From cognition to behavior: a cross cultural study for global business effectiveness.
Strubler, David ; Agarwal, Atul ; Park, Sung-Hee 等
INTRODUCTION
With globalization expanding at an exponential rate, the need for
cross-culturally effective managers and workers has increased
significantly. Professionals with strong and favorable intercultural
attitudes, skills, knowledge, and behavior add value to their
organizations. Cross-cultural effectiveness builds organizational
learning, making it possible to expand and integrate marketing, sales,
supply chains and operations to other countries. Whether professionals
are working overseas as expatriates or conducting short-business trips,
parent and host organizations need methods to ensure the success of
multinational business ventures.
Black, Mendenhall, & Oddou (1991) first supported the idea that
international adjustment was the result of an interaction effect of
multiple variables in sequence. They found that training and previous
experience in the pre-departure stage are prescriptions for improving
the accuracy of an assigned expatriate's perception of themselves
and the situation prior to departure. Some other studies found
pre-departure selection and preparation to be important factors for
adjustment while identifying variables that predict intercultural
effectiveness for either selection or training purposes (Hutchings,
2002; Liu & Lee, 2008). It should be noted that many competency
trait variables, such as flexibility, are cross-culture general traits
so that they apply regardless of which culture the expatriate is
entering (Adler, 1974; Bochner, 1973).
However, in the late 1980s, much of the trait research started to
shift toward integrating cognitive and behavioral research primarily due
to limited support for the links between traits and performance (Hammer,
1987). Other research showed that a combination of these trait and
behavioral factors, in addition to culture specific preparation, may
play an important role in expatriate success (Pires, Stanton, &
Ostenfeld, 2006; Elmer, 1986). For example, in a recent study, Smith and
Reynolds (2009) set out to "assess differences between cognitive
and affective measures and their ability to predict behavioral
intentions and the impact of service features on these measures"
among a population of subjects from a variety of cultures. They found
that for all cultural groups, overall quality, satisfaction, and
positive affect predict behavioral intentions."
Building on and modifying the model of Black et al. (1991),
Strubler & Park (2009) have argued that assigned expatriates should
engage in (and their parent corporations ought to provide)
cross-cultural effectiveness testing for selection purposes and to
ensure the accuracy of pre-departure expectations as shown in Table 1.
While testing should not be the only criteria for expatriate selection,
testing does make both the individual and organization aware of
strengths and weaknesses prior to selection and departure. Testing also
makes it possible to design appropriate training for the expatriate.
Black et al. (1991) identified key factors affecting intercultural
effectiveness and adjustment. They define 1) intercultural effectiveness
as the ability of a person within the intercultural environment, and 2)
adjustment as the overall multifaceted process through which expatriates
develop an increasing degree of satisfaction in being able to cope with
a cross-cultural environment. Therefore, it should be noted that
intercultural effectiveness and adjustment are not equivalent concepts.
Intercultural effectiveness may predispose an individual toward
adjustment and without it the individual may not be able to adjust.
However, adjustment is a complex process involving a large number of
variables, many of which are outside the control of the individual,
e.g., family and organization support. Therefore, intercultural
effectiveness is necessary but insufficient for adjustment.
Still, according to Black et al. (1991), intercultural
effectiveness and adjustment possess four common dimensions, namely,
self-orientation, other-orientation, perceptual skills and cultural
toughness. Self-oriented individuals engage in activities and have
attributes that increase their self-esteem and confidence while finding
replacements for their home interests and activities. They, in turn,
handle stress well and demonstrate efficacy in both the work and social
environments. Other-oriented individuals have the ability to develop
relationships with host nationals and actively seek and find mentors.
Individuals with strong perceptual skills tend to engage in
non-judgmental, non-evaluative mental processing about their situation.
Successful expatriates are often required to adjust to cultural
toughness, which refers to differences in standards of living that
expatriates experience: the greater the difference, the more difficult
the adjustment. Therefore, these individual factors seem to contribute
to adjustment.
[TABLE 1 OMITTED]
Fisher and Hartel (2003) further assert that three personal factors
play a significant role in intercultural effectiveness: 1) ability to
communicate effectively, 2) to establish relationships, and 3) to cope
with psychological stress. Still another recent study proposed that
emotions, especially for individualists working in collectivistic
cultures for long periods of time, play a major role in cross-cultural
success. More specifically, emotional demands caused by cultural
differences in expatriate encounters impact negatively on their
experience (Tan, Hartel, Panipucci & Strybosch, 2005). Therefore,
emotional maturity may be a major factor, at least when cultural
differences are great, in determining competency and possibly whether an
expatriate will complete a long-term assignment. Emotional maturity or
intelligence is defined as "an array of capabilities, competencies,
and skills that influence one's ability to cope with environmental
demands" (Tan et al., 2005). Four key factors are cited: emotional
appraisal and expression, emotional regulation in self and others,
promotion of intellectual and emotional growth, and generation of
emotions to assist in problem solving (Salovey & Mayer, 1990; Tan et
al., 2005). For example, expatriates who can deal with negative emotions
in a positive manner can experience continued job satisfaction in a
cross-cultural environment. Further, expatriates with idiocentric
personalities, i.e., individualists who view the environment as unstable
and themselves as stable, will experience a higher degree of emotional
labor. Also, individualists (regardless of gender) prefer to be frank
about their emotions and will fare better in feminine cultures where
there is more freedom to express even their feelings of frustration
(Hofstede, 1980; Mumby & Putnam, 1992; Ollilainen, 2000). Finally,
high status expatriates serving in collectivistic cultures will
experience less emotional labor than low status expatriates. A deeper
look at the role of emotion in cross-cultural competency implies that
multiple intervening variables, including cultural dimensions and
individual personality factors, create a more complicated interaction
effect.
The Intercultural Competency Scale, used in this study and
developed by Elmer (1986), identified twelve intercultural competency
traits from an extensive literature review. These include the following
twelve dimensions: Approachability, Intercultural Receptivity, Positive
Realistic Orientation, Forthrightness, Social Openness, Enterprise,
Shows Respect, Perseverance, Flexibility, Cultural Perspectivism,
Venturesome, and Social Confidence. From these dimensions, the
Intercultural Competency Scale (ICS) was developed. It tests an
individual for the presence of these cross-culture-general traits. None
of traits are specific to a particular cultural situation. A
cognitively- measured trait approach, the ICS was employed in this study
as a measure of intercultural effectiveness.
A similar concept, also employed in this current study, is Cross
Cultural Social Intelligence (CCSI). Combining the social intelligence
and cross cultural communication literature, Ascalon, Schleicher, and
Born (2008) developed a comprehensive situational judgment test. While
encompassing emotional intelligence, social intelligence is defined as
the "ability to understand the feelings, thoughts, and behavior of
persons, including oneself, in interpersonal situations and to act
appropriately upon that understanding" (Marlow, 1986). Extending
that concept, socially intelligent people can adapt their behavior in a
wide array of social situations (Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987). Because
social intelligence is specific to a particular culture, it may not be
able to explain interpersonal effectiveness across cultures. Therefore,
empathy and ethnocentrism are assumed as the basis for judgment of
social intelligence in cross-cultural interactions. Three abilities are
measured in the CCSI test: 1) recognition and understanding of
(non)verbal cues of people from multiple cultures; 2) ability to
accurately infer social references in multiple cultural encounters; and
3) by accepting and understanding multiple cultures, achieve relevant
social objectives across cultural negotiations. CCSI is an example of a
systematic, interdisciplinary measurement for integrating and examining
cognitive and behavioral dimensions of cross-cultural effectiveness.
However, literature is scant on studies that have explored the
relationship between cognition/perception and the ability to accurately
read cultural situations or take action successfully in cross cultural
interactions. Therefore, this research focuses on identifying
correlations between two testing instruments: ICS and CCSI. Based on
this exploratory study, we measure respondents' intercultural
competency traits through the ICS (a cognitive and perceptual measure)
and also determine the extent to which possession of these traits can
predict the presence of cross-cultural social intelligence (CCSI, a
behavioral assessment). This research is intended to support future
research on the relationship between intercultural effectiveness,
cross-cultural background and success in adjusting to intercultural
living situations.
METHODOLOGY
This research focuses on correlating two instruments that address
intercultural effectiveness: the Intercultural Competency Scale (Elmer,
1986) and the Cross Cultural Social Intelligence test (Ascalon,
Schleicher, & Born, 2008). The Intercultural Competency Scale was
designed to evaluate the presence of cross-cultural abilities to
interact with people from other cultures by testing individuals on
twelve variables previously mentioned. The second study, developed by
Ascalon, Schleicher, and Born (2008), measures Cross-Cultural Social
Intelligence (CCSI Assessment) through a set of scenarios for which
participants are asked to select the most appropriate interaction
between two people in various cross-cultural situations.
Email requests were sent to approximately 100 working professionals
to undertake both the ICS and CCSI surveys, both of which were made
available on a website. To ensure data integrity for the correlation
study, it was important to track and match completion of both surveys by
individual respondents. After ignoring respondents who completed only
one of the ICS or CCSI surveys, we found that 52 respondents undertook
both of the surveys. Since seven out of these 52 respondents had missing
data, the pool of respondents who filled out both the surveys completely
was reduced to 45.
For the ICS test, each of the twelve factors, determined by a total
of 45 items that are scored using a five-point Likert scale, is rated
high, medium, and low. With 225 possible points, the mean score is 158
points, as calculated in the original research by Elmer (1986). These
high, medium and low divisions are set from a range of scores. For
example, a score of 200 is a high score and 120 is a low score.
One's level is determined from the mean of the individual's
test results. A total score is also assigned that explains one's
overall capacity to build strong abiding relationships with host country
people in another culture and to perform one's task in keeping with
their organizational goals. A feedback page is provided to each
participant that colors each of the twelve categories for easy
recognition. It also provides the mean score obtained for each category.
Then participants, by scrolling over that number, can have a more
detailed explanation of what the score means and what a person can do to
improve upon it (Elmer, 1986). Therefore, ICS measures personal
characteristics that contribute to intercultural effectiveness. It is a
cognitive scale which relies on self-reported preferences for engaging
in general interpersonal situations, all of which have been determined
to be indicators of intercultural effectiveness (Elmer, 1986).
By contrast, CCSI measures one's ability to correctly
interpret and then select the best interaction scenario in situations
with people from different cultures. For each question on the CCSI, four
answers are given that must be rated by the participant on
effectiveness. The respondent also indicates whether he/she would use
the response. The responses offered are:
Empathetic-Ethnocentric=EE,Empathetic-Non-ethnocentric=EN,Non-empathetic- Ethnocentric = NE, and Non-empathetic-Non-ethnocentric=NN. For each of
the questions asked, each answer is assigned a CCSI style, is given a
rating of 0-5 on effectiveness, and is rated as a best or worst answer.
Each of these ratings is developed from the evaluation of cultural
experts. Unfortunately, many of the offered answers are considered
inconclusive on the CCSI style and best/worst categories, making
analysis difficult. Feedback for this survey is not immediately given
back to participants, although the data on CCSI style, best/worst
scoring, and effectiveness for each response can be provided to the
participant after completion if requested.
As suggested by Ascalon et al. (2009), we scored the CCSI
instrument based on each respondent's likelihood of performing the
best and worst alternatives. The scores can range from 1 (not at all
likely) to 5 (extremely likely) for each item and scale. First,
respondents' likelihood ratings for each of the worst alternatives
for each scenario were reverse scored. Then item scores were computed by
averaging respondents' likelihood ratings on the best and worst
alternatives. The total scale score was calculated by averaging across
all the items. Higher the CCSI score on the total scale, greater would
be the cross cultural social intelligence. Higher CCSI scores would be
related to empathy, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and
emotional stability.
RESULTS
As indicated in Figure 1 below, the majority of respondents
(90.383) were U.S. Citizens. The remaining respondents were from
Australia, Europe, Asia, Central America, and the Middle East. All of
the respondents were fluent in English, possessed or were completing a
bachelor's or master's degree, and were currently working or
had worked in either full or part-time professional positions. Virtually
none were traditional students without professional work experience.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
From all participants, factors of each test will be based on
background information. The analysis of the data was performed using
Minitab software. The current focus is to determine whether those with a
high level of overall intercultural competency are able to produce a
high overall score in actual implementation of that knowledge and their
traits.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Figures 2 and 3 show the distribution of CCSI and ICS scores for
the respondents. According to Figure 2, only 6% respondents scored low
(< 3) whereas about 60% respondents scored high (> 4) on CCSI
scale, thus implying possession of higher levels of cross cultural
social intelligence by majority of the respondents in this sample.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Figure 3 shows that ICS scores for the same respondents ranged from
120 to 200. The majority (60%) of respondents scored between 150 and 180
on ICS scale. Furthermore, only 6% respondents scored very high (above
180) while 20% respondents scored low between 120 and 140 on ICS scale.
It is interesting to note that none of the respondents scored very low
(< 120) on the ICS scale.
To determine if respondents who scored higher on CCSI scale also
scored higher on ICS scale, we next performed the correlation analysis
between CCSI and ICS scores of all respondents using Minitab statistical
software. The Pearson correlation (r) was found to be statistically
significant at 0.05 level with a value of 0.36. It implies a positive
relationship of moderate strength between ICS and CCSI scores. Since the
ICS score is the cumulative score on 12 intercultural competency
factors, we decided to examine the relationship of each specific factor
with the CCSI score.
Table 2 shows Pearson correlation coefficients between all twelve
intercultural competency factors and the CCSI scores. The results showed
that four ICS factors Approachable, Perseverance, Cultural
Perspectivism, and Venturesome - were significantly correlated with CCSI
scores. Cultural Perspectivism and Social Confidence were found to
exhibit strongest (r=0.4) and weakest (0.1) correlation with CCSI,
respectively. Enterprise was the only factor to be negatively correlated
with CCSI but without any statistical significance. It is interesting to
note the that intuitively obvious factors such as Social Openness, Shows
Respect, and Flexibility showed no significant relationship with CCSI.
To test the stream of research which propounds the effect of
cognitive intercultural traits on one's cross cultural social
performance, we decided to identify which ICS factors, if any, can be
useful and reliable predictors of CCSI scores. A regression model was
developed next using CCSI as the response variable and all twelve ICS
factors as the predictor variables with the objective to keep the model
both parsimonious and statistically useful.
Table 3 shows the stepwise regression model approach used to
identify the most useful predictor variables. The 4-ICS factors found to
be the most useful in predicting the CCSI score are Cultural
Perspectivism, Approachable, Venturesome, and Enterprise. Table 4 shows
the best regression model based on highest [R.sup.2] (35.16%) and
smallest Mallows [C.sub.p] value (0.8) which can be stated as: E(CCSI) =
2.302 + 0.116 (Cultural Perspectivism) + 0.05 (Approachable) + 0.069
(Venturesome) - 0.058 (Enterprise)
According to Table 4, this regression model is globally useful with
an F value = 5.42 and a corresponding p-value =0.001. The partial tests
of usefulness for individual ICS factors are all significant at 0.102
level.
Figures 4 and 5 show the residual analysis and normality plot for
the above regression model. It is clear that there are no major
violations of the underlying assumptions of normality, independence, and
equal variance. Hence, the above regression model can be used for
predicting CCSI scores based on 4 cognitive intercultural predictor
variables.
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
DISCUSSION
Beginning with the overall scores for both tests, why is the
distribution of ICS (150-180, mean = 158) and CCSI, (> 4), higher for
the majority of respondents in this sample? One possibility is that the
education and experience level of the subjects for this study was
greater than scores would be for a control group randomly selected from
the general population. All subjects in the study had professional
industry experience and most were already completing graduate studies
while working full-time. In short, education and professional-level
work experience may be correlated with intercultural competence and
cross-cultural social intelligence. This explanation will be tested in
the next study as the characteristics of the population are correlated
with their CCSI and ICS scores. We should note here that the CCSI is a
new instrument that has not been tested extensively. It may require more
testing with larger samples to develop norms for low, medium and high
scores. This may explain why the CCSI scores did not follow a normal
distribution.
As indicated in Table 2, why are seemingly and intuitively obvious
cognitive factors such as Intercultural Receptivity, Positive
Orientation, Social Openness, Shows Respect, Flexibility, and Social
Confidence not significantly correlated with CCSI? One possibility is
that some cognitions do not necessarily translate into behavior.
Secondly, effects may already be included in the four cognitive factors
that have significant correlation with CCSI. Third, ICS may be measuring
factors that simply were not taken into account by CCSI. Again, CCSI
measures are based on behaviors related to ethnocentrism and empathy.
ICS is measuring traits. For example, Intercultural Receptivity is
defined as being "interested in people, especially people from
other cultures" (Elmer, 1986). This concept appears, on the
surface, to be the opposite of ethnocentrism. However, people who do not
practice ethnocentrism (cognitively or behaviorally) still may not have
an active interest in people from other cultures. In short, absence of
malice toward others who are different than oneself does not necessarily
constitute interest in others. Likewise, neither the absence of
ethnocentrism nor the presence of empathy would necessarily correlate
with social openness (the inclination to interact with people regardless
of their differences), flexibility (open to culture learning), or social
confidence (tends to be self-assured). People who are empathetic are
understanding, aware of, sensitive to, and vicariously experience the
feelings thoughts and experiences of others (Websters, 2003). Therefore,
empathy may readily and reasonably correlate with Cultural
Perspectivism, the capacity to imaginatively enter into another cultural
viewpoint (Elmer, 1986). We may conclude then that certain factors as
defined by both Elmer (1986) and Ascalon et al (2008) are correlated and
useful for research purposes. Other factors such as Social Confidence
are operationally-defined in such a way that there is no correlation.
Therefore, they are not useful in predicting behavior from cognition.
For future research, we can conclude from both the correlation and
regression analysis of ICS and CCSI factors, that Approachable, Cultural
Perspectivism, and Venturesome are useful ICS factors in predicting
behaviors measured by the CCSI.
FUTURE RESEARCH
This empirical study is the first in a series to test Strubler
& Park's (2009) revision of Black et al's (1991) Framework
for International Adjustment. The purpose of the revision was two-fold:
1) to make the model more prescriptive so that organizations could
improve their expatriate success rate and 2) to test hypotheses that
would close multiple gaps in the literature. The model combines the
concepts of intercultural effectiveness and adjustment. In this present
study, the goal was to test and correlate two instruments that measure
intercultural effectiveness. Having reliable instruments that test both
the cognitive and behavioral aspects of effectiveness are necessary to
test hypotheses regarding both effectiveness and adjustment. The next
empirical study will determine the extent to which cross-cultural
experience, i.e., language and culture education, training, family
background, travel, and work experience, is correlated with
intercultural effectiveness. Later studies will focus on the
correlations between extended cross-cultural experience and success in
international adjustment.
REFERENCES
Ascalon, Ma. E., D.J. Schleicher, & M.Ph. Born (2008).
Cross-cultural social intelligence: An assessment for employees working
in cross-cultural contexts. Cross Cultural Management, 15(2), 109-130.
Black, J.S. (1988). Work role transitions: A study of American
expatriate managers in Japan. Journal of International Business Studies,
19(2), 277-294.
Black, J.S. & M. Mendenhall (1990a). Cross-cultural training
effectiveness: A review and theoretical framework for future research.
Academy of Management Review, 15(1), 113-136.
Black, J.S. & M. Mendenhall (1991b). The u-curve adjustment
hypothesis revisited: A review and theoretical framework. Journal of
International Business Studies, 22(2), 225-247.
Black, J.S., M. Mendenhall & G. Oddou (1991). Toward a
Comprehensive Model of International Adjustment: An Integration of
Multiple Theoretical Perspectives. Academy of Management Review, 16(2),
292-317.
Elmer, M. (1986). Intercultural Effectiveness: Development of an
Intercultural Competency Scale. Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State
University, East Lansing, Michigan.
Fisher, G. B. & C.E.J. Hartel (2003). Cross-cultural
effectiveness of western Expatriate-Thai client interactions: Lessons
learned for IHRM research and theory. Cross Cultural Management, 10(4),
4-28.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures Consequences: International
Differences in Work-related Values. Sage Publications: Beverly Hills,
CA.
Marlow, H.A. (1986). Social intelligence: Evidence for
multidimensionality and Construct independence. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 78(1), 52-58.
Mumby, D.K. & L.L. Putnam (1992). The politics of emotion: A
feminist reading of bounded rationality. Academy of Management Review,
17(3), 465-486.
Ollilainen, M. (2000). Gendering emotions: gendering terms:
Construction of emotions in self-managing teamwork. In N.M. Ashkanasy,
Hartel, C.E.J., & Zerbe, W.J. (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace:
Research, theory, and practice: 82-96, Quorum Books: London.
Salovey, P. & J. Mayer (1990). Emotional intelligence.
Imagination, Cognition And Personality, 9 (185-211). Strubler, D.C.
& S. Park (2009). Framework for International Adjustment: Refining
and Extending a Model.
Proceedings of the North American Management Society, Midwest
Business Administration Association Annual Conference.
Tan, J.A.C., C.E.J. Hartel, D. Panipucci, & V.E. Strybosch
(2005). The effect of emotions in cross-cultural expatriate differences.
Cross Cultural Management, 12(2), 4-15.
David Strubler, Kettering University
Atul Agarwal, University of Illinois at Springfield
Sung-Hee Park, Kettering University
Muriel Elmer, Independent Consultant
Table 2: Correlations between CCSI and ICS Factors
ICS Factors Correlation (r)
1. Approachable 0.30 *
2. Intercultural Receptivity 0.24
3. Positive Orientation 0.15
4. Forthrightness 0.17
5. Social Openness 0.24
6. Enterprise -0.05
7. Shows Respect 0.27
8. Perseverance 0.34 *
9. Flexibility 0.28
10. Cultural Perspectivism 0.40 **
11. Venturesome 0.35 *
12. Social Confidence 0.01
* Significant at "=0.05
** Significant at "=0.01
Table 3: Stepwise Regression: CCSI Scores versus 12-ICS Factors
Alpha-to-Enter: 0.15 Alpha-to-Remove: 0.15
Step 1 2 3 4
Constant 2.947 2.042 1.885 2.302
Cultural Perspectivism 0.109 0.113 0.106 0.116
T-Value 2.90 3.16 2.99 3.29
P-value 0.006 0.003 0.005 0.002
Approachable 0.058 0.043 0.050
T-Value 2.43 1.66 1.97
P-value 0.020 0.105 0.056
Venturesome 0.064 0.069
T-Value 1.53 1.69
P-value 0.133 0.099
Enterprise -0.058
T-Value -1.68
P-value 0.101
S 0.491 0.0466 0.458 0.449
R-sq 16.32 26.61 30.60 35.16
R-sq (adj.) 14.37 23.12 25.52 28.67
Mallows Cp 5.2 1.5 1.3 0.8
Table 4: Regression Analysis--CCSI Scores versus significant ICS
Factors
The regression equation is:
CCSI = 2.30 + 0.116 Cultural Perspectivism + 0.0504 Approachable
(0.002) * (0.056) *
+ 0.0690 Venturesome - 0.0581 Enterprise
(0.099) * (0.101) *
* Numbers in parentheses represent the P-value
Analysis of Variance
Source DF SS MS F P
Regression 4 4.3629 1.0907 5.42 0.001
Residual Error 40 8.0467 0.2012
Total 44 12.4096
S = 0.448516 R-Sq = 35.2% R-Sq(adj) = 28.7%