Humility, religion, and spirituality: an endpiece.
Davis, Don E. ; Hook, Joshua, N.
In this conclusion to the special issue, we reflect on the state of
the field of humility, religion, and spirituality, and examine three
important questions. First, we examine how researchers are defining
humility, and look for areas of consensus. Second, we address
measurement issues, and explore how researchers are measuring humility.
Third, we set a research agenda and examine some high priority areas for
future work on humility, religion, and spirituality.
Just a few years ago, we reviewed the literature on humility and
the field was relatively stagnant compared to research on other virtues
such as forgiveness and gratitude (Davis, Worthington, & Hook,
2010). Most studies focused on a story about seemingly intractable
measurement issues. Three years later, something has shifted. A Psych
Info search on December 4th, 2013 on humility in the last three years
revealed almost 100 published articles. Furthermore, emerging theories
and definitions of humility place it in close proximity to several
thriving literatures in the psychology of religion and spirituality,
such as how religion may promote better health, prosociality, or
prejudice (Galen, 2012a, 2012b; Myers, 2012; Saro-glou, 2012). To
conclude this special issue, we want to take stock by addressing three
questions. First, we explore how researchers are defining forgiveness,
and search for possible consensus. Second, we give an update on
measurement issues in the field of humility. Third, we set a research
agenda and describe some priorities for future research on humility,
religion, and spirituality.
Defining Humility
Just a few years ago, we concluded that the study of humility was
making slow progress due in part to problems defining humility (Davis et
al., 2010). Three years later, we noted that there was considerable
overlap in how humility is being operationalized. In this special issue,
researchers generally agreed that humility involves both intrapersonal
and interpersonal components. Researchers agreed that the intrapersonal
component of humility involves an accurate view of self They also agreed
that humility involves an interpersonal component, but there was less
consensus on what that involved (e.g., other-orientedness, respect).
In fact, researchers raised the issue that various subdomains of
humility--such as intellectual humility (Gregg & Mahadevan, 2014;
Hopkin, Hoyle, & Toner, 2014; McElroy et al., 2014) and cultural
humility (e.g., Hook, Davis, Owen, Worthington, & Utsey, 2013; Owen
et al., 2014)--may not be strongly related to each other. The
distinction between intellectual humility and general humility is
particularly new to psychology. Philosophy has recently discussed a
variety of epistemic virtues (Baehr, 2011), one of which is intellectual
humility (Roberts & Wood, 2003). Articles in this special issue
provided an initial sampling of how one might justify this distinction.
One strategy treated the construct of humility as a general
characteristic with several subdomains such as intellectual humility
(McElroy et al., 2014). Namely, if humility is related to
self-regulation of egotism, then the subdomains of humility may involve
various situations that may strain one's ability to express
humility. Intellectual humility, in particular, involves the ability to
regulate one's need to appear "right" or
"correct" in regard to one's beliefs or ideas. From this
framework, humility is not necessarily a trait but perhaps more a set of
abilities related to self-regulation across a variety of contexts. For
most people, high degrees of humility in one domain may apply to other
domains, but some people may have major discrepancies in one particular
area (e.g., a religious/spiritual leader may be humble in his or her
relationship with a partner, but lack intellectual humility when
discussing theological issues). Thus, it is an empirical question
whether these different domains suggest the presence of a general
humility or involve different constructs.
A second strategy not only distinguished intellectual humility from
general humility, but it also divided intellectual humility into
subdomains (e.g., religious, political) in which people sometimes hold
strong convictions (Hopkin et al., 2014). Thus, intellectual humility
may differ depending on the salient identity. This approach is certainly
compatible with McElroy et al.'s (2014) theorizing that several
conditions (e.g., negotiating convictions that are used to judge
loyalty) may strain the practice of intellectual humility.
A third strategy involved aligning intellectual humility with prior
theoretical work on self-enhancement and self-esteem (Gregg &
Mahadevan, 2014). Namely, intellectual humility was defined as having an
accurate view of self in reference to epistemic abilities. This approach
seemed most conservative in terms of trying to focus intellectual
humility on what happens inside a person (intrapsychic qualities). From
this perspective, what happens inside a person is more essential to
humility, and the interpersonal effects are considered more correlative.
This position has several strengths. For example, it aligns humility
with several fields (e.g., self-esteem, self-efficacy) that already have
ample theory and empirical evidence for focusing on subdomains. In
addition, it is a parsimonious way to distinguish humility from
constructs such as narcissism or interpersonal modesty.
Toward Consolidating Definitions
The conceptual expansion of definitions is a natural part of growth
in an emerging, interdisciplinary field. The danger is always that
definitions may fail to converge, or worse, fail to clarify that a
variety of measures purporting to measure humility are actually
measuring different constructs (a common critique of work on
religion/spirituality; e.g., Hill & Pargament, 2008). For example,
the different subdomains of humility may not always be strongly related
to each other (e.g., Jankowski & Sandage, 2014). To avoid this
problem, scientists can draw on theory and empirical strategies to
consolidate definitions.
We can consolidate definitions by conceptualizing humility within a
broader theoretical framework. In the current issue, authors drew upon
theories of evolution (Gregg & Mahadevan, 2014), attachment
(Dwi-wardani et al., 2014; Jankowski & Sandage, 2014), and
self-regulation, altruism, and commitment (McElroy et al., 2014). Their
focus was not necessarily on advancing consensus, but these theories may
help in that regard. For example, based on attachment theory, one might
map the subdomains of humility within the 2 (positive, negative) x 2
(self, other) grid used to describe working models of self and other.
From this perspective, humility involves the ability to balance the
needs of self and other well, derived from having an accurate view of
self and of others.
We can also examine empirical studies in order to consolidate
definitions. One place to start is simply by seeing how researchers have
actually operationalized humility. In Table 1, we examined the content
overlap in 13 published (or widely used) measures of humility. We can
address several points for future dialogue. First, in practice, humility
researchers appear to be including intrapersonal modesty (i.e., accurate
view of self) and interpersonal modesty (i.e., moderating and sharing
positive attention) as subdomains of general humility. Thus, we may need
to revisit the critique that early measures of humility (i.e., NEO-PI-3;
McCrae, Costa, & Martin, 2005; HEXICO-PI; Lee & Ashton, 2004;
VIA-SI; Peterson & Seligman, 2004) conflated humility and modesty.
Modesty is likely an important subdomain of humility. Second, humility
researchers have consistently included interpersonal behavior (i.e.,
other-orientedness, lack of superiority) in their operational
definitions of humility. Thus, although some humility researchers may
seek to focus their definitions on the intrapersonal aspects of
humility, in practice most researchers are operationally defining
humility using both intrapersonal and interpersonal components. Third,
there are clearly some subdomains of humility that are less frequently
addressed, such as spirituality, regulation of greed or status-seeking,
or modesty of dress. Thus, we suspect that the introduction of
distinctions such as intellectual and cultural humility will generate
broader models to integrate the various subdomains of humility.
TABLE 1
Summary of Content Themes in Humility Measures
Humility Scale OO O/LS IM AYS AM/T RNS SE
Cultural humility scale X X
(Hookct al., 2013)
Dispositional Humility X X X X X
Scale (Landrum et al.,
2011)
Expressed Humility Scale X X X
(Owens, Johnson, &
Mitchell, 2012)
Honesty humility scale X X
(Ashton & Lee, 2004)
Humility Differentials X X X
(Rowatt et al., 2006)
Humility Inventory (Brown X X X X X
et al., 2013)
Humility subscale X X X X
(Dierendonck & Nuijten,
2013)
Implicit Associations Test X X X X
(Rowatt et al., 2006)
Intellectual humility X X X X X
scale (McElroy et al.,
2014)
Modesty factor of the X X X
NEO-PI-3 (Costa et al.,
2005)
Relational humility scale X X X X
(Davis et al.. 2011)
Rosemcad Humility Scale X X X X X
(Bollinger et al., 2006)
Spiritual Humility Scale X X
(Davis et al., 2008)
Values in Action Strengths X X
Inventory (Peterson &
Seligman, 2004)
Note. Grey box = content included on the scale; 00 = Other-oriented
/unselfish; OILS = Openness/lack of superiority; IM = interpersonal
modesty; AVS = accurate view of self AM/T = willing to admit mistakes
/teachable; RNS = regulation ofneed for status; SE = spiritual or
existential humility.
Progress Report on Measuring Humility
Several years ago the research on humility seemed to be stagnant,
with most articles preoccupied with so called intractable measurement
issues (Davis et al., 2010). Namely, many researchers questioned the
validity of self-report measures of humility, because describing oneself
as "very humble" seemed akin to bragging. Based on a potential
response bias, called "the modesty effect," researchers
hypothesized that the more humble someone really is, the less they would
self-enhance (and perhaps even self-deprecate) when describing their own
humility on self-report measures. We have argued that this assumption is
empirically testable, and since then researchers have seemed more
comfortable assuming that self-report measures of humility are at least
somewhat valid until there is stronger evidence to the contrary.
In the articles of the special issue, researchers used a variety of
strategies to assess humility: (a) two articles developed new measures
of intellectual humility (Hopkin et al., 2014; McElroy et al., 2014);
(b) two articles used the Rosemead Humility Scale (Bollinger, Kopp,
Hill, & Williams, 2006); (c) two articles used the Values in Action
Inventory (Peterson & Seligman, 2004); and (d) one article used
humility-differential items (Rowatt et at, 2006). In terms of
multi-method assessment of humility, most studies used self-report
measures of humility. Three studies utilized other-report measures of
humility (McElroy et at, 2014; Owen et at, 2014; Rowatt, Kang, Haggard,
& LaBouff, 2014) and one study used a behavioral measure (Van
Ton-geren et al., 2014). Only one study used multi-method assessment of
humility within the same study (Rowatt et at, 2014, who used both self-
and other-report measures of humility). Overall, we are encouraged by
the advances in the measurement of humility, especially strategies using
multiple methods of assessment.
The range of strategies used in this review is also reflective of
the broader field. Several new self-report measures have been published
(e.g., Brown, Chopra, & Schiraldi, 2013; Landrum, 2011; van
Dierendonck & Nuijten, 2011). We are hopeful that self-report
measures of humility and response bias are relatively circumscribed to
people with highest or perhaps lowest levels of humility and can be
sufficiently minimized using strategies to minimize response bias. We
will soon have clearer answers to these questions, as researchers
continue to use multi-method strategies. In addition to other-reports,
we encourage researchers to employ behavioral strategies to measure
humility, such as observational coding or experimental manipulations of
humility-related behavior. Van Tongeren and colleagues took a step in
this direction, operationalizing humility by examining people's
ability to regulate defensive reactions to a provocative essay. We
anticipate an important discussion within personality and social
psychology regarding the experimental paradigms (e.g., self-enhancement,
ego depletion) that can be applied to study humility.
Humility and Religion/Spirituality: Future Research Directions
The articles in the present special issue gave an overview of some
very important topics in the study of the intersection of humility,
religion, and spirituality. Based on these articles, as well as our own
view of the state of the literature, we provide a research agenda for
future study on the intersection of humility, religion, and
spirituality. In particular, we urge researchers to consider how
humility is related to several important debates within the psychology
of religion and spirituality.
1. How is humility related to relationships with others, especially
those who are religiously different? A major theme in the psychology has
been work on the degree to which religion and spirituality are prosocial
(Bloom, 2012; Galen, 2012a, 2012b; Myers, 2012; Saroglou, 2012). This
dialogue has focused on altruism as well as prejudice. We suspect that a
similar debate will resurface in the study of humility. On the one hand,
most world religions promote humility as a virtue and have various
rituals that call individuals to commit to something larger than the
self, including worship of the Sacred, as well as belonging to a
spiritual community. Religions teach adherents to forgo immediate
self-interest for the benefits of belonging to a tight-knit religious
community, which can have many advantages in terms of relationships,
physical health, mental health, and self-regulation (Baumeister, Bauer,
& Lloyd, 2010). But at the same time, religious convictions can
escalate personal or political conflicts, and there is a robust
literature on how certain kinds of religiosity--especially those related
to respect for authority--may facilitate moral disengagement and
prejudice (for a review, see Hall, Matz, & Wood, 2010).
2. How does humility affect religious/spiritual coping? Tragedies
often shift people's perspective of the Sacred (Park, 2010). If
humility is indeed linked to self-regulation, then low levels of
humility may interfere with people's ability to admit limitations
in their worldview and adaptively reconstruct other perspectives.
Following a tragedy, individuals may get stuck trying to put the
shattered pieces of their worldview (including the experience of anger
towards the Sacred; Gruggs & Exline, 2014) back together instead of
tolerating the anxiety of seeking a new sense of meaning--or in a
spiritual context, new ways of understanding and relating to the sacred
(Sandage, Hill, & Vaubel, 2011).
3. How robust are the benefits of humility? Our research programs
are currently focused on three hypotheses (see Davis & Hook, 2013).
In the social bonds hypothesis, we hypothesize that humility helps
regulate social bonds, causing people to think of themselves as part of
a "we" that allows them to enjoy sacrificing for a
relationship. In the social oil hypothesis, we hypothesize that humility
may help buffer competitive traits that would otherwise cause
deterioration in a relationship. In the humility-health hypothesis, we
predict that humility is associated with enhanced self-regulation, which
leads to downstream benefits for coping and overall mental and physical
health. The degree of evidence supporting these hypotheses is limited.
Most studies have been limited by weak research designs using only one
form of measurement (e.g., other-report). Future research must provide
more rigorous and falsifiable tests of the benefits of humility.
4. How do contextual factors affect the perception of humility?
Recent work has emphasized the contextual nature of many virtues. A
virtue practiced at the wrong time can do harm. For example, forgiveness
is generally a good thing, but to forgive well requires someone to also
have the ability to set appropriate boundaries on reconciliation (see
McNulty & Fincham, 2012). In this regard, a potential dark side of
humility is that cultures may apply different norms for humility based
on gender or other aspects of identity. Based our model of relational
humility, social judgments of humility are based on normative and
contextual standards. For example, if gender norms cause people to
expect females, on average, to be more cooperative and less assertive
than males, this may cause systematic bias in how people interpret
behavior. Given than gender norms are often affected by religious
involvement, this is a ripe area for future work in the psychology of
religion and spirituality.
5. How do religious/spiritual beliefs or values affect the
perception of humility in self and others? As noted in several studies
in the special issue, the relationship between humility, religion, and
spirituality is complex. For example, Rowatt et al. (2014) found
positive correlations between humility and some religious constructs,
but nonsignificant relationships for other religious constructs. Hopkin
et al. (2014) found low levels of intellectual humility for both
individuals who showed strong support for religious beliefs and
individuals who showed strong opposition to religious beliefs. In what
we call the humility-values hypothesis, we theorize that constructs such
as moral foundations (Haidt, 2007) or values (Schwartz, 1992) may affect
the kind of evidence people consider relevant to social judgments of
humility. For example, someone who is more politically conservative may
base social judgments of humility on values associated with respect for
authority, loyalty, and purity, whereas someone who is more politically
or theologically liberal may base social judgments of humility on values
related to care, fairness, and universality.
6. What is the importance of humility for religious/spiritual
leaders? The role of a religious/spiritual leader can strain humility at
different levels. Based on theorizing on social capital, one role of
religious leaders is to promote "bonding" social capital
within their community (Hopkins, 2011). This task involves emphasizing
certain belief, rituals, and traditions that promote cohesion and
consolidate commitment among members. For example, leaders may promote
costly behaviors that signal commitment. They may choose to emphasize
particular beliefs (e.g., abortion, sexual ethics) as the basis for
identity within the community. People in the community may view it as
disloyal and arrogant when individuals question these beliefs.
Convictions often become polarized and take on a life of their own when
they represent not just ideas but also are used to diagnose loyalty to
the group.
A second role of religious leaders is to promote
"bridging" social capital (Hopkins, 2011). The costs and
benefits of bridging and seeking alliances with other groups is complex
and affected by a variety of factors, such as characteristics of the
leader, location of the community, and competing groups in the area.
Bridging involves a trade-off that requires regulation. If one
overemphasizes bonding capital, then one's community may become
increasingly insular and marginalized within a community. But bridging
is a high-risk, high-reward strategy. One may invest and gain alliances
within the broader-community, but this may decrease solidarity or
cohesion within the group.
7. How might one work on developing humility in the domain of
religion and spirituality? As counseling psychologists, we are
interested in helping individuals develop humility in their lives,
especially related to how one holds religious and spiritual beliefs,
values, and convictions. As noted by several authors in this issue
(e.g., Hopkin et al., 2014; Van Tongeren, 2014), humility regarding
religious convictions or toward individuals who are religiously
different may be especially difficult to develop or practice. Lavelock
and colleagues (2014) provided an initial investigation on a workbook
intervention to promote humility. Future research might evaluate the
extent to which these types of activities are effective for promoting
humility in the context of religious and spiritual beliefs, values, and
convictions. Furthermore, there could be other types of interventions
and activities that could be especially important to developing humility
about one's religious convictions. For example, based on
Allport's (1954) social contact hypothesis for reducing prejudice
and discrimination between racial groups, we have theorized that, under
certain conditions, positive interactions between individuals from
different religious groups might help improve a person's span,
which we define as one's capacity and motivation to explore and
understand diverse perspectives in order to broaden one's ability
to form strong social bonds with people who hold very different
convictions (Woodruff, Van Tongeren, McElroy, Davis, & Hook, 2013).
Ultimately, this exploration could help a person develop humility and
religious tolerance toward religiously different individuals.
Conclusion
These are exciting times for humility researchers. Many areas of
research in positive psychology have been saturated with empirical work
and may be starting to plateau. In contrast, the study of humility is
rapidly accelerating and is opportunities abound for researchers
interested in humility. Furthermore, the intersection of humility with
religion and spirituality is complex, with several exciting research
questions that address problems germane to our communities, churches,
nation, and world. The research programs that were sampled in this
special issue will require decades of work to fully explore the
questions presented. The field of humility truly represents the quote of
the late John Templeton: "How little we know, how much to learn.
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2008). The HEX'ACO model of
personality structure and the importance of the H factor. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1952-1962.
Baehr, J. (2011). The inquiring mind: On intellectual virtues and
virtue epistemology. Oxford University Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Bauer, I. M., & Lloyd, S. A. (2010). Choice,
free will, and religion. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 2,
67-82.
Bollinger, R. A., Kopp, K. J., Hill, P. C., & Williams, J.
(2006, August). Validation of a measurement of dispositional humility.
Poster session presented at the American Psychological
Association's Annual Convention, New Orleans, LA.
Bloom, P. (2012). Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of
Psychology, 63, 179-199.
Brown, S. L., Chopra, P. K., & Schiraldi, G. R. (2013).
Validation of the Humility Inventory (HI), a five-factor, self-report
measure of humility. The International Journal of Educational and
Psychological Assessment, 12, 57-77.
Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N.. Worthington, E. L., Jr., Van Tongeren,
D. R., Gartner, A. L., Jennings, D. J., & Emmons, R. A. (2011).
Relational humility: Conceptualizing and measuring humility as a
personality judgment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 225-234.
Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Van Tongeren,
D. R., Gartner, A. L., & Jennings, D. J., II. (2010). Relational
spirituality and forgiveness: Development of the Spiritual Humility
Scale (SHS). Journal of Psychology and Theology, 38, 91-100.
Davis, D. E., Worthington Jr, E. L., & Hook, J.N. (2010).
Humility: Review of measurement strategies and conceptualization as
personality judgment. The Journal cgositive Psychology, 5, 243-252.
Dwiwardani, C., Hill, P. C., Bollinger, R. A., Marks, L. E.,
Steele, J. R., Doolin, H. N., & Wood, S. L. (2014). Virtues develop
from a secure base: Attachment and resilience as predictors of humility,
gratitude, and forgiveness. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
42,83-90.
Galen, L. W. (2012a). The complex and elusive nature of religious
prosociality: Reply to Myers (2012) and Saroglou (2012). Psychological
Bulletin, 138, 918-923.
Galen, L. W. (2012b). Does religious belief promote prosocialiry? A
critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 876-906.
Grey:, A. P., & Mahadevan, N. (2014). Intellectual arrogance
and intellectual humility: An evolutionary-epistemological account.
Journal ofPsychology and Theology, 42,7-18.
Gruggs, J. B., & Exline, J.J. (2014). Humbling yourself before
God: Humility as a reliable predictor of lower divine stru:ne. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 42,41-49.
Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science,
316,998-1002.
Hall, D. L., Matz, D. C., & Wood, W. (2010). Why don't we
practice what we preach? A meta-analytic review of religious racism.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 126-139.
Hill, P. C., & Pargament, K. I. (2008). Advances in the
conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality:
Implications for physical and mental health research.
Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Ovven, J., Worthington, E. L., Jr.,
& Utsey, S. 0. (2013). Cultural humility: Measuring openness to
culturally diverse clients. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60,
353-366.
Hopkin, C. R., Hoyle, R. H., & Toner, K. (2014). Intellectual
humility and reactions to opinions about religious beliefs. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 42, 50-51.
Hopkins, N. (2011). Religion and social capital: Identity matters.
Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 21, 528-540.
Jankowski, P. J., & Sandage, S. J. (2014). Attachment to God
and disposirional humility: Indirect and conditional effects models,
Journal of Psychology and Theology, 42,70-82.
Landrum, R. E. (2011). Measuring dispositional humility: A first
approximation. Psychological Reports, 108, 217-228.
Lavclock, C. R., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Davis, D. E., Griffin, B.
J., Reid, C. A., Hook, J. N., & Van Tongeren, D. R. (2014). The
quiet virtue speaks: An intervention to promote humility. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 42,99-110.
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the
HEXACO Personality Inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39,
329-358.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, Jr, P. T., & Martin, T. A. (2005). The
NEO-PI-3: A more readable revised NEO personality inventory. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 84, 261-270.
McElroy, S., Rice, K., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., Hill, P. C.,
Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Van Tongeren, D. R. (2014). Intellectual
Humility: Scale Development and Theoretical Elaborations in the Context
of Religious Leadership. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 42, 19-30.
McNulty, J. K., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Beyond positive
psychology? Toward a contextual view of psychological processes and
wellbeing. American Psychologist, 67(2),101.
Myers, D. G. (2012). Reflections on religious belief and
prosocialiry: Comment on Galen (2012). Psychological Bulletin,
138,913-917.
Owen, J., Jordan, T. A., Turner, D., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N.,
& Leach, M. (2014). Therapists' multicultural orientation:
Client perceptions of cultural humility, spiritual/religious commitment,
and therapy outcomes. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 42,91-98.
Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: an
integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to
stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 257.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths
and virtues: A handbook and classification. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association; New York: Oxford University Press.
Roberts, R. C., & Wood, W. J. (2003). Humility and epistemic
goods. In M. DePaul and L. Zagzebski (Eds.), Intellectual virtue:
Perspectives from ethics and epistemology (pp. 203-226).0xford:
Clarendon Press.
Rowan, W. C., Kang, L. L., Haggard, M. C., & LaBouff, J. P.
(2014). A social-personality perspective on humility, religiousness, and
spirituality. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 42, 31-40.
Rowart, W. C., Powers, C., Targhetta, V., Comer, J., Kennedy, S.,
& Labouff, J. (2006). Development and initial validation of an
implicit measure of humility relative to arrogance. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 1,198-211.
Owens, B. P., Johnson, M. D., & Mitchell, T. R. (2013).
Expressed humility in organizations: Implications for performance,
teams, and leadership. Organization Science, 24, 1517-1538.
Sandage, S. J., Hill, P. C., & Vaubel, D. C. (2011).
Gencrativity, relational spirituality, gratitude, and mental health:
Relationships and pathways. International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion, 21, 1-16.
Saroglou, V. (2012). Is religion not prosocial at all? Comment on
Galen (2012). Psychological Bulletin, 138, 907-912.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of
values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1-65.
van Dierendonck, D., & Nuijten, I. (2011). The servant
leadership survey: Development and validation of a multidimensional
measure. Journal of Business and Psychology, 26, 249-267.
Van Tongeren, D. R., Green, J. D., Hulsey, T. L., Legare, C. H.,
Bromley, D. G., & Houtman, A. M. (2014). A meaning-based approach to
humility: Meaning affirmation reduces worldview defense. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 42,62-69.
Woodruff; E., Van Tongeren, D. R., McElroy, S., Davis, D. E., &
Hook, J. N. (2013). Humility and religion: Benefits, difficulties, and a
model of religious tolerance. In C. Kim-Prieto (Ed.). Positive
Psychology of Religion and Spirituality Across Cultures. New York:
Springer Publishing Co.
Don E. Davis
Georgia State University
Joshua N. Hook
University of North Texas
Author Note: We would like to acknowledge the generous financial
support of a grant from the Fuller Theological Seminary / Thrive Center
in concert with the John Templeton Foundation (Grant No. 108.
Intellectual Humility in Religious Leaders), as well as the John
Templeton Foundation (Grant No. 29630, The Development, Validation, and
Dissemination of Measures of Intellectual Humility and Humility; Grant
No. 14979, Relational Humility: An Interdisciplinary Approach to the
Study of Humility). The opinions expressed in this publication are those
of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Fuller
Thrive Center or the John Templeton Foundation.
Author Information
DAVIS, DON E. PhD. Address: Department of Counseling and
Psychological Services, Georgia State University, P.O. Box 3980,
Atlanta, GA 30302-3980. Title: Assistant Professor of Counseling and
Psychological Services Georgia State University. Degrees: PhD
(Counseling Psychology) Virginia Commonwealth University; BA
(Psychology) Yale University. Specializations: humility, forgiveness,
positive psychology, religion/spirituality.
HOOK, JOSHUA N. Address: University of North Texas, 1155 Union
Circle #311280, Denton, TX 76203. Email: joshua.hook@untedu. Title:
Assistant Professor of Psychology. Degrees: BS (Psychology) University
of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; MS (Counseling Psychology) Virginia
Commonwealth University; PhD (Counseling Psychology) Virginia
Commonwealth University. Specializations: humility, forgiveness,
religion/spirituality.