Attitudes toward and stereotypes of persons with body art: implications for marketing management.
Totten, Jeff W. ; Lipscomb, Thomas J. ; Jones, Michael A. 等
INTRODUCTION
A critical concern for many organizations is the image projected by
their personnel to customers or other interest groups. Many
organizations have dress codes designed to project a specific image
(Wich, 2007). An issue which appears to be of increasing concern within
the context of dress codes in the U.S.A. is the apparent increase in the
prevalence of tattoos and body piercing among employees, adornments
commonly referred to as body art. The concern results from the fact that
body art has historically been associated with negative behavior and
connotations. Though historically considered to be somewhat deviant
behavior in Western society, social scientists argued that the use of
body art is becoming increasingly diffused and embraced by the middle
class (Carroll, Riffenburgh, Roberts & Myhre, 2002; DeMello, 2000;
Rock, 2008).
Although the display of body art by employees is may be of
particular concern to retailers in the U.S.A., the issue is actually
more widespread: law firms, hospitals, non-profit organizations, and
even government agencies such as state parks "wrestle" with
the issue of how employees should dress (e.g., Body Art and Tattoos,
2006; Business Legal Reports, 2006; Dale, Bevill, Roach, Glasgow &
Bracy, 2008; Felton-O'Brien, 2007; Mlodzik, 2007). The issue for
managers is that both tattoos and body piercings with adornment have in
the past often been associated with risky and deviant behavior in our
culture. Uncertainty abounds from a management perspective as to how
accepting of body art customers may be, and what stereotypes might come
into play. What size tattoo is acceptable? How many are acceptable and
on what parts of one's body? Are they more acceptable on women, or
by women? The concern is not only about the existence, number, size and
location of the tattoos, but what the tattoo might express (i.e.
symbolism).
The issue is complicated by the fact that dress codes can quickly
become legal "minefields" (Barron, 2007). Legal ramifications associated with the restriction of body art can include issues of sex
discrimination, freedom of religion and freedom of speech. In fact,
numerous lawsuits have resulted from the establishment mandatory dress
codes for employees that included proscriptions against body art. One
case in 2006 involved Red Robin Gourmet Burgers which was sued in a
Washington state federal court as a result of terminating a member of
the wait staff who had refused to cover tattoos on his wrists. The
server who brought suit claimed in court that the tattoos were of
religious significance and symbolized his devotion to Ra, the Egyptian
sun god. The company countered that forbidding visible tattoos was
essential to maintaining its "family-friendly" image (Barron,
2007).
LITERATURE REVIEW
Although there is a growing body of literature concerned with body
art from the perspectives of the social sciences and the medical field,
relatively little attention has been devoted to the topic from the
marketing perspective. Much of the medical literature on tattooing and
body piercing has focused on the risks and complications associated with
of the procedures themselves (Armstrong, Koch, Saunders, Roberts &
Owen, 2007; Carroll et al., 2002; Food and Drug Administration, 2007).
Studies published in the sociology literature have primarily focused on
the display of body art as a form of "deviant" behavior, as
well as descriptions of marginal and sub-cultural groups associated with
tattooing and body art (Forbes, 2001) while studies published in the
psychology literature have tended to focus on psychopathology and
intrapersonal motivational factors in the display of body art (Vail,
1999). A group of academicians from several disciplines at Texas Tech
University has been conducting body art research since the 1990s.
Recent surveys of adults have found the existence of body art to
range from 4% to 24% in the general population (Gardyn & Whelan,
2001; Laumann & Derick, 2006; "Tattooed Emotions," 2004).
Evidence suggests that the prevalence of body art has grown most quickly
among young adults. Even mothers are taking daughters to get body art;
apparently the greatest numbers of people getting tattoos are girls
between the ages of 17 and 25 (Hein, 2006). The use of body art may be
even higher among college students, with studies reporting rates of 20%
to 60% (Anderson, 2003; Dale, Bevill, Roach & Glasgow, 2007; Forbes,
2001; Mayers, Judelson, Moriarty & Rundell, 2002; "University
Survey," 2003). A 2006 study published in the Journal of the
American Academy of Dermatology found that 48% of "20-somethings
and 30 percent of all adults have a tattoo or body piercing" and
"23 percent of those with body art hold at least a bachelor's degree" (Fiorentino, 2006, p. D1). Another recent study reported
that 40.5% of 496 college business students reported having some form of
body art, with 26.3% of male students and 56.8% of female students
reporting either tattoos or body piercings (Lipscomb, Jones &
Totten, 2008).
There is an emerging literature from the consumer behavior
perspective, in which researchers have begun to explore the
"consumption" of body art and the decision making process that
underlies it (e.g., Armstrong, Owen, Roberts & Koch, 2002; Hawkins,
Mothersbaugh & Best, 2007; Hoerr, Concklin, Marusich & Griffin,
1994; Lipscomb, et al., 2008; Solomon, 2004; Velliquette, Murray &
Creyer, 1998).
As is apparent from the preceding literature review, little
attention has been paid to the increasing popularity of body art from
the marketing perspective. Since individuals engaged in a variety of
marketing-related activities in diverse industries (retail, legal,
medical, etc.) must interact with consumers on a face-to-face basis, it
is important to understand the manner in which individuals with body art
are perceived by consumers and the general public. The purpose of this
study was to investigate attitudes toward persons with body art. The
sample, business majors at several universities, was selected in part
based on convenience and in part based on the fact that they may be
representative of future managers and leaders in business communities.
METHODOLOGY
The instrument used in this study was developed by the authors on
the basis of three focus groups in which college student participants
were asked to discuss their attitudes toward body art in the form of
tattoos and body piercing and toward persons possessing body art. These
qualitative data were content analyzed by means of the Constant
Comparative Method (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). From the unique
dimensions identified, items for the quantitative investigation of
attitudes toward person with body art were constructed. All items
utilized a five-point Likert-type scale that had both verbal and
numerical descriptors for each point on the scale (Strongly Disagree,
Disagree, Neither, Agree, Strongly Agree). The resulting instrument was
designed to provide data concerning various aspects of attitudes toward
persons with body art as well as to gather basic demographic information
concerning the respondent. Of interest here were the 37 items assessing
stereotypical attitudes toward persons with body art.
Students enrolled in principles of marketing classes at14 colleges
and universities geographically dispersed throughout the U.S. were
administered questionnaires in their classrooms. The institutions
participating in the study were selected by means of purposive sampling
for geographic representation in the U.S. A faculty member who taught
principles of marketing at each institution was recruited by phone or
email to participate. Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was
obtained prior to data collection from the authors' institutions
well as sister boards at the other participating universities as
required. Copies of the questionnaire were mailed to participating
faculty for distribution. Completed questionnaires were returned to the
authors by U.S. mail. Questionnaires were also distributed by the
authors, themselves, at their own university. The response rate was
65.7%, based on an approximate total of 755 surveys distributed,
including the ones handed out by the authors in their classes.
RESPONDENT PROFILE
Completed questionnaires were received from a total of 496
respondents. The respondents ranged in age from 18 to 62 years. The
average age of the respondents was 22.33 years, with a median age of 21
years. Among this sample, 47.2% were female while 52.8% were male. Four
regions of the U.S. were represented in the study with 33.5% of the
sample living in the Pacific region, 31.9% in the Midwest region, 21.8%
in the Southern region, and 12.7 % in the East Coast region. The
demographic variables used in the analyses reported below included: age,
gender, region of residence, and whether or not the respondent reported
having body art.
Survey instructions asked that respondents exclude pierced earlobes
on women from consideration in their responses. The data revealed that,
40.5% of those responding indicated that they themselves had some form
of body art. Of these individuals reporting some form of body art, 48.2
% reported having tattoos and 79.4% indicated having piercings. Of those
having tattoos, the majority reported having one (64.1%), 19.8% reported
having two. Three individuals (3.3%) reported having six or more
tattoos. The overall pattern was similar for piercings with the largest
percentage (41.6%) reporting having one piercing and 26.0% reporting
having two. Three individuals (1.8%) reported having 10 or more
piercings. These results are presented in Table 1.
DATA REDUCTION
Exploratory Factor Analysis (Principal Component Analysis
extraction with varimax rotation using Kaiser normalization) was
performed in order to discover inherent factors in the data. Inspection
of the Scree plot that resulted from the initial analysis revealed
evidence for three discrete factors. Additional factors appearing in the
initial analysis were composed of items that either loaded below .40 or
had a single item loading, typically one that loaded negatively with an
item that also loaded positively on one of the three primary factors.
Accordingly, a three factor solution was applied. The rotation converged
in three iterations and accounted for 44.6 % of the variance. Inspection
of the factor loadings of the 37 items analyzed revealed that the first
two factors subsumed positive and negative characteristics associated
with persons with body art respectively. Eighteen items loaded at .40 or
greater on Factor 1 which was labeled, "Positive Characteristics
Associated with Persons having Body Art". These 18 items measure
the perceived attractiveness of persons with body art. Fifteen items
loaded at .40 or greater on Factor 2, labeled as "Negative
Characteristics Associated with Persons having Body Art". This
factor is composed of items assessing attributions such as sexual
promiscuity, aggressiveness, and other dimensions of negative social
connotation. The third factor, "Number and Stereotyping," was
composed of only two items both of which pertained to social views of
persons with body art and were specifically concerned with the manner in
which the respondent understood society's views of a person as
being affected by the number of tattoos and/or piercings he/she has.
Only two of the 37 items did not load above .40 on one or the other of
these factors. Items along with their respective factor loadings are
presented in Table 2. Response percentages for each of the items are
provided in Table 3.
Further analyses revealed many significant differences by gender
(Table 4), region of the country (Table 5), presence of body art (Table
6) and age (Table 7). These differences are discussed on a
factor-by-factor basis, looking at tattoo-based items then
piercing-based items within each factor.
FACTOR 1: POSITIVE CHARACTERISTICS
The majority of respondents (65%) indicated that tattoos can be
attractive (combined percentages for responses of 4, Agree, and 5,
Strongly Agree; see Table 3). This was a fairly robust finding and did
not vary significantly as a function of gender of the respondent.
Analysis using Pearson Chi Square for Independence indicated, however,
that there was a relationship between this item and the area of the
country in which the respondent resided. A higher percentage of the
respondents residing in the East Coast region of the U.S. agreed
(combined percentages for responses of 4, Agree and 5, Strongly Agree;
see Table 5) that tattoos are attractive (87.3 %) as compared to those
in the Pacific, Midwestern, and Southern regions (m = 61.4 %), [chi
square] (12) = 28.84, p = .004. There was also a relatively small but
statistically significant negative correlation of this item with age of
the respondent, r = -.116, p = .011 (see Table 7). An interesting
pattern emerged in response to items concerning the attractiveness of
tattoos for persons of each sex (see Table 4). Further, analysis using
Pearson Chi Square for Independence indicated that these attitudes were
tempered by the gender of the respondent to a statistically significant
extent. Considering the attractiveness of tattoos on men, a total of
51.3 % of the female respondents indicated that they agreed that tattoos
on men are attractive. On the other hand, a total of 24.9 % of the men
indicated agreement, [chi square] (4) = 44.397, p = .001. Concerning the
attractiveness of tattoos on women, the pattern was essentially the
converse. Whereas 49.4% of the men agreed that tattoos on women are
attractive, only 32.4 % of women agreed. Again, this relationship was
found to be statistically significant, [chi square] (4) = 15.23, p =
.004 (see Table 4). There was also a significant regional variation for
the attitude that tattoos on women are attractive, [chi square] (12) =
37.25, p = .001 (see Table 5). A smaller percentage of respondents
residing in the Southern region ascribed to this view (29.9 %) as
compared to those in the Pacific, Mid Western, and East Coast regions (m
= 61.86 %). In addition, there was also a relatively small but
statistically significant negative correlation of this item with age of
the respondent, r = -.093, p = .041 (see Table 7).
The results also indicated that the extent of the presence of
tattoos on both men and women is related to attributions of
attractiveness by the respondents. Specifically, 78.1 % and 87.5 % of
respondents disagreed that extensive tattoos are attractive on men and
women respectively (see Table 3). In the case of tattoos on men,
however, there was a significant relationship with gender, [chi square]
(4) = 18.59, p = .001 (see Table 4). More women disagreed that extensive
tattoos on men are attractive (85.6 %) as compared to male respondents
(71.6 %). There was no significant relationship of gender to the item
concerning the attractiveness of extensive tattoos on women.
A specific sub-dimension of attractiveness that was examined is
that of sexual attraction. This dimension was referred to in various
items by the adjective, "sexy". Interestingly, whereas the
great majority of respondents disagreed that extensive tattoos on both
men and women are attractive, the majority, 62.1 %, agreed that a small
discrete tattoo on a woman is "sexy" while only 22.4 % agreed
that this was so for a man (see Table 3). Consistent with the
cross-gender pattern discussed above, there was a statistically
significant relationship between the gender of the respondent and
his/her attributions of sexual attractiveness for a small discrete
tattoo on women, [chi square] (4) = 33.81, p = .001 (Table 4). In this
comparison, 71.6 % of men agreed that a small discrete tattoo on women
is "sexy" as opposed to 51.4 % of female respondents. A
similar cross-gender effect was in evidence for the companion item
concerning the "sexiness" of a small discrete tattoo on a man,
p2 (4) = 55.65, p = .001 (Table 4). Significantly more women (35.2 %)
agreed that a small discrete tattoo on a man is "sexy" as
compared to men (11.5%), [chi square] (4) = 55.62, p= .001 (Table 4).
In response to the item, "Tattoos are appropriate for a person
of any age", 24.2 % of the sample expressed agreement while 60.9%
expressed disagreement (see Table 3). For the item, "Tattoos are
appropriate for a parent," 30 % agreed, 41.3% were neutral and
27.8% disagreed (see Table 3). For the sample as a whole, 38.5 % agreed
that tattoos are indicative of "free spiritedness" while 23.7
% disagreed and 36.9 % were neutral (see Table 3). There was fairly
uniform agreement for these three items as there were no significant
relationships to the demographic variables considered with the exception
of whether or not the respondent him/herself reported having body art
(discussed below).
As was the case with tattoos, the majority of respondents (71 %)
indicated that piercings with body jewelry can be attractive (combined
percentages for responses of 4, Agree and 5, Strongly Agree; see Table
3). With the exception of whether or not the respondent him/herself
reported having body art (see Table 6), there was fairly uniform
agreement as there were no significant relationships of this item to
gender or region of residence. There was, however, a statistically
significant negative correlation with age of the respondent, r = -.225,
p = .001 (Table 7). Also as was the case for tattoos, a considerably
higher percentage of respondents viewed piercings with body jewelry on
women to be attractive (67.5 %) as compared to men (12.1 %) (see Table
3). Again, statistically significant patterns were evident as functions
of the gender of the respondents. Thus, a statistically significantly
greater percentage of men reported piercings with body jewelry on women
to be attractive (72.7 %) as compared to women respondents (61.3 %),
[chi square] (4) = 17.03, p = .002 (Table 4). This item was
significantly negatively correlated with the age of the respondent, r =
-.231, p = .001 (Table 7). Likewise, a statistically significantly
greater percentage of women reported piercings with body jewelry on men
to be attractive (19.3 %) as compared to men respondents (6.0 %), [chi
square] (4) = 41.59, p = .0001 (Table 4).
Also as was the case for tattoos, the extent of the presence of
piercings with body jewelry on both men and women was found to be
related to attributions of attractiveness. Specifically, the majority of
respondents disagreed that extensive piercings are attractive on men
(80.3 %) and women respectively (79.8 %) (see Table 3). There was a
significant relationship between gender and agreement that extensive
piercings on men are attractive, [chi square] (4) = 27.16, p = .001
(Table 4). A greater percentage of women (88.5 %) disagreed that
extensive piercings on men are attractive as compared to men (75.3 %).
There was no such relationship between gender and the attribution that
extensive piercings on women are attractive. There was, however, a
significant regional variation for this item, [chi square] (12) = 26.09,
p = .010 (Table 5), wherein a smaller percentage of those in the Eastern
region disagreed that extensive piercings on women are attractive (60.3
%) than was the case for any of the other three regions of the U.S. (m =
83.06 %). Likewise, there was a significant relationship between region
and the attribution that extensive piercings on men are attractive, [chi
square] (12) = 23.21, p = .026 (Table 5), with a smaller percentage of
respondents in the eastern region disagreeing with the statement (66.1
%) than in the other three geographic regions (m = 84.26 %).
Concerning the specific sub-dimension of the sexual attractiveness
of persons with piercings and body jewelry, 15.3 % of the sample agreed
that a small discrete piercing on a man is "sexy" while 64.7 %
agreed that this was so for a woman, a similar pattern to that found
with tattoos (see Table 3). There was a small but significant negative
correlation for the item concerning the "sexiness" of a small,
discrete tattoo on a woman with age of the respondent, r = -.175, p =
.001 (Table 7). Also as was found with tattoos, there were significant
cross-gender relationships wherein a higher percentage of women (26.9 %)
as compared to men (5.5 %) agreed that a small discrete piercing with
body jewelry is "sexy" on men, [chi square] (4) = 58.91, p=
.001 (Table 4) while more men (70.2 %) as compared to women (58.8 %)
agreed that this was true for women, [chi square] (4) = 10.58, p = .032
(Table 4).
Considering the entire sample, the majority (64.3 %) disagreed that
piercings with body art are appropriate for persons of any age (see
Table 3). In response to the related item "Piercing(s) with body
jewelry is appropriate for a parent", 21.7% of the respondents
agreed while 41.3 % disagreed (see Table 3). There was a small but
significant negative correlation of this latter item with age of the
respondent, r = -.225, p = .000 (Table 7).
Although there are too many significant relationships to list, for
every variable having to do with the attractiveness or
"sexiness" of tattoos or piercings, chi square analysis
revealed a statistically significant relationship between these
attitudes and whether or not the respondent him/herself reported having
body art (see Table 6). Not surprisingly, those respondents who reported
having body art more frequently agreed that tattoos and piercings are
attractive and "sexy" than did those who did not report having
body art.
FACTOR 2: NEGATIVE CHARACTERISTICS
The majority of the respondents (58%) disagreed that a man with a
tattoo is sexually promiscuous (see Table 3). Significantly more women
(69.2 %) expressed disagreement as compared to men (49.2 %), [chi
square] (4) = 39.59, p = .001 (Table 4). Similarly, the majority of the
sample (56.6 %) disagreed that a woman with a tattoo is sexually
promiscuous (see Table 3). Again, significantly more women disagreed
(68.2 %) with this statement as compared to men (46.5 %), [chi square]
(4) = 28.45, p = .001 (Table 4). The majority of the respondents
disagreed that a person with a tattoo is aggressive (55.4 %) (see Table
3). This opinion was quite uniform across the sample as with the
exception of whether the respondent reported having body art (Table 6),
there were no statistically significant relationships between this item
and the other demographic variables considered. So too, the majority of
the sample disagreed that a person with a tattoo has a "bad
image" (64.1%). In the case of this item, however, there was a
significant relationship to gender with more women (73.7 %) disagreeing
with the statement than men (56.9 %), [chi square] (4) = 17.61, p = .001
(Table 4).
Roughly a fourth of the sample (21.6 %) agreed that tattoos are
indicative of a "partying lifestyle" while 45.8% disagreed
(see Table 3). This view was relatively uniform as there were no
significant relationships of this item to any of the demographic
variables considered. A relatively small percentage of respondents
ascribed to the view that a person with tattoos engages in substance
abuse (4.5 %) with the majority disagreeing with this assertion (see
Table 3). A smaller percentage of men (71.8%), however, disagreed as
compared to women (84.1%), [chi square] (4) = 12.12, p = .017 (Table 4).
There was a small but statistically significant positive correlation of
this item to the age of the respondent, r = .143, p = .002 (Table 7).
Likewise, the majority of respondents (61.3) disagreed that a person
with a tattoo is "tough." There were no statistically
significant relationships for this variable to any of the demographic
items considered.
In general, the results for piercing paralleled those for tattoos.
The results for the sample as a whole indicated that the majority of
respondents disagreed that a person with piercings with body art is
"tough" (75.2%) or has a "bad image" (69.4%) (Table
3). There was a significant positive correlation of this item with the
age of the respondent, r = .217, p = .000 (Table 7). Concerning the
notion that person with piercings are "tough" there was a
significant relationship of this item to gender, [chi square] (4) =
10.26, p = .036 (Table 4). A greater percentage of the female
respondents disagreed (80.7 %) as compared to men (72.1 %). For the item
concerning a person with piercings having a "bad image", there
was also a significant relationship to gender, [chi square] (4) = 17.66,
p = .001 (Table 4). Once again, more women disagreed (78.0 %) as
compared to men (62.4 %). There was a significant positive correlation
of this item and age of the respondent, r = .217, p = .001 (Table 7).
In a similar fashion the majority of respondents (69.9 %) disagreed
that a person with piercings with body jewelry is aggressive. So too,
the majority of respondents disagreed that a person with piercing with
body jewelry abuses alcohol or drugs (78.4%) There was, however, a
significant relationship to gender for this item, [chi square] (4) =
17.66, p = .001 (Table 4). More women disagreed with the assertion (86.0
%) as compared to men (73.0 %) (Table 3). There was a small but positive
correlation of this item with age of the respondent, r = .156, p = .001
(Table 7). Similarly, the majority of the sample disagreed that a person
with piercings with body jewelry leads a "partying lifestyle"
(63.0 %) (see Table 3). There was a small but positive correlation of
this item with age of the respondent, r = .101, p = .026 (Table 7).
As was the case for the items comprising Factor 1, for those items
comprising factor 2 there were statistically significant relationships
to whether the respondent reported having body art (see Table 6). A
significantly higher percentage of those respondents reporting having
body art disagreed with the negative stereotypes of persons with body
art.
FACTOR 3: NUMBER AND STEREOTYPING
As mentioned above, this factor was composed of only two items both
of which pertained to social views of persons with body art and were
specifically concerned with the manner in which the respondent
understood society's views of a person as being affected by the
number of tattoos and/or piercings he/she has. The majority of
respondents agreed that the number of tattoos (85.3%) and piercings
(80.8%) a person has makes a difference in how he/she is perceived by
others (see Table 3). The sample was relatively consistent in this
assessment as there were no statistically significant relationships of
either item to the demographic variables considered including whether
the respondent reported having body art.
There were two items that were not part of the three factors and,
indeed, appear to be conceptually unrelated to one another. The first of
these concerned the extent to which society stereotypes persons with
tattoos. The majority of respondents (86.9%) agreed that this is so (see
Table 3). The sample was relatively uniform in this belief as there was
no statistically significant relationship of this item to the
demographic variables considered. The second "non-factor" item
was concerned with extent to which piercings with body jewelry indicate
"free spiritedness." For this item, 29.2% of the sample agreed
while 34.5% were neutral and 35.7% disagreed (see Table 3). There was a
statistically significant relationship to region of residence, [chi
square] (12) = 31.51, p = .002 (Table 5). More respondents residing in
the East Coast Region (52.3 %) agreed with this statement than did those
in the three other regions (m = 37.33 %). It did not vary significantly
as a function of the other demographic items considered including
whether the respondent reported having body art.
DISCUSSION
In general, the present results indicate that the majority of the
university business majors comprising the sample not only do not harbor
negative attitudes or stereotypes toward persons with body art but, in
fact, express many positive attributions. The majority of those
surveyed, however, believed that society in general does, in fact,
stereotype persons with either type of body art. At the most global
level, the majority of the sample expressed the attitudes that both
tattoos and body piercings are attractive. This tended to be true for
both female and male respondents. There was, however, regional variation
in the case of tattoos with those respondents attending universities in
the East Coast region of the U.S. finding tattoos to be more attractive
than those in the other regions of the country. Another interesting and
not completely unexpected finding is that the younger persons in the
sample tend to find both piercings and tattoos on others to be more
attractive than do the older respondents.
Rather interestingly, the sex of the individual about whom an
attribution of attractiveness is made was found to make a difference.
Specifically, both piercings and tattoos are considered to be more
attractive on women than on men. In addition, cross-gender patterns were
evident for both forms of body art with men finding tattoos and
piercings on women to be more attractive than on men and women finding
them to be more attractive on men than women. It is recognized, of
course, that this latter finding may well be related to men in the
sample simply being more attracted to women in general and women being
more attracted to men. If this were true, this last finding would be
somewhat artifactual but still interesting and not without implication.
There was also a regional variation concerning the attractiveness of
tattoos on women with those respondents attending universities in the
southern region of the U.S. reporting tattoos on women to be less
attractive that those in the other regions of the country.
Another interesting finding had to do with the extensiveness of
body art. Apparently, the majority of respondents find body art of both
types to be attractive as long as these are not overdone. The majority
of the sample disagreed that extensive tattoos and extensive piercings
on persons of either sex are attractive. Along these same lines, the
majority of the sample reported that a small discrete tattoo or piercing
is "sexy" on a woman while a minority of the sample found this
to be so for men. In addition, for both tattoos and piercings there were
tendencies for younger persons to find small, discrete tattoos or
piercings "sexy" as opposed to older ones.
In general, the majority of the sample disagreed with various
negative stereotypes and attributions concerning persons with tattoos
and/or piercings. Thus, the majority of those surveyed disagreed that
persons with either tattoos or body piercings are necessarily
promiscuous, aggressive, engage in substance abuse, are
"tough" or have a "bad image". Nearly half disagreed
that persons with either type of body art have a "partying"
lifestyle. There was a tendency for women to disagree more than men and
for younger persons to disagree more than older ones. In this way, it
can be argued that both women and younger persons are less prone to
negative stereotyping of persons with body art than are men and older
persons and are, therefore, more accepting of body art on others. Never
the less, the majority of the sample disagreed that tattoos and body art
are appropriate for a person of any age and were somewhat ambivalent concerning the appropriateness of either tattoos or piercings for
parents.
In terms of the issues and questions introduced earlier then and to
the extent that the present results may be generalizable to college
students in the U.S. more broadly, it would appear that there is
relatively little reason for concern for those companies and products
that target college students that negative attitudes and stereotypes
associated with persons with body art will negatively impact marketing
efforts. On the contrary, it would appear that among this important
consumer group most find tattoos and body piercing to be attractive and
relatively few harbor negative stereotypes of persons with body art.
This was true for those individuals attending universities in all
regions of the country but particularly so for those in the East Coast
region. Also, younger persons in the present study found body art to be
more attractive than did their older counter parts. There was also
evidence that consumers represented in the present sample find body art
to be most attractive on women. These results are mitigated somewhat by
the finding that most respondents were apparently of the opinion that
body art can be overdone as the majority of the sample disagreed that
extensive tattoos and extensive piercings on persons of either sex are
attractive.
The present findings may have implication for marketing managers.
To the extent that recommendations are appropriate based on the present
results and procedures, one such general statement would be that there
need be little concern among managers that persons in their employ with
body art who are visible to the public will be viewed negatively by
customers or detract from potential sales. This would appear to be
particularly so if the representative in question is a woman. The
exception may be for company representatives who display extensive body
art. The present data suggest that these individuals may not be viewed
favorably by consumers. (It should be noted that those with body art
less frequently ascribed to this more general view; marketing activities
directed specifically toward these consumers is the exception.) In
addition, the make-up of the target market must be considered. As a
general rule, younger persons in the present sample and those residing
in the East Coast region of the country find body art to be the most
attractive. Marketing managers in the East Coast region and those
directing their strategic marketing efforts toward younger consumers in
particular need not be overly concerned that persons in their employ
display body art.
REFERENCES
Anderson, C. (2003, May 12). EMU study paints different picture
about tattoos and body piercing. The Northern Illinois University Daily
Star. Retrieved February 28, 2009, from
http://www.emich.edu/univcomm/releases/tattoos.html.
Armstrong, M. L., J.R. Koch, J.C. Saunders, A.E. Roberts & D.C.
Owen (2007). The hole picture: Risks, decision making, purpose,
regulations, and the future of body piercing. Clinics in Dermatology,
25, 398-406.
Armstrong, M. L., D.C. Owen, A.E. Roberts & J.R. Koch (2002).
College tattoos: More than skin deep. Dermatology Nursing, (45)5,
317-323.
Barron, D. L. (2007, April 15). Fashion victims, Progressive
Grocer, 58.
Body Art and Tattoos in the Workplace (2006, October 21). Retrieved
July 23, 2008, from http://www.foxnews.com/story/0,2933,223178,00.html.
Business Legal Reports, Inc. (2008, June 27). Drawing the line on
tattoos in the workplace. Retrieved July 23, 2008, from
http://hr.blr.com/news.aspx?id=18637.
Carroll, S. T., Riffenburgh, R. H., Roberts, T. A., & Myhre, E.
B. (2002). Tattoos and body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk-taking behaviors." Pediatrics, 109, 1021-1027.
Dale, L.R., S. Bevill, T. Roach & S. Glasgow (2007). To Tattoo
or Not Tattoo Is The Question. Proceedings of the Academy for Economics
and Economic Education, 10(2), 11-18. Retrieved February 28, 2009, from
http://www.alliedacademies.org/Public/Proceedings/ Proceedings21/
AEEE%20Proceedings.pdf.
Dale, L.R., S. Bevill, T. Roach, S. Glasgow & C. Bracy (2008).
Body Adornment: A Comparison of the Attitudes of Businesspeople and
Students in Three States. Proceedings of the Academy of Educational
Leadership, 11(1), 17. Retrieved February 28, 2009, from
http://www.alliedacademies.org/Public/Proceedings/Proceedings22/
AEL%20Proceedings.pdf.
DeMello, M. (2000). Bodies of inscription: A cultural history of
the modern tattoo community. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Felton-O'Brien, M. (2007, July 16). Inked at work. Retrieved
July 23, 2008, from http://www.hrexecutive.com/HRE/story.jsp?storyId=20189555.
Fiorentino, A. (2006, November 6). Tattoos in the workplace,
Portland (ME) Press Herald, D1.
Food and Drug Administration (2007, December 6). Think before you
ink: Are tattoos safe? Retrieved February 28, 2009, from
http://www.fda.gov/consumer/features/tattoos120607.html.
Forbes, G. B. (2001). College students with tattoos and piercings:
Motives, family experiences, personality factors, and perceptions by
others. Psychological Reports, 89(3), 774-786.
Gardyn, R., & D. Whelan (2001). Ink me stud. American
Demographics, 23(12), 9-11.
Hawkins, D. I., D.L. Mothersbaugh & R.J. Best (2007). Consumer
behavior: Building marketing strategy (Tenth Edition). New York:
McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
Hein, K. (2006, November 27). Motley Criie front man fronts brand
portfolio, Brandweek, 9.
Hoerr, L., K. Concklin, B. Marusich & M. Griffin (1994). An
exploratory investigation into the acquisition and consumption of
tattoos. Marketing: Advances in Theory and Thought, 85-90.
Laumann, A. E. & A.J. Derick, A. J. (2006). Tattoos and body
piercings in the United States: A national data set. Journal of the
American Academy of Dermatology. Retrieved February 28, 2009, from
http://academic.csuohio.edu/holcombj/TATTOOS.pdf.
Lipscomb, T. J., M.I. Jones & J.W. Totten (2008). Body art:
Prevalence, search and evaluation among university business students.
Services Marketing Quarterly, 28(4), 42-65.
Mayers, L., D. Judelson, B. Moriarty & K. Rundell (2002).
Prevalence of body art in university undergraduates and incidents of
medical complications. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 77, 29-34.
Mlodzik, C. (2007, January 27). Tattoos in Workplace: Your Right to
Bear Arms. Retrieved July 23, 2008, from
http://www.wdsu.com/smallbusiness/14816221/detail.html.
Rock, S. (2008, January 27). Scary guy forces people to face fears,
The (Baton Rouge, LA) Advocate, 5A.
Solomon, M. R. (2004). Consumer behavior: Buying, having, and being
(Sixth Edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Strauss, A. & J. Corbin (1990). Basics of qualitative research:
Grounded theory procedure and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Tattooed Emotions (2004, February 1). Marketing News, 3.
University survey tracks body art among students (2003, January
18). The Chronicle of Higher Education, 48(19), A37.
Vail, D. A. (1999). Tattoos are like potato chips ... you
can't have just one: The process of becoming and being a collector.
Deviant Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 253-273.
Velliquette, A. M., J.B. Murray & E.H. Creyer (1998). The
tattoo renaissance: An ethnographic account of symbolic consumer
behavior. Advances in Consumer Research, 461-467.
Wich, S. M. (2007, March). Kentucky State Parks' Dress Code
Enforced. HR Magazine, 117.
Jeff W. Totten, McNeese State University
Thomas J. Lipscomb, The University of Southern Mississippi
Michael A. Jones, Southeastern Louisiana University
Table 1: Prevalence of Tattoos and Piercings Among
Those With Body Art
Type/Number n %
Tattoos 92 48.2
One Tattoo 59 64.1
Two Tattoos 18 19.6
Three Tattoos 8 8.7
Four Tattoos 4 4.3
Type/Number n %
Five Tattoos 0 0
Six Tattoos 1 1.1
Seven Tattoos 1 1.1
More than Seven 1 1.1
Piercings 154 79.4
One Piercing 64 41.6
Two Piercings 40 26
Three Piercings 27 17.5
Four Piercings 7 4.5
Five Piercings 1 0.6
Six Piercings 2 1.3
Seven Piercings 4 2.6
Eight Piercings 6 3.9
More than Eight 3 1.8
Table 2: Factor Loadings
Factor
Item 1 2 3
Tattoos can be attractive 0.755 0.109 0.203
Tattoos on men are attractive 0.701 0.168 0.092
Tattoos on women are attractive 0.712 0.118 0.135
A small, discrete tattoo is "sexy" on a 0.651 0.127 0.332
woman
Extensive tattoos are attractive on a man 0.491 0.288 -0.366
A man with a tattoo(s) is sexually 0.069 0.584 -0.059
promiscuous
Extensive tattoos are attractive on a woman 0.432 0.236 -0.33
A small, discrete tattoo is "sexy" on a man 0.537 0.113 0.043
A woman with a tattoo(s) is sexually 0.069 0.642 0.049
promiscuous
Piercings with body jewelry can be 0.729 -0.007 0.256
attractive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on women is 0.679 0.023 0.265
attractive
A small, discrete piercing with body 0.588 0.174 -0.193
jewelry is "sexy" on a man
Extensive piercings with body jewelry are 0.466 0.348 -0.308
attractive on a woman
A man with piercing(s) and body jewelry is 0.089 0.672 -0.008
sexually promiscuous
A woman with piercing(s) and body jewelry 0.066 0.676 -0.008
is sexually promiscuous
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on men is 0.618 0.204 -0.188
attractive
A small, discrete piercing with body 0.653 0.06 0.289
jewelry is "sexy" on a woman
Extensive piercings with body jewelry are 0.445 0.371 -0.467
attractive on a man
A person with a tattoo(s) is aggressive -0.309 0.59 0.082
I consider a person with a tattoo(s) to -0.548 0.434 0.037
have a "bad" image
Tattoos are appropriate for a person of 0.518 0.123 -0.198
any age
People tend to stereotype persons with a -0.091 0.082 0.309
tattoo(s) *
The number of tattoos that a person has -0.051 0.078 0.526
makes a difference in how he/is
perceived by others
Tattoos are appropriate for a parent 0.708 0.012 -0.028
Tattoos indicate "free spiritedness"* 0.348 0.37 0.354
Tattoos indicate a "partying lifestyle" -0.258 0.578 0.165
Tattoos indicate that the person abuses -0.455 0.559 -0.097
alcohol or drugs
A person with a tattoo(s) is "tough". -0.086 0.614 0.082
A person with piercing(s) with body -0.008 0.598 -0.131
jewelry is "tough"
I consider a person with a piercing(s) with -0.536 0.495 0.022
body jewelry to have a "bad" image
Piercing(s) with body jewelry is 0.494 0.096 -0.281
appropriate for persons of any age
The number of piercings with body jewelry 0.028 0.092 0.584
that a person has makes a
difference in how he/is perceived by others
A person with a piercing(s) with body -0.215 0.703 -0.039
jewelry is aggressive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry indicate 0.213 0.436 0.397
free spiritedness
Piercing(s) with body jewelry indicate that -0.446 0.6 -0.053
the person abuses alcohol or drugs
Piercing(s) with body jewelry is 0.659 -0.031 -0.106
appropriate for a parent
Piercing(s) with body jewelry indicate a -0.324 0.657 0.034
partying lifestyle
* Did not load on any factor.
Table 3: Item Results in Percentages *
Strongly Disagree Neither
Item Disagree
Tattoos can be attractive 7.3 (36) 13.7 (68) 13.9 (69)
Tattoos on men are attractive 15.2 (75) 16.1 (79) 31.3 (154)
Tattoos on women are attractive 13.6 (67) 19.4 (96) 25.1 (124)
A small, discrete tattoo is 7.1 (35) 12.6 (62) 18.0 (89)
"sexy" on a woman
Extensive tattoos are 44.9 33.4 (165) 16.0 (79)
attractive on a man
-222
A man with a tattoo(s) is 27.4 31.1 (153) 33.9 (167)
sexually promiscuous
-135
Extensive tattoos are 62.5 (308) 25.6 (126) 7.3 (36)
attractive on a woman
A small, discrete tattoo is 23.8 (117) 22.4 (110) 31.3 (154)
"sexy" on a man
A woman with a tattoo(s) is 27.5 (136) 29.4 (145) 30.6 (151)
sexually promiscuous
Piercings with body jewelry can 6.9 (34) 10.1 (50) 12.1 (60)
be attractive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 6.9 (34) 10.3 (51) 15.3 (76)
on women is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 29.1 (143) 29.9 (147) 25.6 (126)
body jewelry is "sexy" on
a man
Extensive piercings with body 41 39.0 (193) 13.7 (68)
jewelry are attractive on a
woman
-203
A man with piercing(s) and body 31.0 (152) 28.5 (140) 32.0 (157)
jewelry is sexually
promiscuous
A woman with piercing(s) and 26.3 (130) 30.1 (149) 28.7 (142)
body jewelry is sexually
promiscuous
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 28.9 (141) 33.0 (161) 25.8 (126)
on men is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 8.3 (41) 11.2 (55) 15.2 (75)
body jewelry is "sexy" on a
woman
Extensive piercings with body 49.5 32.2 (157) 16.2 (79)
jewelry are attractive
on a man
-241
A person with a tattoo(s) is 15.2 (75) 40.7 (200) 32.1 (158)
aggressive
I consider a person with a 21.6 (106) 43.3 (212) 20.4 (100)
tattoo(s) to have a "bad"
image
Tattoos are appropriate for a 23.4 (115) 38.0 (187) 14.2 (70)
person of any age
People tend to stereotype .6 (3) 4.3 (21) 6.6 (32)
persons with a tattoo(s)
The number of tattoos that a 1.2 (6) 3.5 (17) 9.2 (45)
person has makes a difference
in how he/is perceived by
others
Tattoos are appropriate for a 10.6 (52) 17.5 (86) 41.7 (205)
parent
Tattoos indicate "free 5.5 (27) 18.5 (91) 37.2 (183)
spiritedness"
Tattoos indicate a "partying 9.4 (46) 37.0 (181) 31.7 (155)
lifestyle"
Tattoos indicate that the 37.1 (182) 40.5 (199) 17.9 (88)
person abuses alcohol or
drugs
A person with a tattoo(s) is 23.6 (116) 38.3 (188) 28.3 (139)
"tough".
A person with piercing(s) with 25.3 (124) 50.7 (249) 19.8 (97)
body jewelry is "tough"
I consider a person with a 20.1 (99) 49.8 (245) 18.3 (90)
piercing(s) with body jewelry
to have a "bad" image
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 21.0 (103) 44.0 (216) 15.9 (78)
is appropriate for persons of
any age
The number of piercings with 2.6 (13) 6.7 (33) 9.0 (44)
body jewelry that a person
has makes a difference in how
he/is perceived by others
A person with a piercing(s) 16.7 (82) 54.0 (265) 22.6 (111)
with body jewelry is
aggressive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 6.9(34) 29.0 (143) 34.7 (171)
indicate "free spiritedness"
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 29.3 (144) 49.8 (245) 17.1 (84)
indicate that the person
abuses alcohol or drugs
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 14.6 (72) 27.0 (133) 36.6 (180)
is appropriate for a parent
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 16.5 (81) 46.5 (229) 22.2 (109)
indicate a "partying
lifestyle"
Agree Strongly
Item Agree
Tattoos can be attractive 51.9 (257) 13.1 (65)
Tattoos on men are attractive 30.5 (150) 6.9 (34)
Tattoos on women are attractive 34.4 (170) 7.5 (37)
A small, discrete tattoo is 41.5 (205) 20.9 (103)
"sexy" on a woman
Extensive tattoos are 4.5 (22) 1.2 (6)
attractive on a man
A man with a tattoo(s) is 6.1 (30) 1.4 (7)
sexually promiscuous
Extensive tattoos are 3.4 (17) 1.2 (6)
attractive on a woman
A small, discrete tattoo is 20.5 (101) 2.0 (10)
"sexy" on a man
A woman with a tattoo(s) is 10.9 (54) 1.6 (8)
sexually promiscuous
Piercings with body jewelry can 56.5 (280) 14.5 (72)
be attractive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 54.2 (269) 13.3 (66)
on women is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 13.2 (65) 2.2 (11)
body jewelry is "sexy" on
a man
Extensive piercings with body 5.1 (25) 1.2 (6)
jewelry are attractive on a
woman
A man with piercing(s) and body 7.9 (39) .6 (3)
jewelry is sexually
promiscuous
A woman with piercing(s) and 12.9 (64) 2.0 (10)
body jewelry is sexually
promiscuous
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 11.1 (54) 1.2 (6)
on men is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 53.7 (264) 11.6 (57)
body jewelry is "sexy" on a
woman
Extensive piercings with body 1.6 (8) .4 (2)
jewelry are attractive
on a man
A person with a tattoo(s) is 11.6 (57) .4 (2)
aggressive
I consider a person with a 12.7 (62) 2.0 (10)
tattoo(s) to have a "bad"
image
Tattoos are appropriate for a 20.9 (103) 3.5 (17)
person of any age
People tend to stereotype 62.2 (303) 26.3 (128)
persons with a tattoo(s)
The number of tattoos that a 61.1 (300) 25.1 (123)
person has makes a difference
in how he/is perceived by
others
Tattoos are appropriate for a 27.6 (136) 2.6 (13)
parent
Tattoos indicate "free 36.2 (178) 2.6 (13)
spiritedness"
Tattoos indicate a "partying 20.7 (101) 1.2 (6)
lifestyle"
Tattoos indicate that the 4.1 (20) .4 (2)
person abuses alcohol or
drugs
A person with a tattoo(s) is 8.8 (43) 1.0 (5)
"tough".
A person with piercing(s) with 4.1 (20) .2 (1)
body jewelry is "tough"
I consider a person with a 10.2 (50) 1.6 (8)
piercing(s) with body jewelry
to have a "bad" image
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 16.5 (81) 2.6 (13)
is appropriate for persons of
any age
The number of piercings with 63.1 (310) 18.5 (91)
body jewelry that a person
has makes a difference in how
he/is perceived by others
A person with a piercing(s) 6.3 (31) .4 (2)
with body jewelry is
aggressive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 28.8 (142) .6 (3)
indicate "free spiritedness"
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 3.5 (17) .4 (2)
indicate that the person
abuses alcohol or drugs
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 19.5 (96) 2.2 (11)
is appropriate for a parent
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 14.2 (70) .6 (3)
indicate a "partying
lifestyle"
* Items in parentheses are frequencies
Table 4: Percentage Agreement and Disagreement for Items Significantly
Related to Gender of Respondent (a)
Item Disagree Agree
Male Female Male Female
Tattoos on men are attractive 36.3 26.1 24.9 51.3
Tattoos on women are attractive 29.2 37.7 49.4 32.4
Extensive tattoos are attractive on a 71.9 85.6 6.7 4.8
man
A small, discrete tattoo is "sexy" on 51.8 39.6 11.5 35.2
a man
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on women 17.5 17.4 72.7 61.3
is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with body 63.6 53.0 5.5 26.9
jewelry is "sexy" on a man
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on men 64.1 59.2 6.0 19.3
is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with body 17.6 21.9 70.2 58.8
jewelry is "sexy" on a woman
Extensive piercings with body jewelry 75.3 88.5 2.0 2.2
are attractive on a man
A man with a tattoo(s) is sexually 49.2 69.2 7.1 7.9
promiscuous
A woman with a tattoo(s) is sexually 46.5 68.2 17.5 6.1
promiscuous
I consider a person with a tattoo(s) 56.9 73.7 17.0 11.8
to have a "bad" image
Tattoos indicate that the person 71.8 84.1 5.5 3.5
abuses alcohol or drugs
A person with piercing(s) with body 72.1 80.7 5.5 3.1
jewelry is "tough"
I consider a person with a piercing(s) 62.4 78.0 15.3 8.3
with body jewelry to have a "bad"
image
(a) Combined responses of Strongly Disagree and Disagree; Strongly
Agree and Agree
Table 5: percentage Agreement and Disagreement for Items
Significantly Related to Region of Residence
Region Pacific Mid Western
Item Disagree Agree Disagree Agree
Tattoos can be attractive 22.4 64.2 25.3 62.7
Tattoos on women are 31.6 42.4 36.7 43.7
attractive
Extensive piercings 78.3 7.2 86.7 3.8
with body jewelry are
attractive on a woman
Piercing(s) with body 27.9 28.5 23.5 43.9
jewelry indicate "free
spiritedness"
Region Southern East Coast
Item Disagree Agree Disagree Agree
Tattoos can be attractive 19.5 57.4 7.9 87.3
Tattoos on women are 37.4 29.9 19.1 57.1
attractive
Extensive piercings 84.2 4.7 60.3 12.7
with body jewelry are
attractive on a woman
Piercing(s) with body 24.5 39.6 27.7 52.3
jewelry indicate "free
spiritedness"
(a) Combined responses of Strongly Disagree and Disagree; Strongly
Agree and Agree
Table 6: Percentage Agreement and Disagreement for Items Significantly
Related to Report of Having and Not Having Body Art a
Does Not Have Has Body Art
Body Art
Item Disagree Agree Disagree Agree
Tattoos can be attractive 26.6 55.5 13.1 78.3
Tattoos on men are attractive 43.0 22.3 14.2 59.9
Tattoos on women are attractive 41.7 34.8 20.3 51.8
A small, discrete tattoo is 25.9 56.3 10.7 70.5
"sexy" on a woman
Extensive tattoos are attractive 81.5 3.7 74.0 8.7
on a man
Extensive tattoos are attractive 91.3 2.4 82.7 8.2
on a woman
A small, discrete tattoo is 51.9 18.6 38.6 27.4
"sexy" on a man
Piercings with body jewelry can 23.7 63.2 7.0 82.8
be attractive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on 23.0 60.5 8.1 78.3
women is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 69.9 6.9 42.6 27.9
body jewelry is "sexy" on a
man
Extensive piercings with body 82.8 3.4 76.7 9.1
jewelry are attractive on a
woman
A man with piercing(s) and body 56.4 8.3 64.4 8.6
jewelry is sexually
promiscuous
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on 72.1 5.6 47.2 22.6
men is attractive
A small, discrete piercing with 25.2 59.9 10.2 73.9
body jewelry is "sexy" on a
woman
Extensive piercings with body 84.5 1.4 77.9 3.1
jewelry are attractive on a
man
A person with a tattoo(s) is 48.8 13.5 66.3 10.2
aggressive
I consider a person with a 56.6 19.4 76.4 8.2
tattoo(s) to have a "bad"
image
Tattoos are appropriate for a 68.5 16.9 50.5 35.2
person of any age
Tattoos are appropriate for a 34.6 22.5 17.8 42.4
parent
Tattoos indicate "free 28.0 33.9 18.4 45.9
spiritedness
Tattoos indicate that the person 71.5 5.9 87.3 2.6
abuses alcohol or drugs
I consider a person with a 61.3 16.6 82.1 5.1
piercing(s) with body jewelry
to have a "bad" image
Piercing(s) with body jewelry is 72.3 12.2 54.1 29.1
appropriate for persons of any
age
A person with a piercing(s) with 68.5 8.6 75.0 3.6
body jewelry is aggressive
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 73.8 4.8 86.3 2.5
indicate that the person
abuses alcohol or drugs
Piercing(s) with body jewelry is 46.9 16.9 34.1 29.5
appropriate for a parent
Piercing(s) with body jewelry 58.6 18.2 70.0 9.6
indicate a "partying
lifestyle"
(a) Combined responses of Strongly Disagree and Disagree; Strongly
Agree and Agree
Table 7: Items Significantly Correlated with Age of Respondent
Item
Tattoos can be attractive r = -.116 p = .011
Tattoos on women are attractive r = -.093 p = .041
Piercings with body jewelry can be attractive r = -.225 p = .000
Piercing(s) with body jewelry on women is r = -.231 p = .000
attractive
A small discrete piercing with body jewelry is r = -.175 p = .000
"sexy" on a woman
Tattoos are appropriate for a parent r = -.118 p = .010
Tattoos indicate that the person abuses alcohol r = -.143 p = .002
or drugs
I consider a person with a piercing(s) with r = -.217 p = .000
body jewelry to have a "bad" image
Piercing(s) with body jewelry indicate that the r = -.156 p = .001
person abuses alcohol or drugs
Piercing(s) with body jewelry indicate a r = -.101 p = .026
"partying lifestyle"