A framework for examining the role of culture in individuals likelihood to engage in self-gift behavior.
Weisfeld-Spolter, Suri ; Thakkar, Maneesh
INTRODUCTION
"Amy White is getting an early start on her holiday shopping
list. The St. Louis attorney has already picked out a Harry Potter DVD,
a $70 coffee-table book and a $39 Victoria's Secret sweater.
There's even a $625 purple vase on the list. And the lucky
recipient ... Ms. White herself" (Mechling, 2010).
Gift giving theory and research has primarily focused on
interpersonal gifts (e.g. Belk, 1979). However, as demonstrated by the
statement above, people may also give gifts to themselves, and it is
believed that the self- gift phenomenon may be widely occurring in
American society (Mick and DeMoss, 1990a, 1990b). In fact, previous
self-gifting research has primarily focused on Western consumer behavior
(e.g. self-gift motivations and occasions (Mick and eMoss, 1990a,
1990b), cognitive processes (Olshavsky and Lee, 1993), materialism
(McKeage, Richins, and Debevec, 1993), attribution of achievement
outcomes (Faure and Mick, 1993; Mick and Faure, 1998) etc.) However, it
is well established that people with different cultural backgrounds may
behave differently and have different reactions to similar situations.
More specifically, research has shown that different cultural
identifications have an impact on the way people think, feel, and behave
(e.g. Markus and Kitayama, 1991). This leads to the question then of
whether the results of previous self-gifting studies can be generalized
to consumers living in other countries. More specifically, the first
question we have to ask is does the concept of a self-gift even exist in
other countries or is it one unique to the United States. This research
proposes to answer this question by developing a theory that examines
different types of consumers attitude and likelihood towards self-gift
giving.
More specifically, to investigate potential individual differences
(reflecting differences in culture) that may exist with regards to
consumers attitude and behavior of self-gifting, the Theory of Reasoned
Action, with its established usefulness for predicting and explaining
behavior, combined with self-construal, one of the most popular
constructs used to explain the effects of culture on a variety of
outcomes, will be used to develop a theory to answer this question.
SELF-GIFTING AND ADVERTISING
"A new name has cropped up on holiday shopping lists: Me"
(Mayk, 2009). Self-gifts are conceptualized as (1) personally symbolic
self-communication through (2) special indulgences that tend to be (3)
premeditated and (4) highly context bound (Mick and DeMoss 1990b,
p.328). Previous research has substantiated the notion that self-gifts
are a fairly common and important phenomenon particularly in western
consumer behavior (e.g. Faure and Mick, 1993). According to social
researchers, Western individuals have become increasingly self-oriented
in their purchases and consumption behavior (Mick, DeMoss and Faber
1992), and an example of this phenomena has been labeled self-gifts.
Similarly, McKeage et al. (1993) believe that people have been giving
gifts to themselves since the early beginnings of self-indulgence.
Marketers have recognized this trend in the United States and have
directed their product development efforts and advertising messages
accordingly. For example, the diamond industry has caught onto the new
"me" mood, with slogans like "Your left hand says
'we,' you're right hand says 'me'," urging
women to buy diamonds for themselves. Slogans such as "You deserve
a break today" (McDonald's) and "The perfect little
thank-me" (Andes candies) present indulgences as personal rewards
(Mick and DeMoss, 1990b).
American advertisers have been capitalizing on consumers'
self-gift propensities for some time. The question then becomes whether
the propensity to self gift is confined to the United States, or a more
wide spread phenomenon. This has important managerial implications for
marketers of self-gifts, particularly those in Western cultures that
want to market their products abroad. Specifically, the question of
which advertising strategy to pursue- one of customization or
standardization arises. In other words, can they use the same
"self-gift" message (standardization) that they use in the
United States or do they need to develop a more tailored message
(customization) for consumers in different countries stressing different
benefits.
The controversy of choosing a standardization strategy versus a
customization strategy is one that is heavily debated in the
international marketing literature (e.g. Buzzel 1968; Levitt 1983;
Onkvisit Shaw 1987; Yip 1989; Jain 1989; Saimee and Roth 1992; Szymanski
et al. 1993 etc.). Proponents of standardization argue that consumers
are becoming more homogeneous due to advances in technology and that
standardization results in economies of scale which translate into
higher profits. On the other hand, those that argue for customization
claim that the world is not one big homogeneous market, and that for
companies to succeed they have to "think local and act local."
They believe that marketers need to segment their audience and target
their products and strategy accordingly or risk losing out to local
competitors who better meet their needs.
Research has been done in the United States regarding marketing
strategies for self-gift retailers (Mick, DeMoss and Faber, 1992). For
example, based on their study of women purchasing perfume for their own
use, Mick, DeMoss and Faber (1992), suggested that advertising that
emphasized message themes about the increasing professional and personal
independence of women may heighten the self gift propensity of women.
However, to date no studies have been conducted on the self-gift
propensity of international consumers.
The primary goal of this research therefore is to identify if
self-gifting is a universal phenomenon, and address the gap in the
literature regarding self-gift behavior in a context other than the
United States. The results of this research have important implications
for managers, particularly when choosing an international advertising
strategy. It will help companies who market their products in the United
Sates using self-gift appeals in their advertisements decide whether
they should use these messages in other countries or need to modify them
to successfully sell their products.
SELF-CONSTRUAL AND SELF-GIFTING
Overall, it has been suggested that self-gifts represent a complex
class of personal acquisitions that offer intriguing insights on
self-directed consumer behavior (Mick and DeMoss 1990b). "Self gift
theory will likely benefit from drawing on additionally relevant
psychological research" (Mick and DeMoss, 1990b p. 329) since
"with rich and complex qualities, self-gifts provide a window
through which consumer behavior can be viewed in some of its most
adaptive, dramatic and personal significant forms" (Mick and
DeMoss, 1990b p.331).
As demonstrated by its name and definition, one of the predominant
aspects of self-gifting is the direct focus and communication with the
self. Self-concept, defined as ones beliefs of the images that he/she
has about the self (Sirgy, 1982) is crucial to self-gifts in that
self-gift giving is intertwined with self-concepts (Mick and DeMoss
1990a, 1990b; Mick, DeMoss and Faber, 1992). Clearly, if people view the
self differently, they will react differently to self-gifting.
Additionally, considerable evidence exists of cross-cultural differences
in the nature of self-concept (e.g. Dhawan et al., 1995; Ip and Bond,
1995) and at least some of the self-concept differences between cultures
can be explained by underlying cultural dimensions such as individualism
and collectivism (Watkins et al. 2003). Triandis (1989, 1990) has
proposed that in individualist cultures, idiocentric self-conceptions
are more likely, whereas in collectivist cultures, the self is more
likely to be relational in nature with group cognitions prominent.
Self construals are aspects of self-concept that have been advanced
as individual level cultural dimensions that are thought to explain the
effects of culture on a variety of outcome measures. They are considered
to be individual differences that reflect cultural differences.
Self-construal is conceptualized as "constellation of thoughts,
feelings, and actions concerning one's relationship to others, and
the self as distinct from others" (Singelis, 1994, p. 581). Taken
together with the recommendation above by Mick and DeMoss (1990), we
propose to draw on the psychological research of Markus and Kitayama
(1991) who identified two dimensions of the self that can be used to
characterize consumers self-construal as well as explain and identify
differences between cultures: independence and interdependence.
According to Markus and Kitayama, self-construal can be
conceptualized by the degree of independence/interdependence that a
person possesses. They further state that all people contain both an
independent and interdependent self, but that the culture in which they
are bought up in influences which one dominates. For example, Europeans
and Caucasian Americans are typically said to have an independent self
because they tend to emphasize the individual whereas people from Asian
cultures are typically interdependent because they tend to emphasize the
group as more important than the individual. People with independent
self-construals strive to develop and express their unique
characteristics, whereas people with interdependent self-construals
place value on harmonious relationships with others and acceptance in
their community. Those with well-developed independent self-construals
gain self-esteem through expressing the self and validating their
internal attributes, whereas harmonious interpersonal relationships and
the ability to adjust to various situations are sources of self esteem
for the interdependent self-construal (Singelis 1994). After reviewing
an extensive array of studies, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argue that
these independent and interdependent views of the self influence
cognition, emotion, and motivation and help to explain individual
differences between cultural groups.
The influence of culture on behavior is supported empirically in
various studies (e.g. Aaker and Maheswaran 1997). Triandis (1988)
presented an explanation on the influence of culture on behavior by
using the concept of the self as a mediating variable between culture
and individual behavior. He concluded that culture affects behavior both
by influencing self-image and by defining situations. Similarly, we
propose that consumers self-construal will have an impact on their
attitude and likelihood to self-gift as well as the type of self-gift.
More specifically, it seems that people with an independent
self-construal will be more likely to self-gift, as they tend to have
self-benefiting motivations, such as the need to achieve and
self-enhance. Conversely, people with an interdependent self-construal
will be less likely to self-gift as they tend to derive their
motivations from what benefits others and a group as a whole, such as
the need to be agreeable to others, to accommodate to their needs, and
to restrain one's own wishes or wants.
In short, self-construal as defined by independence versus
interdependence is the construct that will be used as a determinant of
consumers attitudes towards self-gifting, since it emphasizes
differences of ones view of the self and may influence the cognition,
emotion and motivation to self-gift. In addition, the Theory Of Reasoned
Action (TRA) (Fishbein and Ajzen;1975, 1981), one of the most widely
accepted and employed predictor of behavior theories in the social
sciences (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1973; Ryan and Bonfield, 1975; Sheppard,
Hartwick and Warshaw 1988) will be used to develop our theory on
self-gift likelihood across cultures. The TRA has also been tested cross
culturally and the findings have generally been consistent with the
theory (e.g., Gidin et al., 1996).
The Theory of Reasoned Action predicts that a person's
attitude and subjective norm both impact behavioral intent, which
subsequently predicts behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1970, 1974, 1980;
Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). The value of the attitudinal and normative
components, as well as their relative weights in predicting behavioral
intention, vary from person to person depending on a variety of social,
cultural and individual factors. For example, self-related constructs
(e.g. Miller and Grush, 1986), and culture (e.g. Lee and Green, 1991)
are some of the factors that influence the relative weights of the two
components in predicting behavioral intention. Charng, Piliavin and
Callero (1988) suggest that "one might attain better prediction and
better understanding of repeated behaviors if one were to add to the
variables included in the theory of reasoned action some measure of the
individual's self-concept in relation to the behavior one is trying
to predict" (p.304). According to Park and Levine (1999)
self-construals merit consideration as factors affecting the attitudinal
and normative components. "Self-construals represent another branch
in the study of self with an emphasis on how the self may differ in
different cultural contexts."(Park and Levine, 1999, p.200). The
conceptual model can be seen below.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
HYPOTHESIS DEVELOPMENT
According to our model, self-construal will influence people's
subjective norm and attitude towards self-gifting. Together, the
subjective norm and attitude towards self-gifting will be indicative of
consumers intention to self-gift, and this intention according to the
theory is the best predictor of what their actual behavior will be like.
We also propose that their behavior will be moderated by the context of
the self-gift, depending on whether they are self-gifting for reward or
therapeutic reasons. This will be discussed below.
Self-Construal and Attitude
As mentioned earlier, it appears that self-gift purchasing may be
linked to both cultural and personal values. For example, Shapiro (1993)
suggested that self-gift behavior may be particularly linked to cultural
beliefs that purchasing and consumption are appropriate to the pursuit
of individual happiness. In addition, McKeage, Richins, and Debevec
(1993) showed that materialists are more likely to self-gift than
non-materialists due to their attitude of materialism as a personal
value.
Clearly attitude is a key construct in determining self-gift
behavior. We propose that self-construal will have an effect on the
persons attitude towards self-gifting in general and towards
self-gifting advertisements specifically. More specifically, since
people with an independent self-construal place an emphasis on "a)
internal abilities, thoughts and feelings, b) being unique and
expressing the self, c) realizing internal attributes and promoting ones
own goals, and d) being direct in communication" (Singelis 1994 p.
581) and people with an interdependent self-construal emphasize "a)
external public features such as statuses, roles and relationships, b)
belonging and fitting in, c) occupying one's proper place and
engaging in appropriate action, and d) being indirect in
communication" their attitude towards self-gifting will reflect
these tendencies. People with an independent self-construal will have a
more favorable attitude towards self-gifting, as they tend to have
self-benefiting motivations, such as the need to achieve and
self-enhance. Conversely, people with an interdependent self-construal
will have a less favorable attitude towards self-gifting as they tend to
derive their motivations from what benefits others and a group as a
whole, such as the need to be agreeable to others, to accommodate to
their needs, and to restrain one's own wishes or wants.
In addition, Park and Levine, (1999) found that the strength of an
individuals independent self-construal was positively correlated with
their personal attitude towards behavior and that the strength of an
individuals interdependent self-construal was negatively related to ones
personal attitude toward behavior. This is in support of our hypotheses.
More formally:
H1: Individuals with independent (interdependent) self-construals
will have a more (less) favorable attitude towards self-gifting than
individuals with interdependent (independent) self-construals.
Self-Gift Ads and attitude
Belk, Bryce, and Pollay (1985) suggest that in order for ads to be
effective, they have to appeal to the values that are salient in the
culture of its intended audience. Not surprisingly, ads emphasizing
values that are consistent with the value orientation of the society
tend to be more persuasive. It has also been shown that one very
important dimension of culture to consider when developing international
advertising messages is that of individualism-collectivism (Han and
Shavitt, 1994; Zandpour et al., 1994). Similarly, Gregory and Munch
(1997) found that cultural values (norms and roles) are important
dimensions to consider when developing international advertisements.
In their 1994 study, Han and Shavitt showed that members of
individualistic and collectivistic societies responded differently to
ads emphasizing individualistic versus collectivistic appeals. They
found that subjects in the United States were more persuaded overall by
ads emphasizing individualistic benefits, whereas subjects in Korea
tended to be more persuaded by ads emphasizing collectivistic benefits.
"It is evident that cultural differences in
individualism-collectivism play an important role in the persuasion
processes both at the societal and the individual level, influencing the
prevalence and effectiveness of different types of advertising
appeals" (343).
Therefore, ads presenting norms and roles that are consistent with
individuals' self-construal will be more persuasive. Self-gift
appeals will be favored by individuals with independent self-construals
but will not be responded to favorably by individuals with
interdependent self-construals. Conversely, if self-gift advertisers can
use an appeal not emphasizing the independence and deservingness of
individuals, but rather something more inline with the subjective norm
of interdependents, these ads will be responded to more favorably by
interdependents and may increase the likelihood of their self-gift
behavior. More specifically:
H2a: Individuals with independent self-construals (compared with
interdependent self-construals) will respond more favorably to self-gift
ads that present cultural norms consistent with an individualistic
orientation. (e.g. "Because I'm worth it," Loreal)
H2b: Individuals with interdependent self-construals (compared with
independent self-construals) will respond more favorably to self-gift
ads that present cultural norms consistent with a collectivist
orientation. (e.g. "Your family thinks your worth it")
Self-Construal and Subjective Norm
Subjective norm is composed of two components: normative beliefs
and motivation to comply. Normative beliefs represent an
individual's perception of what significant others would think
about his/her performing behavior and the motivation to comply with
others refers to ones willingness to follow others wishes. Because those
with independent self-construals emphasize their own thoughts, feelings
and actions, rather than others, and like to feel independent, they will
most likely not feel compelled to comply with others thoughts on
self-gifting, because it will be an invasion on their sense of
independence. Conversely, individuals with interdependent
self-construals who do not view conformity to what others think and feel
in a negative light, but rather consider it rewarding to meet others
expectations and maintain harmony with significant others, will be more
likely to comply with the expectations of others.
This is in line with research done by Kim and Markus (1999) who did
a study comparing values of uniqueness and conformity in East Asia and
the United States. Referring to East Asians and their desire for
conformity, they say "it is not that they conform because they
experience social pressure to conform but that they actively like to
conform in the sense of being connected to others, For east Asians,
following norms validates the self as a good person" (p.786). In
contrast, individuals in the United States believe that "attitudes,
feelings, and behavior should be determined by the self without being
controlled by any external cause.... conformity threatens the self as a
worthy individual whereas uniqueness symbolizes the assertion of
individuality and self-worth ..." (p.786).
Similarly, we predict that individuals with independent
self-construals will not be motivated to comply with how significant
others view self-gifting, though the normative belief will probably be
positive, and that individuals with interdependent self-construals will
be motivated to comply with the normative beliefs of others, and these
normative beliefs about self-gifting will not be positive.
In sum:
H3a: Individuals with independent self-construals will have
positive normative beliefs about self-gifting.
H3b: Individuals with interdependent self-construals will have
negative normative beliefs about self-gifting.
H4a: Individuals with independent self-construals will not be
motivated to comply with the thoughts of significant others
H4b: Individuals with interdependent self-construals will be
motivated to comply with the thoughts of significant others.
Behavioral Intention
Based on the model, the attitude and subjective norm will indicate
the behavioral intention of the individual to self-gift. The weights
attached to these two constructs vary depending on the topic being
investigated. Factors such as individual or cultural differences
influence both a person's attitude toward behavior and subjective
norm as well as the relative weights of the two. For example, subjective
norm was found to be a stronger determinant of the intention to buy
sneakers for Koreans, while attitude toward behavior was a better
predictor of the behavioral intention for Americans ( Lee and Green,
1991).
We therefore propose that for individuals with independent
self-construals, their own attitude (rather than the subjective norm)
towards self-gifting will play a more influential role in predicting
their intention to self-gift, and that for individuals with
interdependent self-construals, the subjective norm (rather than
attitude) will play a more influential role in determining their
intention to self-gift.
More formally,
H5a: For individuals with independent self-construals, attitude,
rather than subjective norm will be more influential in predicting
individuals' behavioral intention to self-gift.
H5b: For individuals with interdependent self-construals, the
subjective norm, rather than attitude will be more influential in
predicting individuals' behavioral intention to self gift.
Self-gift Behavior: Moderated By Context
According to the model, the context of the self-gift will play a
moderating role on individuals' self-gift behavior. These two
contexts are reward (buying yourself a gift after a great
accomplishment) and therapy (buying yourself a gift to cheer yourself up
after a disappointment). These contexts and their role in the model will
be discussed below.
Early research on self-gifts revealed that self-gifts appeared to
be acquired within a relatively confined set of circumstances and
motivations (Mick and DeMoss 1990a). More specifically, eight contexts
for self-gifts were identified based on focus group discussions and
prior qualitative survey research (Mick and DeMoss 1990a, 1990b): 1) to
reward yourself, 2) to cheer yourself up, 3) because it was a holiday,
4) to relieve stress, 5) as an incentive to reach a personal goal, 6)
because it was your birthday, 7) just to be nice to yourself, and
8)because you had extra money to spend (Mick and DeMoss 1992).
As mentioned above, eight contexts of self-gifts were identified,
however the reward and therapy contexts appear to be the two predominant
contexts of self-gifts (Mick and DeMoss 1990a, 1990b, 1992) and are the
two contexts that this research will study. Specifically, we hypothesize
that these two contexts will play a moderating role on self-gift
behavior.
In their 1993 study, Faure and Mick suggest testing whether or not
there are any differences between the types of self-gifts one buys after
a success or after a failure. Mick and Faure (1998) also suggested that
therapeutic self-gifts may result from a different type of psychological
process than reward self-gifts. In keeping with this train of thought,
we propose that the context of the self-gift may play a moderating role
in consumers' likelihood to self-gift.
More specifically, previous research has shown that when
self-gifting, there was a higher frequency of the reward motivation
reported by males (Mick and DeMoss's 1990a) and Mick and DeMoss
(1992) found that females were more likely to engage in self gift
behavior in therapeutic and nice to self contexts, whereas males were
more inclined in situations where the self gift served as an incentive
to reach a goal. Similarly, Markus and Kitayama (1991) suggest that
American men and women develop divergent self-construals similar to
those observed cross-culturally. Men typically develop an independent
self-construal, common in Western cultures, in which representations of
others are separate from the self. Similar to Eastern societies, women
typically develop an interdependent self-construal, in which others are
viewed as part of the self (Cross & Madson, 1997).
Taken together, these results point to the following propositions:
H6a: Individuals with independent self-construals will be more
likely to self-gift in reward contexts and less likely to self-gift in
therapy contexts.
H6b: Individuals with interdependent self-construals will be more
likely to self-gift in therapy contexts and less likely in reward
contexts.
CONCLUSION
The purpose of this research was to propose a theory to investigate
whether self-gifting is a universal phenomenon, or one confined to
Western societies. Does everybody self-gift? What are the influences on
self-gift behavior? In an attempt to establish who is most likely to
self-gift, we have employed the Theory of Reasoned Action and
incorporated self-construal to make various predictions. In short, it is
hypothesized that individuals who possess a dominant independent
self-construal will have more favorable attitudes towards self-gifting
and be more likely to self-gift, whereas individuals with dominant
interdependent self-construals will tend to have more negative attitudes
towards self-gifting and a strong motivation to comply with references
groups feelings about self-gifting, and consequently be less likely to
self gift.
In addition, though the hypotheses predict that individual with
independent self-construals will a more favorable attitude towards
self-gifting and be more likely to self-gift than individuals with
interdependent self-construals, this does not mean that individual with
interdependent self-construals cannot be effectively targeted and
motivated to self-gift. The right appeal combined with a positive
subjective norm can encourage individuals with interdependent
self-construals to self-gift as well. For example, rather than using
slogans like those used in the United States that emphasizes the worth
of the individual, (i.e. You deserve it), slogans that emphasize family
and friends will be more effective for individuals with interdependent
self-construals.
This research has contributed to the self-gifting literature by
extending the self-gift research cross culturally, as well as by
developing a theory that predicts self-gift attitude and behavior. In a
recent study, based on empirical evidences, Tsai (2005) suggests that
self-gifting behavior is a significant antecedent in individuals'
likelihood of buying luxury brands. Further, it has important marketing
implications for marketers of self-gift products and for marketers that
use self-gift appeals in their advertisements that want to target
consumers that are not the typical western independent individual.
REFERENCES
Aaker, J., & Maheswaran, (1997). The effect of Cultural
Orientation on Persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (December)
241-261.
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1970). The Prediction of Behavior for
Attitudinal and Variables. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 5:
400-416
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M (1973). Attitudinal and Normative
Variables as Predictors of Specific Behaviors. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 6, 466-487.
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1974). Factors Influencing Intentions
and Intention-Behavior Relation. Human Relations 21: 1-15.
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M (1980). Understanding Attitudes and
Predicting Social Behavior" Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Belk, Russel W. (1979). Gift-giving Behavior. In Research in
Marketing, Vol. 2, ed. Jagdish Seth, Greewich, CT: JAI, 95-126.
Belk, R. W. and R. W. Pollay (1985). Images of Oursleves: The Good
Life in Twentieth Century Advertising. Journal of Consumer Research,
11(March): 887-897.
Belk, Russel W. (1988). Possessions and the Extended Self. Journal
of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 139-168
Buzzell, R. (1968). Can You Standardize Multinational Marketing.
Harvard Business Reviews, 46(6), November-December, 702.
Charng, H.W., Pillavin, J.A, and Callero, P.L. (1988). Role
Identity and Reasoned Action in the Prediction of Repeated Behavior.
Social Psychology Quarterly, Vol.51, No. 4, p. 303-317.
Cross, S.E. & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the Self:
Self-construals and Gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37.
Dhawan, N., Roseman, J., Naidu, R.K., Thapa, K., & Rettek, S.I.
(1995). Self-concepts Across Two Cultures: India and the United States.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 606-621.
Faure, Corinne; Mick, David Glen (1993). Self-gifts Through the
Lens of Attribution Theory. Advances in Consumer Research, 1993, Vol. 20
Issue 1, p553, 4p.
Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I (1975). Belief, Attiude Intention and
Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading , MA: Addison-
Wealey.
Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1981). On Construct Validity: A
Critique of Miniard and Cohen's Paper. Journal of Social
Psychology, 17: 340-350.
Godin, G. Maticka-Tynadale, E., Adrian, A., Manson Singer, S.,
Williams, D., and Cappon, P. (1996). Cross Cultural Testing of three
Social Cognitive Theories: An application of condom use. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1556-1586.
Gould, Stephen J. (1997). An interpretive study of purposeful, mood
self-regulating consumption: The consumption and mood. Psychology &
Marketing, Jul97, Vol. 14 Issue 4, p395, 32p.
Gregory, Gary and Munch, James (1997). Cultural Values in
International Advertising: An Examination of Familial Norms and Roles in
Mexico. Psychology and Marketing, March 1997, Volume 14 (2) P.99-119.
Han, S., & Shavitt, S. (1994). Persuasion and Culture:
Advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic societies
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 326-350.
Hirschman Elizabeth C.; Morris B. Holbrook (1982). Hedonic
Consumption: Emergent Concepts, Methods and Propositions. Journal of
Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures consequences. Beverley Hills, CA:
Sage.
Hofstede, G. (1983) National Cultures in four dimensions, a
research based theory on cultural differences among nations.
International Studies in Management and Organizations, 14, 1-2, 46-74.
Hofstede, G. (1994). Cultures and Organizations: Intercultural
Cooperation and its Imprtance For Survival. McGraw-Hill, UK, 1994.
Ip, G.W.M., & Bond, M.H. (1995). Culture, Values and the
Spontaneous Self-Concept. Asian Journal of Psychology, 1, 29-35.
Jain, Subhash C. (1989). Standardization of International Marketing
Strategy: Some Research Hypotheses. Journal of Marketing, 53(1),
January, 70-79.
Kim, H., & Markus, H.R. (1999). Deviance or Uniqueness, Harmony
or Conformity? A Cultural Analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, vol. 77 (4) 785-800.
Lee, C. and Green, R.T. (1991). Cross Cultural Examination of the
Fishbein Behavioral Intentions Model. Journal of International business
Studies, second quarter, 289-309.
Levitt, Theodore (1983). The Globalization of Markets. Harvard
Business Review, 61(3), May-June, 92-102.
Luomala, Harri T.; Laaksonen, Martti (1997). Mood-regulatory
self-gifts: Development of a conceptual framework. Journal of Economic
Psychology, Jun97, Vol. 18 Issue 4, p407, 28p.
Luomala, Harri T.; Laaksonen, Martti (1999). A qualitative
exploration of mood-regulatory self-gift behaviors. Journal of Economic
Psychology, Vol 20(2), Apr 1999. pp. 147-182.
Markus, H.R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the
self-implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological
Review, 20, 568-579.
Mayk, Lauren; (2009). Looking out for number one; Survey: Consumers
plan to shop for themselves this holiday season. Sarasota
Herald-Tribune, October 2009, D1.
McGrath, Mary Ann; Sherry Jr., John F.; Levy, Sidney J. (1993).
Giving Voice to the Gift: The Use of Projective Techniques to Recover
Lost Meanings. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1993, Vol. 2 Issue 2,
p171, 21p.
McKeage, Kim K.R.; Richins, Marsha L (1993). Self-gifts and the
manifestation of material values. Advances in Consumer Research, 1993,
Vol. 20 Issue 1, p359, 6p.
Mechling, Lauren (2010). The Me Christmas. The Wall Street Journal,
November 2010 A1.
Mick, David Glen; DeMoss, Michelle (1990a). To Me From Me: A
Descriptive Phenomenology of Self-Gifts. Advances in Consumer Research,
1990, Vol. 17 Issue 1, p677, 6p.
Mick, David Glen; DeMoss, Michelle. (1990b). Self-Gifts:
Phenomenological Insights from Four Contexts. Journal of Consumer
Research, Dec90, Vol. 17 Issue 3, p322, 11p.
Mick, David Glen; DeMoss, Michelle (1992). Further findings on
self-gifts: Products, qualities, and socioeconomic correlates. Advances
in Consumer Research, 1992, Vol. 19 Issue 1, p140, 7p.
Mick, David Glen; DeMoss, Michelle; Faber, Ronald J. (1992). A
Projective Study of Motivations and Meanings of Self-Gifts: Implications
for Retail Management. Journal of Retailing, Summer 92, Vol. 68 Issue 2,
p122, 23p.
Mick, David (1993). Self-gifts: An emerging category of consumer
behavior from multiple perspectives. Advances in Consumer Research,
1993, Vol. 20 Issue 1, p546, 1p.
Mick, David Glen; Faure, Corinne (1998). Consumer self-gifts in
achievement contexts: The role of ... International Journal of Research
in Marketing, Oct98, Vol. 15 Issue 4, p293, 15p.
Miller, L.E., and Grush, J.E. (1986). Individual differences in
attitudinal versus normative determination of behavior. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 190-202.
Minowa, Yuko; Gould, Stephen J (1999). Love My Gift, Love Me or is
it Love Me, Love My Gift: A Study of the Cultural Construction of
Romantic Gift Giving among Japanese Couples. Advances in Consumer
Research, 1999, Vol. 26 Issue 1, p119, 6p.
Olshavsky, Richard W.; Lee, Dong Hwan (1993). Self-gifts: A
metacognition perspective. Advances in Consumer Research, 1993, Vol. 20
Issue 1, p547, 6p.
Onkvisit, Sak and John J. Shaw (1987). Standardized International
Advertising: A Review and Critical Evaluation of the Theoretical and
Empirical Evidence. Columbia Journal of World Business, 22(3), Fall,
43-55.
Park, Hee and Levine, Timothy (1999). The Theory of Reasoned Action
and Self-Construal: Evidence from Three Cultures. Communication
Monographs, Volume 66, p. 199-218.
Ryan, M.J., and Bonfield, E.H. (1975). The Fishbein extended model
and Consumer Behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 2, 118-136.
Samiee, Saeed, and Kendell Roth (1992). The Influence of Global
Marketing Standardization on Performance. Journal of Marketing, 56,
April 1-17
Sayre, Shay; Horne, David, (1996). I shop, therefore I am: The role
of possessions for self definition. Advances in Consumer Research, 1996,
Vol. 23 Issue 1, p323, 6p.
Shapiro, John M (1993). Compulsive buying and self-gifts: A
motivational perspective. Advances in Consumer Research, 1993, Vol. 20
Issue 1, p557.
Sheppard, B.H., Hartwick, J., and Warshaw, P.R. (1988). The Theory
of Reasoned Action: A Meta Analysis of Past Research with
recommendations for Modification and Future Research. Journal of
Consumer Research 15, 325-343.
Singelis, T.M. (1994). The measurement of independent and
interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 20, 580-591
Sirgy, M. Joseph (1982). Self Concept in Consumer Behavior: A
Critical Review. Journal of Consumer Research, December, Volume 9 Issue
3, P287 14p.
Szymanski, David M., Sundar G. Bharadwaj, and P. Rajan Varadarajan
(1993). Standardization versus Adaptation of international Marketing
Strategy: An Empirical Investigation. Journal of Marketing, 57
(October), 1-17.
Triandis, H.C. (1989) The Self and Social Behavior in Differing
Cultural Contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 3, 506-520.
Tsai, Shu-pei. (2005). Impact of personal orientation on
luxury-brand purchase value: An international investigation.
International Journal of Market Research, .Vol 47(4), p. 429.
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and
collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1989.
(pp. 41-133) Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.
Watkins, D., Cheng, C., Mpofu, E., Olowu, S., Singh-Sengupta, S.,
& Regmi, M., Gender differences in Self-Construal: How Generalizable
are Western Findings? Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 143, 4, p501,
19p.
Williams, Laura A.; Burns, Alvin C (1994). The halcyon days of
youth: A phenomenological account of experiences and feelings
accompanying. Advances in Consumer Research, 1994, Vol. 21 Issue 1, p98,
6p.
Yip, George S., Pierre M. Loewe, and Michael Y. Yoshino (1988). How
to Take your Company to the Global Market, Columbia Journal of World
Business, 23(4), Winter, 37-47.
Zandpour, F., Campos, V., Catalano, J., Chang, C., Cho, Y.,
Hoobyar, R., Jiang, S., Lin, M., Madrid, S., Scheidler, H., &
Osborn, S. (1994). Global reach and Local Touch: Achieving Cultural
fitness in TV advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 35-63.
Suri Weisfeld-Spolter, Nova University
Maneesh Thakkar, Radford University