, the special insight into
God's meaning and purpose behind the creation. All too often,
special revelation is presented not in juxtaposition with general
revelation, but rather as a superordinate clause that provides the exact
formula for handling epistemological conflict when (not if) it happens.
It is as if we are dealt a hand with the ace of spades, knowing that we
can play it with authority whenever we deem best. It is in this sense
that evangelicals too often think of scripture as authoritative. I want
to suggest that scripture is authoritative also because of what it has
to offer--its unique insight and the potential explanatory power of such
insight--and not just because Christians believe they hold some sort of
ultimate trump card that negates the value of all else.
If indeed God is the author of all truth, as evangelicals are quick
to claim, a good Christian epistemology should be open to every
legitimate and reliable source of knowledge. And, indeed, Scripture
itself points to other legitimate sources of knowledge (Matt 12:22f;
Luke 11:14f; Phil 4:8) so we need not be concerned that we are somehow
undermining the role of special revelation. Furthermore, when
theologians discuss the sufficiency and primacy of Scripture (based upon
Scripture's inerrancy and resulting regulatory force over other
beliefs), it is often understood as the only defining criterion of the
basic essentials of Christian faith and practice (Packer, 1958, 1996).
Thus, while Scripture should be given final and ultimate authority in
matters of faith and practice directly relating to that faith (e.g.,
moral behavior), it does not mean that other sources of knowledge are
not helpful or perhaps even necessary. In this sense psychology can
provide insight to theology.
But even more pertinent to my argument here is that the
integrationist living on the boundary must be cognizant of the limits of
Scripture's sufficiency and primacy. That is, though Scripture
appears to be clear and direct in how we are to live moral lives, it is
less clear (and less authoritative), for example, about basic cognitive
and affective psychological processes--but nonetheless, it might have
something to add to our understanding of such processes.
I propose that if indeed God is the author of all truth and not
just the author of special revelation, and if indeed there are multiple
sources of knowledge that leads us into a greater understanding of that
truth, then the authority of scripture must be evaluated not only by its
own claims that appeal to just one source of knowledge (revelation), but
also by claims from other sources of knowledge, including reason and
empirical knowledge. Once scripture also gains explanatory power through
these other sources of knowledge, it gains authoritative status. Perhaps
no one is in a better position to help scripture reclaim its lost
authority in an age of science than those of us living on the boundary
of Christianity and science.
Before moving beyond this section, I should add a brief caveat. I
do not propose that by expanding the explanatory power of the Bible, we
are somehow going to "prove" the Bible to be true. This should
not be our motivation. Rather, I propose that as the explanatory power
of the Bible is truly expanded, it will simply be given greater
authority as a source of knowledge. As ambassadors to the academic
world, those of us living on the boundary are in a unique and privileged
position, but with all of the responsibilities that accompany such
privilege, of making the case for scriptural authority not just by the
claims of the text itself, but by demonstrating through all
epistemological channels the benefits to our understanding of taking the
text seriously. We are called to not just claim, but to show scriptural
authority within our respective disciplines. In a nutshell, the
integrationist (of theology and psychology in this case) is left with
the question, "what does integration offer the discipline of
psychology?" Only when we can substantively answer that question do
we deserve a broader hearing.
A Nondialogical First Step: The Scientific Study of Religion
The much respected social psychologist and methodologist Donald T.
Campbell (1976) once claimed that "present-day psychology and
psychiatry in all their major forms are more hostile to the inhibitory
messages of traditional religious moralizing than is scientifically
justified" (p. 1103). What is significant about this quotation is
the context and not the content of Campbell's statement--as
significant as the admission of psychology's myopic guilt might be.
This statement was part of Campbell's introduction to what turned
out to be a highly controversial presidential address to the American
Psychological Association at a time that fit squarely within the Cold
War era between psychology and religion. Today, there are discernable
signs that religion in general, and religious claims in general, are
given more weight in scientific discourse than in decades past. In the
field of psychology, such change has occurred to the point that just
last summer a book was published by the American Psychological
Association (APA) on Judeo-Christian perspectives on psychology, with
four evangelicals counted among the thirteen contributors.
It should be pointed out, however, that psychology's
engagement of religion and theology is as an equal partner--and hardly
should it be. That is, despite rather persuasive arguments from
contemporary philosophy of science that scientific knowledge is not
categorically separated from other forms of knowing, we can expect that,
in practice, mainstream scientific researchers, even those sympathetic
to the claims of Christianity, will approach this subject matter from a
rather limited scientific perspective. Religious experience will be
investigated only from the perspective of accrued knowledge gained from
the standard acceptable methodology of the social scientific
disciplines, including psychology. The dominant point of reference for
psychologists will be science and not religion. The simplicity of such a
recognition should not be confused with the importance of its
implications, including the fact that we should not expect that
mainstream psychology will embrace a dialogical or interactive mode with
religion, at least not until we convincingly can make the case, on
scientific grounds, that such interaction will be fruitful. Instead,
psychologists will likely approach this topic as any other
topic--cautiously, with skepticism, and within accepted scientific
models of investigation. We can also expect that the
"legitimacy" of studying religion and spirituality will, in
the final analysis, be judged by the scientific community through
standard scientific criteria (see Larson, Swyers, & McCullough,
1998). Many scientists, including psychologists, resonate with the
opening words of theologian and scientist Ian Barbour in chapter 1 of
his book Religion in an Age of Science (1990). "The first major
challenge to religion in an age of science is the success of the methods
of science. Science seems to provide the only reliable path to
knowledge. Many people view science as objective, universal, rational,
and based on solid observational evidence. Religion, by contrast, seems
to be subjective, parochial, emotional, and based on traditions or
authorities that disagree with each other" (p. 3). Though what is
reported by Barbour is an uncritical modernistic understanding of
science, it is clear that many scientists of all epistemological stripes
are convinced of the usefulness of scientific methodology.
Those living on the boundary can react in one of two ways. One
reaction might be that ultimately, such a rigid adherence to an
either-or nondialogical approach, where the value of religious teachings
are determined only by the terms of science, is not only imperialistic
and myopic, but eventually is also subject to the law of diminishing
returns. In this sense, religion is little different than most any other
applied topic of interest to psychologists that has come and gone; once
the research ideas begin to dry up, once researchers get hopelessly
bogged down by methodological minutia, once it is difficult to find many
statistically significant results or where the effect sizes remain
disappointingly low, or once funding agencies lose enthusiasm for the
religion variable, then it will be time to move on to another applied
topic. But this pessimistic view is just one possible scenario.
However, the argument can be made that we are indeed on the verge of opening interdisciplinary parlance. A postpositivistic philosophy of
science that teaches that scientific theories are underdetermined by
facts, that data are not bare but are rather theory and value-ladened,
that science is a human and cultural activity, and that advancement in
science is ultimately measured not by the accumulation of facts but
rather by theoretical refinement, is far more commensurable to religion
than the positivistic philosophy of psychology as science that was so
thoroughly dominant until the past few decades. Still, however, for the
content and truth claims of religion to be taken seriously, it is
necessary that religion be shown to be a unique variable with
substantial explanatory power that can supplement in a nonthreatening
manner a scientific understanding of human experience. Thus, it is
argued that a nondialogical approach, though limited, is likely a
necessary first step to jump-start even the potential for interaction
between religion and science.
ENCOURAGING SIGNS FOR THOSE LIVING ON THE BOUNDARY
Any effort to demonstrate the explanatory power of a psychology
integrated with Christian thought has benefited greatly during the past
two decades by an unusual degree of favorable change that has swept
across psychology and its related disciplines, a change described by
Richards and Bergin (1997) as a "new zeitgeist" that is
conducive to incorporating the study of religion and spirituality into
psychology's research agenda. Such change has been precipitated by
many factors, three of which will be discussed here: the recent emphasis
on spirituality, postmodern thought, and research in cognitive science.
None of these factors are independent of the other two.
Three Confluent Factors
The New Spirituality. It is hardly news that we are witnessing a
persistent and perhaps even renewed commitment to religious and
spiritual belief in our culture as a whole (Gallup, 1994; Gallup &
Castelli, 1989). Though I caution later of a developing perspective in
western society--one that tends to polarize in a simplistic fashion an
individual (read "good") spirituality versus an institutional
(read "bad") religiosity--there is evidence (Zinnbauer et al.,
1997) suggesting that the majority of Americans see themselves as both
spiritual (most frequently viewed only in personal or experiential terms
such as a belief in or relationship with God or a higher power) and
religious (most frequently understood in terms of both personal beliefs
in or experiences with God or a higher power and institutional beliefs
and practices such as church membership or attendance). As Hill et al.
(2000) state: "it appears that many individuals approach the sacred
through the personal, subjective, and experiential path of spirituality;
it is also apparent that this experiential path often includes
organizational or institutional beliefs and practices" (p. 16). So
what we have then is clearly a cultural interest in matters spiritual
which may help legitimate, at least to some extent, what religion brings
to our pursuit of knowledge.
Postmodernism. Psychology, like many other scientific disciplines,
has encountered a transition from a rigid positivistic philosophical
adherence to a more permeable epistemology that allows for other sources
of knowledge, including knowledge socially constructed (Gergen, 1985,
1994; Howard, 1986; Manicas & Secord, 1983), the validity of which
is a foundational premise of postmodern thought (Grenz, 1996). The
essential indeterminacy of nature expounded in Heisenberg's
Uncertainty Principle, the disruption of the commonsensical notion that
time and space are absolute by Einstein's Special Theory of
Relativity, and the reluctant acknowledgement forced upon us by Quantum
Theory that scientific inquiry is limited, all have had a ripple effect on psychology-as-science. Add to this Thomas Kuhn's well-known and
controversial arguments (but highly regarded by some) that (a) science
too is socially constructed, even as it defines reality (i.e., a belief
system, or paradigm, that prevails in the given scientific community as
to what constitutes the legitimate domain of science) and (b) that
progress in science is best realized through paradigm shifts--creative
bursts that allow the scientific community to view reality in perhaps a
refreshingly new manner (i.e., a scientific revolution).
Suddenly the epistemological barriers between science and religion
no longer seem quite so insurmountable. Though maintaining that the
results of this shift in construing what it means to call psychology (or
any discipline for that matter) a science is ambiguous at best, one
positive implication identified by Van Leeuwen (1996) for Christians in
academic psychology is that
truth is no longer reduced to what is empirically or analytically
demonstrable, with everything else (e.g., revelation literary and
artistic productions) considered so much subjective 'nonsense' in terms
of truth value. If all research is equally autobiographical, then the
discipline's gatekeepers are no more justified in rejecting well-crafted
theories based on a Christian worldview than ones coming from a
feminist, socialist, sociobiological, or any other standpoint. (p. 155)
From this, she concludes that religionists in general and
Christians in particular are "now less marginalized in the wake of
postmodern philosophical shifts" (p. 155).
Cognitive Science. A third development in psychology is the
recently broadened conceptualization of human consciousness (Eccles
& Robinson, 1984; Sperry, 1988), based on contemporary research on
the brain and cognition, that appears to be more conducive to a
religious understanding of human nature. For example, Sperry, who takes
a multilevel view of the mind/body relationship distances himself from
both the dualist and materialist positions. Arguing that consciousness
cannot exist independent of physical events, yet holding that mental
states and physical events are dissimilar, Sperry contends that mental
states are higher-level, holistic, and emergent yet law-abiding
properties of the brain, that are causally effective downward, making
use of but not violating the laws of lower levels of human existence.
Barbour (1990) maintains that Sperry's position on human nature, by
guarding against both a materialist reductionism and a mind/body
dualism, is compatible not only with contemporary views of science but
also with a religious worldview. Though the process theology proposed by
Barbour as especially integrative with this expanded conceptualization
is hardly evangelical, it does appear that the recent theoretical
developments in cognitive science have helped create a new and more
religion-friendly zeitgeist in psychology.
The Challenges Ahead
From the New Spirituality. These same three confluent factors also
present hurdles, especially for the evangelical, as we attempt to
establish a constructive relationship between religion and psychology.
Though I argued earlier that the common tendency to polarize religion
and spirituality is in part a social mispersonification, there is no
denying that the religious landscape is changing in western society.
What is emerging is a new breed of spirituality that is increasingly
distinct from traditional, and certainly evangelical, belief structures.
As a result, classic secularization theory in the sociology of religion has been revised to suggest not the elimination, but rather the
transformation, of religion in contemporary society. This transformation
is what Hunter (1983) calls the "deinstitutionalization of
religious reality" (p. 14) as he describes the quandary of American
evangelicalism. It appears that the term "spirituality,"
distinctively different and holding for a more positive connotation than
the term "religion" (Pargament, 1999), has become the favored
descriptor of this privatization of religious experience.
Some scholars (e.g., Turner, Lukoff, Barnhouse, & Lu, 1995;
Sheldrake, 1992) see the gestation period for this transformation as the
1960s and 1970s, a period that reflected among other factors a growing
disillusionment with the established church and other religious
institutions. Others, most notably historians of religion (e.g., Silk,
1998), remind us that such religious individualism characterizes much
Christian experience and can be traced back at least to the
Reformation's emphasis on the removal of the church or clergy as a
mediator in favor of a direct and personal approach to God (see Tillich,
1952, pp. 160-163). Regardless, we can say with assurance that much
contemporary religious life in the United States is captured well by
Bellah and his associates (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, &
Tipton, 1985) in their description of the young nurse "Sheila
Larson," who claimed that her religion is a belief in God
unencumbered by church attendance and other social features. Rather,
"It's Sheilaism. Just my own little voice" (p. 221).
To study only the functional benefits (or liabilities) of such
privatized religion as "Sheilaism" is of limited value. While
simply having virtually any religious or spiritual belief regardless of
content, has potential implications for psychological study, the problem
is that by empirically studying something so broadly defined as
"just one's own little voice," the distinctive
characteristics of religion and spirituality are lost. Such a
functional-only concept of religion begins to sound strikingly similar
to an earlier generation of humanistic psychology: authenticity,
individuation, and self-actualization. If this field, however, is to
truly move forward, we must extend our research agendas beyond strictly
a functional analysis of religion and spirituality and begin
investigating the substance of religious and spiritual belief. In other
words, as we begin to more fully address, with regards to the now well
documented link between religion and both mental and physical health,
the why question (i.e., "Why is religion good for your
health?"), we must eventually come to grips with the specific
personalized content and "grammar" of the religious or
spiritual experience, whether clearly understood and articulated by the
individual or not.
Once the questions get to this point, the doors for integrative
work begin to creep open and those living on the boundary have an
opportunity to step forward. Are evangelical scholars ready?
From Postmodernism. I suggested earlier that the sweeping inroads into psychology and the social sciences by postmodern thinking provides
a potential platform for Christian psychologists. My impression is that
those psychologists, who question the modern scientific worldview in
favor of a contextualized social constructionism, are inviting
Christians (as well as everyone else) to the party, with the
understanding that no one at the party is to be too visible or allowed
to make exclusive claims. After all, everyone at the party has their own
legitimate take on the world and it's probably good that all in
attendance try to see where the others are coming from.
In theory, postmodern psychologists are skeptical about the
possibility of genuine advance in our understanding of persons; thus all
positions theoretically have an equally legitimate stake and it is
difficult to argue for the merits of one position over another. In
practice, however, it is the exercise of social power (in this case, the
party's gatekeepers) that determines who and how positions are
provided respect and a listening ear, reflecting "a process of
intellectual and professional disunification or disintegration"
(Jones, 1996, pp. 138-139; see also Vande Kemp's, 1996, pp.
162-165, discussion of the "forces of disintegration" in
psychology). If this analysis of postmodernism is at all correct, the
future does not bode well, for there are several inherent messages
within Christianity, some valid and some (mis)perceived, that because
they run so contrary to the course charted by psychology's
gatekeepers, will likely keep Christian psychology from being a power
broker within the discipline. For example, these inherent messages
include an emphasis on respecting and following authority, an exclusive
claim to knowing Truth, belief in the existence of eternal damnation,
and conservative positions on politically inflammatory issues such as
sexual orientation. Post-modernism's current challenge to the
dialogue between psychology and the Christian faith will likely
intensify in the years ahead (Carter, 1996).
From Cognitive Science. Despite the aforementioned views of Roger
Sperry, among others, regarding a broader understanding of human
consciousness, cognitive science also presents itself as a formidable
hurdle. The Christian psychologist Jay Brand (1997) reviews the
literature from neuropsychology, computational cognition, neural
networks, and evolutionary psychology and concludes that through the
recent advances in these fields, there is now sufficient evidence to
encourage the reductionist to think that he or she may account for most
complexities of human experience. Brand sees a strict scientific
adherence to the principle of parsimony as the chief culprit that
encourages such a reductionistic view and presents a challenge to
Christians who accept science as a legitimate source of knowledge:
Succinctly, the challenge is this: If we wish to retain a scientific
approach to the mind/body problem, we will eventually need to abandon
either the necessity of supernatural intervention and spirituality in
our proximal accounts of individual behavior, or the principle of
parsimony in the theoretical construction of those accounts. Currently,
at least in this author's estimation, those who take both the biblical
perspective on human psychology and its scientific alternatives
seriously, have not adequately addressed how crucial will become the
principle of parsimony in the maintenance of their uneasy truce. (p.
242)
So, what I have identified are three distinct yet overlapping
factors that both facilitate and also inhibit the full study of
psychology informed by theology, and vice-versa. Certainly other factors
could be discussed, including a parallel set of factors that both
encourage and discourage theology's willingness to dialogue with
psychology. However, as a psychologist, I am far more comfortable
speaking about the limits of my own discipline and, hence, will
therefore unidirectionally discuss theology's potential
contributions to contemporary psychology.
WHAT DOES THE INTEGRATION OF PSYCHOLOGY WITH CHRISTIAN THOUGHT HAVE
TO OFFER PSYCHOLOGY?
Jones (1994) postulated three modes of interaction between religion
and science. The first mode is a dialogical mode where neither religion
nor psychology can dictate to the other how to define and interpret its
task, thus avoiding either a religious or scientific imperialism. The
second mode is a critical-evaluative mode where each discipline can
serve as an external evaluator to the other and thereby help major
models and paradigms be appropriately self-critical, especially about
underlying assumptions. It seems to me that those living on the boundary
of integration of Christianity and psychology have spent a
disproportionate amount of their time functioning in either or both of
these two modes and have neglected Jones' third mode, the
constructive mode. The constructive mode of interaction, when relating
religion to science, for example, "... should occur when religious
belief contributes positively to the progress of science by suggesting
new modes of thought that transform an area of study by shaping new
perceptions of the data and new theories" (p. 194). A constructive
mode of interaction, whether theoretical or phenomenological, is not
imperialistic. It does not suggest that theology is to replace or even
discount science or change science's fundamental nature. Nor does
this constructive interaction mode suggest that science replace,
discount, or alter theology's fundamental nature. It does contend
that the scientist and theologian must be open to a transformation of
how to think about their primary subject matter when the other domain of
study has something positive to contribute to an understanding of the
phenomena under investigation. An attitude of epistemological humility
(Carter & Narramore, 1979)--whereby we expand disciplinary
boundaries through conjunctive analysis and perhaps release territorial
comfort zones--is necessary for such interaction to be truly
constructive.
It seems to me that if we ever hold out any hope of reestablishing
scripture's authoritative claim beyond any declarative statement of
a high view, then we must do so through a constructive mode of
interaction. What does integration have to constructively offer
psychology? Elsewhere (Hill, 1999), I have suggested a number of
possibilities. I will now focus on one of those possibilities in greater
detail, utilizing Jones' (1994) constructive mode of interaction.
AN EXAMPLE OF HOW THEOLOGY CAN CONTRIBUTE TO PSYCHOLOGICAL INQUIRY:
FUNCTIONING AS KNOWING PERSONS
In the last portion of this article, I will review a psychological
theory of how knowledge, including religious and scientific knowledge,
is constructed. The review of this theory serves two purposes: (a) to
demonstrate that both science and religious faith utilize the major
processes outlined in the theory, suggesting that scientific and
religious knowing are less differentiated than commonly supposed, and
(b) to provide a concrete illustration of a constructive mode of
interaction--in this case how religion or theology can help transform
the perceptions of data and theory in psychology.
The Cognitive-Experiential Self Theory (CEST) proposed by Seymour
Epstein (1993, 1994) suggests that when human beings function as knowing
persons, they utilize in an adaptive fashion two parallel but
interacting modes of information processing: (a) a rational system that
is primarily conscious and often free from affect, and (b) a largely
unconscious experiential system that is closely attached to affect. The
rational system is analytic, reason oriented, involves conscious
appraisals, encodes reality in abstract symbols and words, and is slower
in processing but more rapidly changes than the experiential system. In
contrast, the experiential system is holistic, affective (pleasure-pain)
oriented, involves associationistic connections, encodes reality in
concrete images, and is more rapid in processing but more slowly changes
than the rational system. Everyone utilizes both information processing
modes, though people differ in terms of how effective, how frequent, and
under what circumstances each mode might be utilized (Epstein, Pacini,
Denes-Raj, & Heier, 1996).
Despite common misconceptions, both religion and science are no
different than other types of human experience in that they employ both
the rational and experiential modes, though perhaps to varying degrees.
Epstein (1994), for example, contends that religion is well suited for
communicating within the experiential system and calls for psychologists
to investigate religion as a means to test CEST's distinction
between the two modes. Indeed, Ozorak's (1997) claim that religious
faith can be conceptualized as "an emotional landscape of
primordial strength and simplicity upon which we erect structures of
reasoned belief" (p. 199) argues well for both the primacy of the
experiential mode but also as well the subsequent utilization of the
rational mode in religious experience. In contrast, science may give
priority to the rational mode, though experiential mode processing is
far more involved that commonly presumed (Brown, 1977; Howard, 1985,
1986; Jones, 1994, Laudan, 1984).
A provocative aspect of CEST are four postulated implicit belief
systems, each associated with a basic human need. Each need requires
satisfaction, which is assessed primarily (but not entirely) on the
experiential level (hence, these are identified as "implicit"
belief systems). The four implicit belief systems with the related four
basic needs include: (a) the degree to which the world is perceived as
benevolent or malevolent (associated with the basic need to manage
pleasure and pain); (b) the extent to which the world is perceived as
meaningful or meaningless (associated with the need to develop a
coherent conceptual system); (c) the degree to which the self is worthy
or unworthy (related to the need for self-esteem); and (d) the extent to
which the social world is comforting, trustworthy and dependable versus
dangerous and undependable (associated with the need for relatedness).
Much empirical research by social and behavioral scientists over
the past several decades has focused on constructs that comprise these
belief systems and needs with little if any consideration of what
theology or philosophy has to offer, despite the fact that theologians
and philosophers have for millennia led the ongoing search for a
scholarly understanding of such basic human needs and beliefs. The
effort here is a small attempt to encourage others to address this
oversight by suggesting that religious experience and the theological
underpinnings of such experience have much to constructively offer a
psychological understanding of these implicit belief systems and their
associated needs.
1. The Degree to Which the World is Perceived as Benevolent or
Malevolent.
In psychological terms, this belief structure is perhaps most
thoroughly investigated in terms of research on optimism and pessimism.
Both optimism and pessimism are stable personality traits with
implications for physical and psychological health (Scheier &
Carver, 1985). Generally, research shows that optimists fare better than
pessimists on both health (Peterson, Seligman, & Vaillant, 1988;
Scheier & Carver, 1987; Seligman, 1991) and achievement related
outcomes (Peterson & Barrett, 1987), though researchers also caution
about the ill effects (e.g., unnecessary risk-taking, less vigilance) of
a naive optimism (Burger & Burns, 1988).
Does an intrinsic Christian faith promote either a sense of
optimism or pessimism? If so (in either direction), then what is it in
the faith experience that promotes such a greater sense? There is not a
conclusive empirical answer to the first question though one study
(Sethi & Seligrnan, 1993) suggests that a "fundamental"
type of religion (such as orthodox Judaism or Calvinism), may promote
more optimistic explanation of events.
With regard to the second question, most intrinsically religious
(i.e., where religious faith is one's
"mastermotive"--Allport, 1950) individuals see God as highly
active, utilizing multiple channels, and working through or with natural
factors including one's own behavior (Smith & Gorsuch, 1989).
Hill (1995) suggests that this particular pattern of attributional
logic, by providing a sense of hope especially when one faces adversity,
may prevent an over-whelming sense of pessimism.
The basic psychological need associated with this implicit belief
dimension, according to CEST, is the management of pleasure and pain.
Such a need has been postulated within the classical psychoanalytic
tradition as the most fundamental of all guiding principles for living.
Of course, in its most extreme and traditional form, the psychoanalytic
position sees pleasure as instinctually rooted in the singularity of the
individual unencumbered by the necessary requirements of social living.
In fact, the psychoanalytic perspective maintains that for many it is
through religious teachings that such social requirements are
unconsciously internalized and serve to counter the pleasures of
biological gratification.
The balance of pleasure over pain is a notion commonly used by
psychologists to conceptualize or even define happiness. For example,
Parducci (1995) defines happiness as a "theoretical summation or
average of the separate pleasures and pains experienced over whatever
period is being considered" (p. 19). But, of course, such a
definition is frustratingly elusive in that the terms
"pleasure" and "pain" are slippery with a multitude
of definitions and interpretations. For example, pleasure is often
associated with such hedonistic notions as wealth, achievement, freedom
from work, success, or power, yet Myers (1992) reports a plethora of
studies that suggests that such constructs are, at most (even when taken
in combination), only slightly correlated with happiness. So, if there
is legitimacy at all to defining happiness as some sort of experiential
ratio of pleasure to pain it is necessary to adopt a broader
understanding of pleasure than its commonly perceived association with
wealth, success, or leisure.
The need to manage pleasure and pain is also addressed
theologically. To the psychologist, however, theology adds an
interesting twist in that what is pleasurable may not necessarily be
defined as good. For example, it may be that the experientially-based
sense of pleasure is perceived by the Christian as either something good
or something bad. Though perhaps strongly felt, this evaluation, because
it is rooted in the arational experiential system of knowing (i.e.,
holistic, associationistic, crudely differentiated, self evidently
valid, etc.) may not be easily articulated. For some Christians on at
least some (but likely not all) issues, experiential knowing of a given
issue brings closure and subsequent rational processing, if done at all,
simply confirms the experientially-based judgment.
For example, though a study (a rational processing mode) of how the
word "pleasure" is used in the Bible suggests that the
experience itself can be morally positive (e.g., many scriptures refer
to God as a pleasure-loving God), the very idea may be viewed with
caution by some Christians through their experiential processing
mode--perhaps because of strong associationistic connections with a
"worldly-pleasure," or through concrete and stereotypical
images of "pleasurable" living (carousing, lust, etc.), or
through a general sense that it "just doesn't feel right"
to enjoy life's pleasures. Further cognitive elaboration by the
rational system may confirm (e.g., a concern about an excessive focus on
the self or that such pleasure can lead to idolatry) or qualify (e.g.,
we are created in the image of a God who finds pleasure in the creation
or that pleasure in the beauty of the creation brings one closer to the
Creator) the experiential assessment.
Bassett and Hill (1998) propose a model that links the study of
affect with specific Christian constructs. They suggest that for the
intrinsically committed Christian, two general dimensions of affect
well-established in the empirical literature (positive affect and
negative affect) are supplemented by the bi-polar dimension of
constructive and destructive affect which, in turn, are determined by
perceptions of God's character. That is, if the affective
experience is congruent with perceptions of God's character and
brings God pleasure, then it is constructive within the Christian
experience. On the other hand, if the experience is believed to be
incongruent with God's perceived character, it is a destructive
experience. The Bassett and Hill model maintains that all emotional
experiences have the capacity to be morally ambidextrous and are
typically assessed at both a reflective (the rational mode of knowing)
and nonreflective (the experiential mode of knowing) level.
Parducci's (1995) discussion of the simplified life of a nun,
Sister Chantel, provides an excellent example in demonstrating not only
the rational and experiential systems at work, but how such systems may
operate in the life of a committed believer. Parducci reports that
Sister Chantel has chosen the life of a hermit in a small cave in the
south of France to experience (in her own words) "a closer, more
loving relationship to God, without the distracting intellectual dogma
of the monastic order." Once a month she hitches a ride to the
nearest town to sell the woolen socks she has knitted since her last
market day, earning the necessary $30 to meet her basic needs until the
next monthly trip.
For Chantel, the presence of God is as real as the stone walls of her
cave--not as a schizophrenic who hears voices or has visions but as a
sensible woman who experiences this presence as a private, subjective
experience. And it is while engaging in the highly automatized activity
of knitting that she most often experiences this presence. The rest of
her life, the relative isolation from other people, the living in a
cave, the minimal participation in the market economy, all of this
simplifies her concentration upon her loving relationship with God. (p.
201)
2. The Degree to Which the World is Perceived as Predictable,
Controllable, and Meaningful Versus Chaotic and Meaningless.
The fundamental need associated with this belief structure is the
necessity for a coherent conceptual system to give meaning to events.
Baumeister's (1991) extensive review of the literature led him to
conclude that there are four characteristics crucial for psychological
well-being from making meaning: a sense of purpose, a need for value, a
sense of efficacy, and an affirmation of self-worth. I will briefly
discuss the first three of these characteristics, saving the last
characteristic for my discussion of CEST's third belief structure.
A Sense of Purpose. The need for meaning is found in people's
desires for their lives to have purpose, which psychologists have sorted
into two broad categories: goals and fulfillments (see Baumeister,
1991). Goals are extrinsically motivated to bring about future desired
circumstances and serve to organize present activities, even when
unpleasant. My goal of completing this article, for example, dictates
that I remain in my office to write rather than to play golf.
Fulfillments, on the other hand, are intrinsically motivated and
typically involve some form of positive affect that often accompanies
the reaching of such goals. Thus, I remain in my office to write because
of my expected sense of accomplishment when this article is finally
finished. In this sense, fulfillment is better understood as a type of
idea--some subjective notion of an ideal state rather than an objective
condition. Baumeister (1991) concludes that "It may not be in human
nature to find lasting fulfillment in the present" (p. 34).
Given that higher levels of meaning are associated with broader
timeframes (Vallacher & Wegner, 1985, 1987), religious thinking
allows one to be aware of events and actions, including those
surrounding one's self, at the highest and broadest possible levels
(i.e., matters of "ultimate concerns"--Emmons, 1999). To
glorify and enjoy God (the absolute standard of being from which all of
creation, including one's self, emanates) forever, as the chief end
of humanity according to the Shorter Catechism, thus creates the
ultimate purposive context for making sense of anything that happens. It
is this expansive and decisive nature of religious explanation that
allows even the most banal of life events the capability of being imbued
with religious meaning.
How might such religious thinking, rooted in such a fundamental
theological construct as the chief end of humanity, supplement and even
transform the psychological understanding of meaning-as-purpose? Three
ideas come to mind. First, religious thinking addresses
Baumeister's (1991) concern about the elusive nature of
fulfillment. Christianity teaches that fulfillment is more than an idea;
fulfillment is an actual experience, potentially available to anyone,
found in one's eternally-based (which, presumably, includes the
present) glorification and enjoyment of God. Hence, a
theologically-informed psychology suggests that humans are capable of
direct and tangible experiences of fulfillment, though such an
experience is perhaps tainted by humanity's fallen condition.
Second, if meaning and purpose are indeed important to human
existence as Baumeister (1991) contends, and if religion is capable of
providing answers about meaning and purpose, then religious belief and
experience may be something that people really care about. Not only does
this suggest that religion is worthy of study in its own right by
psychologists, but religion may also provide psychologists a topic upon
which people hold strong belief, something valuable to the study of
basic psychological processes. For example, Abelson (1988) proposes that
the historical disarray of scientific research on attitudes is that
researchers have not devoted "enough attention to those attitudes
that make a difference to people and society, attitudes that people hold
with some degree of conviction" (p. 267). By arguing that the study
of strong belief has much to offer the field of psychology, Abelson
recommends religion as an important, yet heretofore overlooked, domain
for attitudinal research.
Third, Emmons (1999) demonstrates how an understanding of meaning
and purpose experienced through spiritual and religious themes (what he
calls "spiritual strivings") has important implications for
such psychological processes as personality integration and setting
goals in coping with stress. That is, once religious meaning is
established, it can be useful in understandings of oneself and how one
can effectively cope with environmental demands.
This is what Maclntyre (1981) means when he suggests that the best
way to understand a person's life is to think of it as a story
within the context of a larger story and that the unity of life is found
in the unity of a narrative quest. To see one's life as a sensible
plot not only has the power to explain, but perhaps more importantly,
the power to sustain (Kilpatrick, 1987).
A Need for Value. Baumeister (1991) contends that values,
understood as the legitimization or justification of behavior, are an
important aspect of meaning and that religion, as a value base,
"still holds strong appeal to individuals" (p. 84). Recent
developments in psychology, prompted by the former American
Psychological Association President Martin Seligman's (1998)
observation and critique that "while plumbing the depths of what is
worst in life, psychology lost its connection to the positive side of
life--the knowledge about what makes life most worth living, most
fulfilling, most enjoyable and most productive" (p. 2), point to
new research agendas investigating human strengths and virtues. Religion
is far more familiar with the study of virtues than psychology.
In fact, how a person conceptualizes what constitutes a virtue is part
of a larger value system or "grammar" that is frequently defined, often
explicitly, by a religious tradition. To say that Christian gratitude or
Aristotelian pride or Rogerian congruence each have a "grammar" is just
to say that the concepts of these virtues differ in determinate ways
which can be expressed in rule-like formulae specifying the connections
and disconnections of these virtues with other virtues, beliefs,
experiences, emotions, motives, actions, and so forth ... To know the
grammar of a virtue is to have a schematic notion of the kind of "life"
lived by someone who possesses the virtue in question. (Roberts, 1987,
p. 193)
Thus, for example, to understand how one might practice a life
defined by Aquinas' four "Cardinal" virtues of prudence,
justice, temperance, and fortitude, one must decipher Aquinas'
grammar of what these virtues consist.
A Sense of Efficacy. Baumeister (1991) contends that through our
sense of meaning, we are better able to predict and control our world
around us. While this is often one of our justifications for science,
which is quite adept at predicting and controlling the natural
environment, Baumeister also recognizes that science is less capable
than religion in regulating how we emotionally and behaviorally react to
our environment, which often provides a subjective sense of control.
"If people turn away from their religion, they will lose the
powerful consolations, ecstasies, and moral certainties that religion
can offer" (p. 183).
Tenets of the Christian faith, as well as the tenets of other
religious traditions, can provide adherents three different senses of
control: interpretive control, predictive control, and vicarious control
(Hood, Spilka, Hunsberger, & Gorsuch, 1996). The idea that we can
find joy in the midst of suffering (Isa. 12:3--interpretive control),
that God provides signs for future events (Isa. 7:14--predictive
control), and that all things are accomplished through the will of God
(Jas 4:15--vicarious control) are examples of each.
3. The Degree to Which One is Perceived as Competent, Good, and
Worthy of Respect and Love.
The associated need with this belief structure is something
commonly discussed in western culture--the need for high self-esteem;
that is, the need to positively evaluate the totality of inferences that
a person makes about him/herself. This affirmation of self-worth is also
one of the four crucial elements identified by Baumeister (1991) for
making meaning out of our lives.
Perhaps when trying to understand why the need for high self-esteem
is so important (besides the simple fact that people like to feel good
about themselves) a brief review of a few major findings is most
enlightening. First, people desire to maintain consistent views of
themselves (Swann, 1987) and people with higher self-esteem seem to be
more successful in maintaining such consistency (Campbell, 1990).
Second, people with higher self-esteem appear to have more
self-knowledge (or at least think they do), with the certainty of such
knowledge associated with positive affect (Baumgardner, 1990). Third,
people with high self-esteem are more adaptive to initial failure,
either by trying harder (autoplastic adaptation) or by avoiding
(sometimes cleverly) the same task (alloplastic adaptation) in the
future (Baumeister & Tice, 1985). This is true despite the fact that
both high and low self-esteem individuals demonstrate about the same
degree of success or failure; therefore, self-esteem is not necessarily
an accurate assessment of one's competence level (Campbell, 1981).
Fourth, high self-esteem individuals appear better at setting goals at
appropriate levels of difficulty and are more able to follow through on
goal-related commitments (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1993).
Finally, high self-esteem is a central component to successful coping
behavior (Silver & Wortman, 1980).
Given this review, is there anything problematic with having high
self-esteem? As long as everything is going well, research has uncovered
little wrong with higher levels of self-esteem. However, once someone
with high self-esteem experiences a challenging threat to their ego over
which they have little perceived control, their ability to respond
positively is often severely reduced and they may begin to demonstrate
the self-defeating forms of behavior associated with lower self-esteem
(Baumeister et al., 1993). Still, however, on balance, the psychological
literature suggests that a higher level of self-esteem is both
indicative of and a contributor one's psychological well being.
In a review of 22 studies that have empirically investigated the
relationship between religion and self-esteem, Gartner (1996) found that
seven studies identified a positive relationship, five studies uncovered
a negative relationship, and ten studies found no relationship between
the two measures. Given the different measures of religious commitment
used (Gartner, 1996) as well as the conceptual confusion and
definitional inconsistency of the self-esteem construct (Harter, 1983)
and the potential pitfalls using traditional measures of self-esteem
with a religious population (Watson, Hood, Morris, & Hall, 1985), it
is little wonder that such inconsistent results have been found.
Though self-esteem is conceptually fuzzy, research now suggests
that competency, autonomy, and relatedness (Connell & Wellborn,
1991; Harter, 1983; Ryan, Connell & Deci, 1985) play pivotal roles
in its development and maintenance. In an effort to define and measure
self-esteem among Christians, Bassett, Singleton, and Altman (1996)
found that the importance of identifying "spiritual gifts"
(competence), the freedom and accountability for personal choices
(autonomy), and both the privilege and responsibility of acceptance
within a religious body (relatedness) are all contributors to the
Christian's general level of self-esteem.
4. That people are supporting, accepting, and trustworthy versus
threatening, rejecting, and untrustworthy.
The need for a close relationship with others is associated with
this belief structure. Though tainted by sin, relationships with others
are highly valued in the Bible. For example, Christians are described as
brothers and sisters (Jas. 2:15) and believers as the family of God (Mt.
23:8). As family members, we are called on to love one another (1 Cor.
13; Lev. 19:18), to show compassion and concern for those less fortunate
(Jas. 1:27; 1 Pet. 3:8), to refrain from social discriminations (Rom.
1:14; Jas. 2:1-4), to confess our sins to each other (Jas. 5:16), and to
forgive those who have sinned against us (Gen. 50:17-21). From a
Christian perspective, fundamental to all understanding of maturity and
completeness as an individual is one's relationship to God. Hence,
in contrast to a humanistic psychology's emphasis on the
authenticity and self-actualization of a singularly isolated individual,
the Christian view is that "Life is relationship. Death is
fundamentally separation, not cessation. Separation from God or others
is the product of sin and nowhere more clearly illustrated than in Adam
and Eve's separation from God's presence in the Garden with
the resulting mutual alienation" (Carter & Barnhurst, 1986, p.
4). Though this relationship is frequently stressed in scripture and by
theology through a rational mode of knowing, there is a personal and
experiential aspect to this relationship as well, best characterized by
the "fruits of the spirit" (Gal. 5:22-23). What results,
ideally, is a spiritual mode of being that flows out of an I-Thou
relationship (Buber, 1970) characterized by mutual giving and openness,
intimacy, sense of vulnerability, desire to spend time together, and
expression of affection. Trust and faith grows through this
relationship, ever producing greater stability in the relationship
itself (Carter & Barnhurst, 1986) and thereby helping meet the need
for relatedness.
CONCLUSION
The focus of this article has been the role of scriptural authority
for those Christians who seriously desire to engage their professional
discipline. It is argued that the boundary between psychology and
theology is one where scriptural authority cannot simply be declared,
but must also be demonstrated, for at this boundary the two disciplines
have much constructively to say to each other. It is up to those who
live on the boundary to demonstrate that Christianity has something
worthwhile to offer the field of psychology on its terms. I have tried
to provide but one small example through Epstein's (1993, 1994)
CEST model where Christian thinking has much to offer the field. Whether
psychology is willing to engage evangelical scholarship is, of course, a
pertinent question and one with mixed signals. It is, however, only as
evangelical psychologists accept the challenge to demonstrate and not
simply declare scriptural authority that they will have earned a
hearing.
REFERENCES
Abelson, R. P. (1988). Conviction. American Psychologist, 43,
267-275.
Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his religion. New York:
Macmillan.
Barbour, I. (1990). Religion in an age of science. San Francisco:
HarperSanFrancisco.
Bassett, R. L., & Hill, P. C. (1998). The ACE model of
emotions. Living Jesus Christ while experiencing emotions. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 26, 232-246.
Bassett, R. L., Singleton, M., & Altman, J. H. (1996, August).
One attempt to measure the interplay of religion and self-esteem. Paper
presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association,
Toronto, Ontario.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford
Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1993).
When ego-threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences
of high self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64,
141-156.
Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1985). Self-esteem and
responses to success and failure: Subsequent performance and intrinsic
motivation. Journal of Personality, 53, 450-467.
Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself:
Self-certainty and self-affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 58, 1062-1072.
Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A. &
Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment
in American life. New York: Harper & Row.
Brand, J. L. (1997). Challenges for a Christian psychology from
cognitive science. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 16, 233-246.
Brown, H. (1977). Theory, perception, and commitment: The new
philosophy of science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Buber, M. (1970). I and thou (Walter Kaufmann, Trans.). New York:
CharlesScribner and Sons.
Burger, J. M. & Burns, L. (1988). The illusion of unique
invulnerability and the use of effective contraception. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 264-270.
Campbell, A. (1981). The sense of well-being in America. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and the clarity of the
self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538-549.
Campbell, D. T. (1976). On the conflicts between biological and
social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American
Psychologist, 30, 1103-1126.
Carter, J. D. (1996). Success without finality: The continuing
dialogue of faith and psychology. Journal of Psychology and
Christianity, 15, 116-122.
Carter, J. D., & Barnhurst, L. F. (1986, October). Maturity,
intimacy and spirituality. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Christian Association for Psychological Studies-Eastern Region,
Speculator, NY.
Carter, J. D., & Narramore, S. B. (1979). The integration of
psychology and theology. Grand Rapids, MI: Zondervan.
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, autonomy,
and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self-system processes. In M.
R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Self processes and development: The
Minnesota Symposium on Child Development (Vol. 23, pp 43-77) Hillsdale,
NJ Erlbaum.
Eccles & Robinson (1984). The wonder of being human: Our brain
and our mind New York: The Free Press.
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns:
Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York. Guilford Press.
Epstein, S. (1993). Emotion and self-theory. In M. Lewis & J.
M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp 313-326). New York:
Guilford Press.
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the
psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724.
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996).
Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational
thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
390-405.
Gallup, G., Jr. (1994). The Gallup poll: Public opinion 1993.
Wilmington. DE: Scholarly Resources.
Gallup, G., Jr., & Castelli, J. (1989). The people's
religion. New York: Macmillan.
Gartner, J. (1996). Religious commitment, mental health, and
prosocial behavior: A review of the empirical literature. In E. P.
Shafranske (Ed.), Religion and the clinical practice of psychology (pp.
187-214). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern
psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 266-275.
Gergen, K. J. (1994). Exploring the postmodern. Perils or
potentials? American Psychologist, 49, 412-416.
Grenz, S. J. (1996). A primer on postmodernism. Grand Rapids, MI:
Eerdmans.
Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system.
In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. IV.
Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 275-386). New
York: Wiley & Sons.
Henry, C. F. H. (Ed.) (1966). Jesus of Nazareth: Savior and Lord.
Grand Rapids: Eerdmans.
Hill, P. C. (1995). Affect theory and religious experience. In R.
W. Hood, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of religious experience (pp. 353-377)
Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press.
Hill, P. C. (1999). Giving religion away: What the study of
religion offers psychology. The International Journal for the Psychology
of Religion, 9, 229-249.
Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., Jr., McCullough, M. E.,
Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., et al. (2000). Conceptualizing religion
and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal
for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 30, 51-77.
Hood, R. W., Spilka, B., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (1996).
The psychology of religion: An empirical approach (2nd. ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Howard, G. S. (1985) The role of values in the science of
psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 255-265.
Howard, G. S. (1986). Dare we develop a human science? Notre Dame,
IN: Academic Publications.
Hunter, J. D. (1983). American evangelicalism: Conservative
religion and the quandary of modernity. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University Press.
Jones, S. L. (1994) A constructive relationship for religion with
the science and profession of psychology: Perhaps the boldest model yet.
American Psychologist, 49, 184-199.
Jones, S. L. (1996). Reflections on the nature and future of the
Christian psychologies. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 15,
133-142.
Kilpatrick, W. K. (1987). Why secular psychology is not enough. In
T. J. Burke (Ed.), Man and mind: A Christian theory of personality (pp.
133-147). Hillsdale, MI: Hillsdale College Press.
Larson, D. B., Swyers, J. P., & McCullough, M. E. (Eds.)
(1998). Scientific research on spirituality and health: A consensus
report. Bethesda, MD: National Institute for Healthcare Research.
Laudan, L. (1984). Science and values: The aims of science and
their role in scientific debate. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1981). After virtue: A study in moral theory.
London: Duckworth.
Manicas, P. T., & Secord, P. F. (1983). Implications for
psychology of the new philosophy of science. American Psychologist, 38,
399-413.
Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness: Discovering the
pathway to fulfillment, well-being, and enduring personal joy. New York:
Avon Books.
Ozorak, E. W. (1997). In the eye of the beholder: A
social-cognitive model of religious belief. In B. Spilka & D. M.
McIntosh (Eds.), The psychology of religion: Theoretical approaches (pp.
194-203). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Packer, J. I. (1958). "Fundamentalism" and the World of
God: Some evangelical principles. Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans.
Packer, J. I. (1996). Truth and power: The place of Scripture in
the Christian life. Downers Grove, IL: InterVarsity Press.
Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and judgment: The
contextual theory and its applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pargament, K. I. (1999). The psychology of religion and
spirituality? Yes and no. The International Journal for the Psychology
of Religion, 9(1), 3-16.
Peterson, C., & Barrett, L. C. (1987). Explanatory style and
academic performance among university freshmen. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 53, 603-607.
Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988).
Pessimistic explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness: A
thirty-five year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 55, 23-27.
Pinnock, C. H. (1971). Biblical revelation: The foundation of
Christian theology. Chicago: Moody Press.
Richards, P. S., & Bergin, A. E. (1997). A spiritual strategy
for counseling and psychotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Roberts, R. C. (1987). Psychotherapeutic virtues and the grammar of
faith. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 15, 191-204.
Rogers, W. R. (1977, October). The dynamics of psychology and
religion: Teaching in a dialogical field. Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion, Chicago,
IL.
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). A
motivational analysis of self-determination and self-regulation in
education. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in
education: Vol. 1. The classroom milieu (pp. 13-52). Orlando: Academic
Press.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and
health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.
Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional optimism
and physical well-being: The influence of generalized outcome
expectancies on health. Journal of Personality, 55, 169-210.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York, Knopf.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1998, April). Positive social science. APA
Monitor, pp. 2, 5.
Sethi, S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). Optimism and
fundamentalism. Psychological Science, 4, 256-259.
Sheldrake, P. (1992). Spirituality and history: Questions of
interpretation and method. New York: Crossroads.
Silk, M. (1998). Something new, something old: Changes and
continuities in American religious history. Cambridge, MA: The President
and Fellows of Harvard College.
Silver, R. L., & Wortman, C. B. (1980). Coping with undesirable
events. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness:
Theory and applications (pp. 279-343). New York: Academic Press.
Smith, C. S., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1989). Sanctioning and causal
attributions to God: A function of theological position and actors'
characteristics. In M. L. Lynn & D. O. Moberg (Eds.), Research in
the social scientific study of religion, Vol. 1. (pp. 133-152).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Sperry, R. W. (1988). Psychology's mentalist paradigm and the
religion/science tension. American Psychologist, 43, 607-613.
Swann, W. B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1038-1051.
Tillich, P. (1952). The courage to be. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Turner, R. P., Lukoff, D., Barnhouse, R. T., & Lu, F. G.
(1995). Religious or spiritual problem: A culturally sensitive
diagnostic category in the DSM-IV. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 183, 435-444.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A theory of action
identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think
they're doing: Action identification and human behavior.
Psychological Review, 94, 3-15.
Van Leeuwen, M. S. (1996). Five uneasy questions or: Will success
spoil Christian psychologists? Journal of Psychology and Christianity,
15, 150-160.
Vande Kemp, H. (1996). Psychology and Christian spirituality:
Explorations of the inner world. Journal of Psychology and Christianity,
15, 161-174.
Watson, P. J., Hood, R. W., Morris, R. J., & Hall, J. R.
(1985). Religiosity, sin, and self-esteem. Journal of Psychology and
Theology, 13, 116-128.
Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K. I., Cole, B., Rye, M. S., Butter,
E. M., Belavich, T. G., et al. (1997). Religion and spirituality:
Unfuzzying the fuzzy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36,
549-564.
PETER C. HILL
Rosemead School of Psychology
Biola University
AUTHOR
HILL, PETER C., Address: Rosemead School of Psychology, 13800 Biola
Ave., La Mirada, CA 90639. Title: Professor. Degrees: BA, MA, PhD,
University of Houston. Specializations: Psychology of religion, positive
psychology, religious measurement.
This paper was presented at the Scripture and the Disciplines
Conference, Wheaton College, May 25th, 2004. Correspondence concerning
this article may be sent to Peter C. Hill, PhD, Roscmead School of
Psychology, Biola University, 13800 Biola Ave, La Mirada, CA 90639.
Email:
[email protected]