Comparing interpretive methods targeting invasive species management at Cumberland Island National Seashore.
Sharp, Ryan L. ; Larson, Lincoln R. ; Green, Gary T. 等
Introduction
Public land management agencies are responsible increasingly for
incorporating public input into decision-making processes. Although
legislation such as the National Environmental Policy Act (NEPA)
mandates public consultation on important issues, land managers often
struggle to reconcile discrepancies in public perceptions and desires
with sound scientific management principles (Christensen et al., 1996;
Cortner & Moote, 1999). Effective education programs may help to
resolve this dissonance. Research suggests that interpretive programs
designed to increase public understanding and appreciation of important
environmental issues can provide critical support for management actions
in protected areas (Bright, Fishbein, Manfredo, & Bath, 1993; Marion
& Rogers, 1994; Powell & Ham, 2008). Furthermore, general
approaches to conservation--including those centered on education and
interpretation--rarely incorporate an evaluation component (Ferraro
& Pattanayak, 2006). More research is needed to help public land
managers identify optimal, cost-effective strategies for educating the
public about critical land management issues, minimizing human impacts,
and influencing stewardship actions (Coble et al., 2005; Hughes et al.,
2009).
Evaluating Interpretive Approaches
Although education can occur in many places (e.g., websites, news
outlets, schools), studies suggest that on-site interpretive programs
often have the greatest impact on participants' knowledge,
awareness, and attitudes (Henker & Brown, 2011). Typically,
protected area managers will employ two methods of reaching visitors:
interpersonal and non-personal communication (Marion & Reid, 2007).
Interpersonal communication (e.g., interpretive programs) consists of
face-to-face interactions with visitors that allow for two-way
communication. This method, often preferred because it facilitates
personal contact with visitors (Knudson, Cable, & Beck, 2003), is
difficult to consistently implement due to the high cost of training
interpreters and staffing interpretive sites (Munro, Morrison-Saunders,
& Hughes, 2008). Non-personal communication (e.g., flyers,
brochures) enables interpreters to reach a broader audience, but this
approach is relatively inflexible and cannot be adapted easily to meet
the needs of individual visitors. Because non-personal communication
generally requires visitors to create their own meaning from the
material presented, park managers often see it as a less desirable form
of education (Hughes, 2004). However, because of its comparatively low
cost, non-personal forms of interpretation typically dominates
visitors' experiences (Knudson, Cable, & Beck, 2003).
Although the advantages and disadvantages of various interpretive
methods are well established (e.g., Ham, 1992), evaluating the effects
of these interpretive approaches on visitors has proven to be difficult.
Some authors have argued that the intangible, abstract, and emotional
connections inspired by interpretation cannot be captured by traditional
evaluation approaches (Dustin & McAvoy, 1985). Other authors have
lamented that evaluation efforts rarely probe visitor understanding of
key messages and the influence of these messages on visitor beliefs
(Beckmann, 1999; Hughes et al., 2009). Studies that have demonstrated
quantifiable, concrete, interpretation-mediated changes in visitor
knowledge, attitudes, and behavior have focused on the complete
experience and not specific delivery methods (e.g., Powell & Ham,
2008; Zeppel & Muloin, 2008). Unfortunately, little research has
investigated the educational and transformative value of specific
interpretive methods and media on visitors (Henker & Brown, 2011).
Authors have also acknowledged that interpretive outcome assessment is
absolutely necessary for managers who want to develop programmatic
improvements and refine their current understanding of interpretive best
practices (Coble et al., 2005; Silverman & Barrie, 2000).
In general, effective interpretation involves messages that are
enjoyable or entertaining; relevant to the target audience, organized in
a clear and concise manner, and thematic in way that promotes factual
understanding, intellectual growth, and emotional connections (Ham,
1992; Ham & Weiler, 2002). Once these conditions are satisfied, both
interpersonal and non-personal forms of communication can be successful.
In fact, research suggests that the interpretive delivery method may not
be as important as the content and context of the message and the
general nature of the experience (Wiles & Hall, 2005; Winder &
Roggenbuck, 2000). For example, the influence of flyers and brochures on
visitor knowledge and attitudes depends on how they are distributed.
Flyers obtained independently by visitors at a brochure stand have a
reduced impact compared to flyers personally delivered by staff member
at the park or protected area (Moscardo, 1999; Oliver, Roggenbuck, &
Watson, 1985). However, other research has shown that certain
interpretive approaches--notably ranger-led programs--are far more
effective than others at helping visitors develop meaning connections
with resources (Coble et al., 2005; Henker & Brown, 2011).
If all factors are equal, what type of interpretive message is more
powerful? Do the relative benefits of interpersonal communication
justify the costs, or are cheaper non-personal approaches equally
effective at influencing visitor knowledge, attitudes, and behavior
toward environmental issues? To address these questions, this study
evaluated the differential effects of two interpretation methods (a
non-personal, visual-based flyer and an interpersonal, audio-based talk)
on the cognitive and affective components of visitor's experience
within a specific context. In particular, this study focused on the
escalating issue of invasive species management at Cumberland Island
National Seashore, Georgia.
Case Study Context: Public Perceptions of Invasive Species
Management
Invasive species are an issue facing ecosystems throughout the
world and are considered to be a major threat to biodiversity across the
planet (Bremner & Park, 2007; Gurevitch & Padilla, 2004;
Lockwood, Hoopes, & Marchetti, 2008). As economic globalization
increases the movement of species via trade, transportation, travel, and
tourism, the invasive threat will continue to grow (McNeely, 1996;
Pimentel, Zuniga, & Morrison, 2005). Preserving native species
diversity and natural ecological processes has become a full-time
endeavor for the U.S. National Park Service (NPS) as the fight to ward
off invasive species intensifies (Houston & Schreiner, 1995;
Sellars, 1997). In fact, the NPS has acknowledged the important role
that public education campaigns can play in the fight against invasive
species (McNeely, 2001).
Cumberland Island National Seashore (CUIS), a unit managed by the
NPS, is a prime example of an area facing the invasive species
infestation issue. Cumberland Island has some very visible and
destructive invasive species, such as feral hogs and horses, introduced
by previous island inhabitants, that have caused a great deal of
ecological damage (Dilsaver, 2004). The feral horses, in particular,
represent a contentious and controversial issue for CUIS managers.
Although the horses negatively impact the sensitive barrier island
ecosystem, many visitors come to CUIS with the specific goal of viewing
horses (Kirkpatrick, 1995). This situation creates a challenging
management conundrum. In addition to the feral horses and hogs, a new
invasive fungus from Asia is killing red bay and sassafras trees at an
alarming rate with no containment method currently available (Fraedrich
et al., 2008). Furthermore, invasive plant species such as bamboo and
the tung oil tree are altering permanently the coastal landscape. The
CUIS managers have recognized the negative economic and environmental
impacts of invasive species and have stated that invasive feral animals
and exotic plants will be managed "to the point that their threat
is negligible" (NPS, 1984). However, this aggressive management
strategy towards invasive species can only succeed with public support
(Bremner & Park, 2007). If managers do not incorporate
visitors' perspectives into the management planning process, they
run the risk of losing public support for invasive species policies and
control measures (Nimmo & Miller, 2007).
The CUIS context therefore presents an ideal situation for
examining public response to various forms of interpretation targeting a
contentious environmental issue. Because CUIS is primarily accessibly by
ferry, the highly regulated access points create a controlled mechanism
for exposing visitors to interpretation treatments and evaluating their
response. Although the education outcomes examined in this study focused
on invasive species, results could help to refine and adapt similar
interpretation programs targeting other natural resource management
issues occurring across the country.
Outcome Variables
Knowledge & Awareness
Knowledge and understanding of ecological topics is required to
make informed decisions about protected area management. Visitors that
have a greater awareness of ecological issues are generally more likely
to support management decisions (Wiles & Hall, 2005). Educational
and interpretive programs are proven vehicles for enhancing visitor
awareness of salient issues (Powell & Ham, 2008; Zeppel &
Moulin, 2008). Background knowledge also mediates the connections
visitors develop with on-site information. For instance, Roggenbuck et
al. (1982) found that visitors with little prior knowledge upon arrival
at a protected area were more likely to have an increase in knowledge
following their visit. However, well-educated park visitors are often
more receptive to educational and interpretive messages (Hendee &
Dawson, 2002). Given these somewhat contradictory results, more
information is needed to understand the effects of on-site
interpretation on visitors' knowledge and awareness. Hence, this
study examined the effects of interpretation on visitors' general
knowledge of the invasive species issue and specific knowledge of
invasives found at CUIS.
Attitudes
In addition to increasing awareness, educational and interpretive
programs should aim to influence visitors' attitude and emotional
attachment to an area or a particular issue (Wiles & Hall, 2005).
These affective links can help visitors develop a stronger sense of
environmental stewardship (Madin & Fenton, 2004). Although Ham
(2007) suggested that many interpretive contacts are much too short to
have an effective impact on visitor's attitudes, other studies have
shown that object-specific attitudes are fluid and can be changed by
individual contact with effective interpretive messages (Kim, Airey,
& Szivas, 2011; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Individuals'
initial attitudes especially are susceptible to change if they become
attached to a stronger attitude or belief (Williams et al., 2002). These
patterns emerged from the work of Wiles and Hall (2005), who discovered
that individuals with less prior knowledge and weaker attitudes toward
an object were more likely to experience program-mediated change. This
discovery has important implications for public land management. Through
educational and interpretive programs, managers have an opportunity to
increase awareness, transform attitudes, and promote an enhanced land
ethic in hopes of garnering support for a proposed management action
(Marion & Reid, 2007). Hence, this study also examined the effects
of interpretation on visitors' attitudes toward native and
non-native species.
Management Preferences
Public land management occurs in an integrated social environment.
Managers make decisions that not only influence natural resources, but
also impact how visitors experience these resources. Public input into
the decision making process is therefore critical. However, rampant
discord among scientists, land managers, and the public often
complicates this process. While scientists and managers usually act
based on scientific evidence, the public often responds to issues based
on emotional attachments (Rikoon, 2006). Consequently, interpretive
programs must acknowledge and respond to these emotional attachments and
environmental value orientations (McFarlane & Boxall, 2000). By
shaping beliefs, attitudes, and social norms, educational and
interpretive programs can alter stakeholders' management
preferences and potential acceptance of management policies (Absher et
al., 2008). Additional research would help to clarify the effects of
different interpretive approaches on these management-oriented
perspectives. Hence this study also examined the effects of
interpretation on visitors' support for various invasive species
management options.
Statement of Purpose
The purpose of this research was to assess the effects of two
interpretive programs (a flyer and a ranger-led talk) on visitors'
knowledge of, attitudes toward, and support for invasive species
management at CUIS relative to a control group. By comparing mean scores
of these target outcome variables for visitors exposed to different
interpretive treatments, this study hoped to identify optimal strategies
for educating visitors and provide a foundation for informed
participatory decision-making processes regarding invasive species
management on public lands.
Methods
Visitor Education Programs
The two interpretive programs examined in this study featured
identical content delivered in unique ways. The visual interpretive
method (hereafter "flyer") consisted of a two-sided, 8.5-inch
by 11-inch black-and-white, illustrated, tri-fold flyer that was
distributed immediately to visitors as they exited the dock and arrived
on the island (Appendix A). The reading level and general comprehension
of the flyer was tested with several audiences (e.g., university
students, national park visitors) prior to implementation. The audio
interpretive method consisted of a brief five- to seven-minute ranger
talk (hereafter "talk") that was conducted at the unloading
zones immediately after island visitors left the ferry (Appendix B). To
maintain consistency, each iteration of the talk was conducted by the
same interpreter. Both the talk and the flyer contained the same theme:
a focus on the invasive species issue in a global, national, and local
context, including current challenges at CUIS. Both interpretive
approaches were also designed based on Ham's (1992) guidelines for
effective interpretive messages: enjoyable, relevant, organized, and
thematic. Visitors in the control group did not receive either form of
intentional interpretive treatment when they arrived at the island.
Data Collection
A quasi-experimental, static groups comparison approach was used to
evaluate the effects of these two distinct interpretive strategies on a
random sample of visitors to CUIS (N=1,093). Arriving visitors on
predetermined days from April to September of 2008 were randomly
assigned to one of three groups: visitors receiving an interpretive
flyer (n=363), visitors receiving an interpretive talk (n=320), and
visitors who did not receive any formal interpretive treatment focused
on invasive species management (n=410). At the conclusion of their
visit, every third CUIS visitor over age 18 was asked to complete a 10-
to 15-minute intercept survey while waiting at the ferry dock or on the
boat returning to the mainland. This administration approach allowed
ample time for survey completion (response rate = 93%). Data were
collected via a stratified random sampling procedure to help ensure
adequate coverage across days of the week, hours of the day, and dock
sites (two separate ferry docks) within the park.
The public perceptions survey instrument was based upon existing
literature (Bremmer & Park, 2007; Brooks et al., 1999) and targeted
cognitive (What do visitors know about invasive species?) and affective
outcomes (How do visitors feel about invasive species?) related to
dimensions of invasive species management. Questions were with
categorical, multiple-choice, open-ended, or Likert-type responses
guided by the tailored design method for survey construction (Dillman,
2007). Visitors' basic concept of the term "invasive
species" was measured with a single multiple-choice item. General
knowledge-related items (11 total) were measured on a scale from 1 =
"not at all aware or familiar" to 3 = "very aware or
familiar." Visitor awareness of specific invasive species on CUIS
(six total) were also measured on a three-point scale where 1 = "I
don't know what this is," 2 = "I know the name, but I
didn't know it was invasive," and 3 = "I know what this
is and I know it's an invasive species." Attitude-related
items (14 total) were measured on a scale from 1 = "strongly
disagree" to 5 = "strongly agree." Visitors'
management preference items (12 total) were assessed on a scale from 1 =
"unacceptable in all cases" to 5 = "acceptable in all
cases." The final instrument also included socio demographic
questions and information related to visitors' general outdoor
recreation preferences and experience use history at CUIS and other NPS
sites.
Data Analysis
Data were analyzed in several phases. Because pre-visit surveys
were not conducted, the random allocation of visitors to each of the
treatment groups was confirmed before program effects were examined.
Group characteristics were compared using frequencies and chi-square
differences. Next, survey responses were checked to ensure that visitors
who were exposed to a specific educational treatment were aware that
they had received that particular information--validating the
quasi-experimental group assignments. Finally, general effects of the
educational treatments were examined using a priori analysis of variance
(ANOVA) contrasts comparing visitors in both treatment groups (talk and
flyer) to visitors in the control group. Treatment effect size (r) was
also examined for the contrasts using the formula r = [square root of
[[t.sup.2] / ([t.sup.2] + df)]], where r = 0.5 was indicative of a large
treatment effect, r = 0.3 represented a medium effect, and r < 0.1
signified a small effect (Cohen, 1988). For variables where the effects
of the educational treatments were significant in the ANOVA, specific
effects of the talk and flyer were compared using post hoc pair-wise
tests with Bonferonni adjustments.
Limitations
Several limitations affect the inferences that can be made from
this research. First, the feasibility of implementation and park
restrictions prevented this study from using a within-subjects design.
Although this type of pre-post method--which controls for
respondents' initial levels of knowledge and attitudes--would have
been optimal (DiMauro & Dietz, 2001), it was assumed that the
randomly selected control group used in this quasi-experimental approach
represented the views and attitudes of visitors not exposed to the
interpretive treatments (i.e., the baseline levels).
Second, the population of visitors sampled at CUIS does not
necessarily represent the general public at large. Despite this
limitation, however, general demographic information suggested that the
CUIS sample was similar to other samples in studies involving NPS
visitors. The CUIS visitors were fairly homogenous (typically highly
educated, high income, and white), and interpretation methods focused on
other areas, audiences, or contentious issues might yield different
results. In fact, Ballantyne et al. (1998) recommended that interpretive
messages and media should be targeted specifically to meet the needs of
unique groups. Interpretation that works in one situation may not be as
effective in other contexts.
Third, the interpretive messages used in this project focused on an
exceptionally controversial issue. Many of the survey questions focused
on the island's feral horse population, and feral horses are a
sensitive topic at CUIS (Sharp et al., 2011). Hence, the cognitive
dissonance that many visitors experience can be problematic. While many
visitors come to CUIS to see the feral horses, they also recognize the
destructive potential of the Island's unique, non-native
attraction. Hence, some survey responses may have been skewed as
visitors struggled to simultaneously respond to the scientific
information presented to them in relation to their personal beliefs and
attitudes. However, this complex situation highlights the need for
effective interpretive programs and assessments that evaluate their
efficacy.
Results
Defining the Treatment Groups
To compare the effects of the two interpretive treatments relative
to the control group, this study had to first confirm that the group
assignments were unbiased. Data showed that, with the possible exception
of length of stay, groups essentially were homogenous in terms of
demographics and experience use history (Table 1). Therefore, it was
assumed that a large portion of any observed differences in visitor
knowledge of, attitudes toward, and preference for invasive species
management would be due to the various interpretive treatments.
Overall, the CUIS visitors consisted of an equal mix of males and
females. A vast majority of the visitors were white. The mean age of
visitors was approximately 40 years old, and over half of the
participants reported education levels that included an advanced degree.
This elevated educational level is higher than the general U.S.
population but consistent with national park visitors in general (NPS,
2002). A majority of CUIS visitors had been to the island before, and
most had visited at least one other NPS site in the past five years.
About one-half of all visitors said they had been exposed to some type
of information about invasive species in the media during the past year.
Validating Treatment Group Assignments
Although limited access to CUIS helped to ensure that all visitors
entering the park on a particular day were exposed to one of the
designated treatments, this assumption needed to be validated. To
confirm the group assignments on particular days, a survey item was
checked that asked participants, "During your visit to CUIS, from
which of the following sources did you receive information regarding
invasive species?" In general, results suggested that the perceived
group assignments accurately reflected most visitors' exposure to
interpretive programs (Table 2). However, not all visitors in the flyer
and talk treatment groups acknowledged that they had received
educational information or materials in that particular format.
Effects of Interpretive Treatments
Knowledge of Invasive Species.
When asked to choose the best description of an invasive species,
41.3% of visitors selected the correct response, "species foreign
to CUIS that out-compete native species." Visitors exposed to the
talk (50.3%) and flyer (40.5%) interventions chose the correct response
more often than visitors in the control group (39.8%), [chi
square](df=8) = 19.1, p = 0.014, however the difference between the
flyer and the control group was minimal. About twice as many visitors in
the control group (7.6%) were not sure how to describe an invasive
species, compared to only 3.9% of visitors in the treatment groups.
Visitors who experienced the interpretive talk scored higher than
visitors who received the flyer on all of these knowledge items, and
significantly higher than visitors in the control group (Figure 1). Mean
scores for general knowledge items ranged from 1.73 to 2.34, indicating
that most visitors were only slightly aware of invasive species and
their impacts. Visitors' general knowledge of invasive species was
slightly affected by the interpretive treatments when compared to the
control group. For example, the interpretive interventions appeared to
significantly positively affect a visitors' familiarity with why
species are considered invasive [t(1076) = 2.69, p = 0.007, r = 0.08],
why the removal of invasive species is important to the local
ecosystems, t(1073) = 2.43, p = 0.015, r = 0.07, effects of invasive
species on native plants and animals, t(1068) = 2.39, p = 0.017, r =
0.07, and awareness of invasive species' contribution to the
listing of threatened and endangered species, t(1077) = 2.21, p = 0.027,
r = 0.07. However, visitors exposed to the interpretive programs were
not any more aware of the NPS's mission in regards to controlling
invasive species than visitors in the control group [mean score = 1.73,
t(1071) = 0.86, p = 0.390, r = 0.03].
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
The interpretive treatments also appeared to affect visitors'
knowledge of specific invasive species. Although knowledge of less
conspicuous invasive species such as ambrosia beetles, tung oil trees,
and privet was not affected by the treatments, visitors exposed to
either the talk or the flyer indicated an elevated awareness of feral
horses, t(1063) = 2.30, p = 0.022, r = 0.07, feral hogs, t(1060) = 5.57,
p < 0.001, r = 0.17, and bamboo, t(1054) = 2.00, p = 0.046, r = 0.06,
relative to the control group. Both treatments appeared to affect
visitor knowledge of these species to an approximately equal degree
(Figure 2). It should be noted that, although many different invasive
species were described in the text, feral horses, feral hogs, and bamboo
were the only invasive species pictured on the flyer.
Attitudes Toward Invasive Species.
An exploratory factor analysis (principal axis factoring with
oblique rotation) was used to reduce the attitude items on the survey
into two primary constructs (see Sharp et al., 2011 for factor loading
results): absolute ecocentric items (three total) reflected a belief
that all species--even invasives--have a right to persist and should not
be controlled by humans; adaptive ecocentric items (eight total)
reflected a belief that some degree of human intervention is necessary
to manage invasive species to protect native ecosystem integrity.
Contrasts showed that the interpretive treatments did not have a
significant effect on visitors attitudes on either the absolute
ecocentric, t(1073) = 0.53, p = 0.598, r = 0.02, or adaptive ecocentric,
t(1075) = -0.13, p = 0.900, r = 0.00, subscales. In fact, even when
examining individual items, significant differences between the
treatment and control groups were not evident, t(1059) > 1.78, p >
0.075, r < 0.05.
Support for Invasive Species Management.
An exploratory factor analysis (principal axis factoring, oblique
rotation) was also used to reduce the management preference items on the
survey into three major constructs (see Sharp et al., 2011 for factor
loading results): leaving invasive species alone (one item), adaptive
on-site management of invasives (seven items), and complete eradication
of invasives (two items). Contrasts showed that, relative to the
control, the interpretive treatments did not have an effect on visitor
preferences for two of the management categories--leaving invasives
alone, t(1052) = 0.13, p = 0.900, or managing invasives onsite, t(1070)
= -0.47, p = 0.638. However, management preference scores for the
"eradicate invasives" items were significantly different
between the treatment and control groups, t(1057) = -2.33, p = 0.020, r
= 0.07. Contrary to expectations, visitors in the control group actually
displayed a greater likelihood of supporting complete eradication of
invasive species from Cumberland Island than visitors who were exposed
to either the talk or the flyer (Figure 3).
Visitors who listened to the talk were more likely to support any
type of active invasive species management than visitors who received
the flyer. In fact, visitors in the flyer treatment group only scored
higher than the visitors in the talk treatment group for the "leave
invasives alone" option. Mean differences (MD) between the talk and
flyer treatments (talk mean - flyer mean) were the most obvious in the
post hoc pair-wise comparison tests for the following items: sponsoring
invasive hunts (MD = 0.35, p < 0.001), managing non-natives
introduced by humans (MD = 0.18, p = 0.028), spending money to control
invasives (MD = 0.175, p = 0.018), and eliminating invasive animals (MD
= 0.303, p = 0.002). In all cases, visitors who listened to the talk
were significantly more likely to support interventions than visitors
who received the flyer. These item-specific differences between the talk
and flyer treatment groups were also reflected in the overall subscale
scores for on-site management (MD = 0.21, p = 0.002) and complete
eradication (MD = 0.22, p = 0.047) of invasive species.
Discussion
Interpretive Treatments vs. Control
Based on existing research (e.g., Roggenbuck et al., 1991), it was
hypothesized that visitors who received either interpretive treatment
would exhibit an increase in knowledge-based survey scores relative to
the control group. As predicted, visitors exposed to the interpretive
treatments scored slightly higher than visitors in the control on both
the general and specific knowledge scales. However, even after exposure
to interpretive messages, many visitors remained unsure of the invasive
species concept and unfamiliar with different types of invasive
species--a result consistent with previous findings (McNeely, 1996). The
most significant positive treatment effects were observed for the
CUIS-specific knowledge and awareness items. This result supports
previous research suggesting that interpretive messages are most
powerful when they are relevant for a particular audience (Powell &
Ham, 2008). The interpretive program's positive effect on public
knowledge and awareness especially is encouraging considering the
declining environmental literacy rates across the United States (Coyle,
2005; Louv, 2005). If the public is more aware of environmental issues
such as invasive species, perhaps they will become more involved in the
decision making process and develop and stronger sense of stewardship
toward public lands (Dettmann-Easler & Pease, 1999; Orams, 1997).
Contrary to predictions, the interpretive treatments did not appear
to have a significant effect on visitor attitudes. These results suggest
that short-term interventions such as flyers and brief talks may not be
adequate mechanisms for stimulating affective change, supporting earlier
research (DiMauro & Dietz, 2001; Ham, 2007). For example, surveys
revealed that about 30% of visitors who had been exposed to
interpretation (either the flyer or talk) did not realize it. For
greater effects on cognitive domains such as attitudes, a more active
form of message (e.g., discussion groups, repeated mailings, sustained
educational campaigns) may be necessary (Hughes et al., 2009; Marynowski
& Jacobson, 1999). Often, the frequency of the message may be more
important than the content of the message.
Significant differences between the treatment and control group
were observed for one management option: complete eradication of
invasive species. Surprisingly, visitors exposed to the interpretive
messages were less likely to support this option than visitors in the
control group. This could be attributed to the fact that although
visitors in the treatment groups were given information about
alternative means of invasive species population control, visitors in
the control group may have viewed complete removal as the only feasible
option. These responses demonstrate how various forms of interpretive
messages can help managers build a case for adaptive management.
However, the effect of these messages should be closely monitored so
unexpected consequences do not emerge.
Flyer vs. Talk
When comparing the efficacy of the two different interpretive
strategies, results showed that the interpretive talk had a stronger
effect on general visitor knowledge than the educational flyer. The
beneficial effects of both interpretive methods on specific knowledge of
invasive species at CUIS were approximately equal. The awareness scores
for three invasive species--feral horses, feral hogs, and
bamboo--especially were high. These species were all pictured on the
flyer, highlighting the importance of visual cues in non-personal
interpretive messages (Moscardo, 1999; Munro, Morrison-Saunders, &
Hughes, 2008).
Differences in attitude scores between the treatment groups were
not evident, but management preference scores revealed a recurring
pattern. Visitors who received the interpretive talk were significantly
more likely to support some type of adaptive on-site invasive species
management than visitors who received the flyer, supporting a link
between ranger-led (i.e., personal interpretation) programs and
stewardship intentions observed in previous studies (e.g., Henker &
Brown, 2011). The enhanced value of the interpretive talk approach could
be due to several factors. Arriving visitors assigned to the
"talk" group were required to listen to the ranger, whereas
visitors had no obligation to read through the entire flyer. (In fact,
several were directly discarded on the ground at the dock site.) This
discrepancy emphasizes the value of enhancing visitors' experience
through interpersonal communication and interaction with a somewhat
captive audience (Henker & Brown, 2011; Knapp & Benton, 2004).
The talk provided an opportunity to interact with visitors and check for
understanding, making certain the central theme of the interpretive
message was conveyed. Furthermore, the interpersonal talk helped to
situate visitors. A non-personal flyer allows visitors to build upon
pre-existing schema for beliefs and attitudes that may be inappropriate,
inaccurate, or out of context. For controversial issues where the NPS
adopts a particular stance, a talk may be a more appropriate
interpretive method.
On the other hand, the interpretive flyer approach conferred a
potential advantage that was reflected in the management preference
scores. The flyer's message included information in an accessible,
easy-to-read format that visitors could reference at any point during
their trip. As visitors congregated at the Island's docks awaiting
departure, they had ample time to review the flyer at their discretion.
Perhaps visitors who received the flyer and referred to it multiple
times during their visit developed a more positive emotional association
with the focal species (feral hogs and horses), which could explain
their decreased level of support for complete eradication. In any case,
provision of a temporal window for free-choice learning to occur is a
distinct advantage of the flyer approach (Falk, 2001). While the talk
may have seemed like an intrusive burden to many visitors, the flyer
represented a more subtle way to convey a similar message.
Unfortunately, the non-personal delivery format of the flyer also
enabled its messages to be misconstrued. Although personal methods such
as ranger programs occasionally lead to misinterpretation as well, these
errors can generally be controlled as speakers adjust and adapt to
audience cues to ensure delivery of accurate information. The different
effects of both the talk and flyer highlight a well known pitfall of
interpretation--messages that address non-captive audiences and rely on
voluntary participation are highly variable in their success (Hammitt,
1984).
Recommendations
Overall, visitor responses to the two interpretive strategies
yielded some hope with respect to the educational impact of interpretive
messages regarding public land management. The audio-based interpretive
talk appeared to have a stronger effect on the knowledge, awareness, and
management preferences of CUIS visitors than the visual-based
interpretive flyer. To increase the likelihood that information in the
visual treatment is consumed and absorbed, the flyer could be revised to
include less detailed information, more pictures, and more cohesive
theme highlighting the devastating impacts of conspicuous invasives at
CUIS. To improve the efficacy of the audio treatment, visitors could be
briefed on the
island's invasive species (and other important park information)
during their 45-minute ferry ride. This efficient use of time would
capitalize on an inquisitive, captive audience and reduce the
frustration and anxiety associated with a mandatory information session
upon arrival. Research has shown that a non-personal form of audio
communication--audio tours using podcasts and MP3 technology--could also
be an effective way to connect with visitors (Henker & Brown, 2011;
Novey & Hall, 2006). The development of podcasts available on the
park website or interactive visitor center kiosks would make information
available at the visitor's discretion. This approach could allow
for repeated exposure to interpretive messages as visitors digest
material through multiple views and/or listens, increasing their
transformative power.
An integrated interpretive approach may yield the best results.
Individuals learn and react to material in different ways, and responses
to interpretive media can vary across distinct populations (Ballantyne
et al., 1998; Coble et al., 2005; Roggenbuck & Berrier, 1982).
Factors such as race/ethnicity (Coble et al., 2005) and previous
experience at a site (Hughes et al., 2009) often influence outcomes of
interpretive messages. Therefore, a combination of multiple interpretive
media and educational messages are likely needed to help managers
communicate with diverse visitors. Finally, managers should remember
that learning about conservation issues if often a secondary motivation
for park visitors (Absher & Graefe, 1997). Interpretive messages
should therefore be conveyed in a concise and captivated fashion. Public
land management involving controversial problems such as invasive
species is inexorably guided by public awareness of and attitudes toward
important environmental issues. Interpretive programs and educational
campaigns could play a major role in that process, ultimately producing
more informed collaborative approaches to public land management
decisions.
Appendix A--Invasive Species Audio Group Flyer
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Appendix B--Invasive Species Talk Group Script
* Good (morning, afternoon), my name is Ryan and I would like to
take the next three to five minutes to tell you about invasive species.
* Can anyone tell me what an invasive species is? Wait for audience
response, gather two to three.
* Great. Invasive species can mean many things to many people, but
let's start by defining what an exotic species is:
** An exotic species is one (plant or animal) that is not native to
a particular ecosystem, not native being from a different part of the
country or from a different country entirely.
** Many of America's staple crops are exotic (or non-native)
such as corn, wheat and apples, however, these are not invasive species,
simply non-native.
** A species becomes invasive when it begins to outcome native
species, replacing them in that particular ecosystem.
** Can anyone name an invasive species they have heard of? Wait for
two to three responses.
* The National Park Service defines invasive species as:
** Species that grow and spread rapidly and are free from the
natural controls present in their natural lands, such as herbivores,
parasites and diseases.
* So why is this important? Invasive species can have a negative
impact on the environment in the form of trampling vegetation,
out-competing natives species and eliminating future generations by
destruction of nest and interrupting the food chain
** Invasives are responsible:
*** For $20 billion per year in economic damage
*** 42% of all species on the endangered species list
*** Covering 2.6 million acres of NPS lands
** The NPS ranks invasive species in front of air pollution,
off-road vehicle usage, and visitation pressures as the main threat to
the ecosystems of many of its parklands.
** 1,000 species of invasive plants and 500 non native animals.
* Cumberland Island has 66 documented invasive plants such as tung
oil tree, privet, and bamboo.
* Cumberland Island has an invasive beetle that carries a fungus
that infects red bay trees on the island.
* The horses and pigs were introduced to the island in the 1600s.
However the current populations are descended from livestock released on
the island in the early 1900s.
** Pigs have been known to eat sea turtle and sea bird eggs on the
beach.
** Horses eat the sea oats which are vital for dune stabilization;
over forage in the salt marshes reducing critical habitat.
* So what can be done to control invasive species?
** Invasive plant species may be controlled through such methods as
the use of herbicide and pulling.
** Invasive animal species may be controlled through contraceptive
measures, adoption programs, and managed hunts, among other methods.
* So what can you do to help prevent the spread of invasive
species?
** Find out about plants native to your region.
** Check that the plants you are buying for your yard or garden are
not invasive.
** When boating, clean your boat thoroughly before transporting it
to a different body of water.
** Clean your boots before you hike in a new area to get rid of
"hitchhiking" weed seeds and pathogens.
** Don't release aquarium fish and plants, live bait, or other
exotic animals into the wild.
** If you plan to own an exotic pet, do your research and plan
ahead to make sure you can commit to looking after it.
** Volunteer at your local park, refuge, or wildlife area to help
remove invasive species.
** Help educate others about the threat.
References
Absher, J. & Graefe, A. (1997). Trip motives of interpretive
program attendees and nonattendees. Journal of Interpretation Research,
2(1), 55-56.
Absher, J. D., Vaske, J. J., & Bright, A. D. (2008). Basic
beliefs, attitudes, and social norms regarding wildland fire management
in southern California. In D. J. Chavez, J. D. Absher & P. L. Winter
(Eds.), Fire Social Science Research from the Pacific Southwest Research
Station: Studies Supported by National Fire Plan Funds. Albany, CA:
United States Forest Service.
Ballantyne, R., Packer, J., & Beckmann, E. (1998). Targeted
interpretation: Exploring relationships among visitors'
motivations, activities, attitudes, information needs and preferences.
Journal of Tourism Studies, 9(2), 14-25.
Beckmann, E. A. (1999). Evaluating visitors' reactions to
interpretation in Australian national parks. Journal of Interpretation
Research, 4(1), 27-30.
Bremner, A., & Park, K. (2007). Public attitudes to the
management of invasive nonnative species in Scotland. Biological
Conservation, 139(3-4), 306-314.
Bright, A.D., Fishbein, M., Manfredo, M.J., and Bath, A. (1993).
Application of the theory of reasoned action to the National Park
Service's controlled burn policy. Journal of Leisure Research,
25(3), 263-280.
Brooks, J. J., Warren, R. J., Nelms, M. G., & Tarrant, M. A.
(1999). Visitor Attitudes toward and Knowledge of Restored Bobcats on
Cumberland Island National Seashore, Georgia. Wildlife Society Bulletin,
27(4), 1089-1097.
Christensen, N., Bartuska, A., Brown, J., Carpenter, S.,
D'Antonio, C., Francis, R., Franklin, J., MacMahon, J., Noss, R.,
Parsons, D., Peterson, C., Turner, M., & Woodmansee, R. (1996). The
report of the ecological society of America committee on the scientific
basis for ecosystem management. Ecological Applications, 6(3), 665-691.
Coble, T., Lin, H. S., Coble, D.W., Hart, J.L., Williams, P.S.,
& Darville, R. (2005). Visitor Voices Project 2005: Final report.
Retrieved from http://www.wnpa.org/research/ lyjo05-15yr2.pdf.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cortner, H. & Moote, M. (1999). The politics of ecosystem
management. Washington, DC: Island Press.
Coyle, K. (2005). Environmental literacy in America: What ten years
of NEETF/Roper research and related studies say about environmental
literacy in the U.S. Washington, DC: The National Environmental
Education & Training Foundation.
Dettmann-Easler, D., & Pease, J. L. (1999). Evaluating the
effectiveness of residential environmental education programs in
fostering positive attitudes towards wildlife. Journal of Environmental
Education, 31(1), 33-40.
Dillman, D. A. (2007). Mail and internet surveys: The tailored
design method. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Son, Inc.
Dilsaver, L., (2004). Cumberland Island National Seashore: A
History of Conservation Conflict. University of Virginia Press,
Charlottesville, VA.
DiMauro, D., & Dietz, T. (2001). Assessment of an interpretive
program for U.S. Coast Guard personnel. Conservation Biology, 15(3),
776-779.
Dustin, D. & McAvoy, L. (1985). Interpretation as a management
tool: A dissenting opinion. The Interpreter, 16, 18-20.
Falk, J. H. (2001). Free-choice science learning: Framing the
discussion. In J. H. Falk (Ed.), Free-choice science education: How we
learn science outside of school. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Ferraro, P. J., & Pattanayak, S. K. (2006). Money for nothing?
A call for empirical evaluation of biodiversity conservation
investments. PLoS Biology, 4(4), 483-488.
Fraedrich, S.W., Harrington, T.C., Rabaglia, R.J., Ulyshen, M.D.,
Mayfield, A.E., Hanula, J.L., Eickwort, J.M. & Miller, D.R. (2008).
A fungal symbiont of the Redbay Ambrosia Beetle causes a lethal wilt in
Redbay and other lauraceae in the southeastern United States. Plant
Disease, 92(2), 215-224.
Gurevitch, J., & Padilla, D. K. (2004). Are invasive species a
major cause of extinctions? TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution, 19(9),
470-474.
Ham, S. H. (1992). Environmental interpretation: A practical guide
for people with big ideas and small budgets. Golden, CO: Fulcrum
Publishing.
Ham, S. H., & Weiler, B. (2002) Toward a theory of quality in
cruise-based nature guiding. Journal of Interpretation Research, 7(2),
29-49.
Ham, S. H., & Weiler, B. (2007). Isolating the role of on-site
interpretation in a satisfying experience. Journal of Interpretation
Research, 12(2), 5-24.
Hammitt, W. E. (1984). Cognitive processes involved in
environmental interpretation. Journal of Environmental Education 15(4):
11-15.
Hendee, J.C. & Dawson, C.P. (2002). Wilderness visitor
management: Stewardship for quality experiences. In Wilderness
Management: Stewardship and Protection of Resources and Values. Golden,
CO: Fulcrum.
Henker, K. B., & Brown, G. (2011). As good as the real thing? A
comparative study of interpretive podcasts and traditional ranger talks.
Journal of Interpretation Research, 16(1), 7-23.
Houston, D.B. & Schreiner, E.G. (1995). Alien species in
National Parks: Drawing lines in space and time. Conservation Biology,
9(1), 204-209.
Hughes, M., Ham, S., & Brown, T. (2009). Influencing park
visitor behavior: A belief-based approach. Journal of Park and
Recreation Administration, 27(4), 38-53.
Hughes, M. (2004). Influence of varying intensities of natural
on-site interpretation on attitudes and knowledge. Ph.D. Thesis, Murdoch
University.
Kim, A.K., Airey, D. & Szivas, E. (2011). The multiple
assessment of interpretation effectiveness: Promoting visitors'
environmental attitudes and behavior. Journal of Travel Research, 50(3),
321-334.
Kirkpatrick, J.F. (1995). Management of wild horses by fertility
control: The Assategue experience. NPS Scientific Monograph, (26), p.
60.
Knapp, D., & Benton, G. M. (2004). Elements to successful
interpretation: A multiple case study of five National Parks. Journal of
Interpretation Research, 9(2), 9-25.
Knudson, D. M., Cable, T. T., & Beck, L. (2003). Interpretation
of cultural and natural resources (2nd Ed.). State College, PA: Venture
Publishing.
Lockwood, J. L., Hoopes, M. F., & Marchetti, M. P. (2008).
Invasion ecology. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Louv, R. (2005). Last Child in the Woods: Saving Our Children from
Nature-Deficit Disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: Algonquin Books.
Madin, E.M.P. & Fenton, D.M. (2004). Environmental
interpretation in the Great Barrier Reef Marine Park: An assessment of
programme effectiveness. Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 12(2), 121-137.
Marion, J. L., & Rogers, C. S. (1994). The applicability of
terrestrial visitor impact management strategies to the protection of
coral reefs. Ocean & Coastal Management, 22, 153-163.
Marion, J.L.& Reid, S. (2007). Minimizing visitor impacts to
protected areas: the efficacy of low impact education programmes.
Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 15(1), 5-27.
Marynowski, S. B., and S. K. Jacobson. 1999. Ecosystem management
education for public lands. Wildlife Society Bulletin 27, 134-145.
McNeely, J. A. (1996). Human dimensions of invasive alien species:
how global perspectives are relevant to China. Conserving China's
biodiversity (II). Beijing, China Environmental Science Press, 169-181.
McNeely, J.A., (2001). The great reshuffling: Human dimensions of
invasive alien species. Gland, Switzerland & Cambridge, UK: World
Conservation Union (IUCN).
McFarlane, B. L., & Boxall, P. C. (2000). Factors influencing
forest values and attitudes of two stakeholder groups: The case of the
foothills model forest, Alberta, Canada. Society & Natural
Resources, 13, 649-661.
Monroe, J.K., Morrison-Sunders, A. and Hughes, M. (2008).
Environmental interpretation evaluation in natural areas. Journal of
Ecotourism, 7(1), 1-13.
Moscardo, G. (1999). Communicating with two million tourists. A
formative evaluation of an interpretive brochure. Journal of
Interpretation Research, Special Issue: Interpretation in Australia,
4(1), 21-37.
Nimmo, D. G., & Miller, K. K. (2007). Ecological and human
dimensions of management of feral horses in Australia: a review.
Wildlife Research, 34(5), 408.
Novey, L. T., & Hall, T. E. (2006). The effect of audio tours
on learning and social interaction: An evaluation at Carlsbad Caverns
National Park. Science Education, 91, 260-277.
National Park Service (NPS). (1984). General management plan:
Cumberland Island National Seashore, GA. Washington, DC: U.S. Department
of the Interior, National Park Service. Retrieved from
http://home.nps.gov/cuis/parkmgmt/upload/cuis_ centennial_strategy.pdf
National Park Service (NPS). (2002). The National Park Service
comprehensive survey of the American public. Retrieved from
http://www.nature.nps.gov/socialscience/pdf/ AKR_Tech_Rep.pdf
Oliver, S.S., Roggenbuck, J.W. & Watson, A.E. (1985). Education
to reduce impacts in forest campgrounds. Journal of Forestry, 83(4),
234-236.
Orams, M. (1997). The effectiveness of environmental education: Can
we turn tourists in "greenies?" Progress in Tourism and
Hospitality Research, 3, 295-306.
Petty, R. & Cacioppo, J. (1986). Communication and Persuasion:
Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York, USA:
Springer-Verlag.
Pimentel, D., Zuniga, R., Morrsion, D. (2005). Update on the
environmental and economic costs associated with alien-invasive species
in the United States. Ecological Economics, 52, 273-288.
Powell, R. B., & Ham, S. H. (2008). Can ecotourism
interpretation really lead to pro-conservation knowledge, attitudes and
behavior? Evidence from the Galapagos Islands. Journal of Sustainable
Tourism, 16(4), 467-489.
Rikoon, J. S. (2006). Wild horses and the political ecology of
nature restoration in the Missouri Ozarks. Geoforum, 37(2), 200-211.
Roggenbuck, J.W. & Berrier, D.L. (1982). A comparison of the
effectiveness of two communication strategies in dispersing wilderness
campers. Journal of Leisure Research, 14, 77-89.
Roggenbuck, J.W. & Watson, A.E. (1986). Providing information
for management purposes. In: Kulhavy, D.L., Conner, R.N., eds. A
symposium: Wilderness and natural areas in the eastern United States: A
management challenge; 1985 May 13-15; Nacogdoches, TX., pps. 236-242.
Roggenbuck, J. W., Loomis, R. J., & Dagostino, J. (1991). The
learning benefits of leisure. Journal of Leisure Research, 22(2),
112-124.
Sellars, R.W. (1997). Preserving nature in the national parks. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Sharp, R. L., Larson, L. R., & Green, G. T. (2011). Factors
influencing public preferences for invasive alien species management.
Biological Conservation, 144, 2097-2104.
Silverman, L. H., & Barrie, E. R. (2000). Determining social
science research needs in interpretation: A case study. Journal of
Interpretation Research, 5(1), 35-44.
Wider, C.J. and, J.W. (2000). Reducing theft of petrified wood at
Petrified Forest National Park. Journal of Interpretation Research,
5(1), 1-18.
Wiles, R. & Hall, T. (2005). Can interpretive messages change
park visitors' views on wildland fire? Journal of Interpretation
Research, 10(2), 18-35.
Williams, C. K., Ericsson, G., & Heberlein, T. A. (2002). A
Quantitative Summary of Attitudes toward Wolves and Their Reintroduction
(1972-2000). Wildlife Society Bulletin, 30(2), 575-584.
Zeppel, H., & Muloin, S. (2008). Conservation and education
benefits of interpretation on marine wildlife tours. Tourism in Marine
Environments, 5(2), 215-227.
Ryan L. Sharp
Eastern Kentucky University
405 Begley Building
Richmond, KY 40475
[email protected]
Lincoln R. Larson
Daniel B. Warnell School of Forestry and Natural Resources
University of Georgia
Athens, GA 30602
Gary T. Green
Daniel B. Warnell School of Forestry and Natural Resources
University of Georgia
Athens, GA 30602
Sara Tomek
University of Alabama
Box 870231
Tuscaloosa, AL 35487
Table 1. Demographic Composition of Treatment Groups (with
Chi-square Difference Tests)
Treatment Group
Variable Control Flyer Talk TOTAL
N 404 313 351 1093
Gender 48.5 49.3 52.7 50.0
(% male)
Age 37.2 44.9 36.9 39.6
(% under 40)
Race/Ethnicity 93.5 94.6 92.6 93.6
(% White)
Education 53.9 59.9 57.6 56.9
(% w/ advanced degree)
CUIS Length of Stay 57.2 71.5 63.6 63.8
(% day use only)
Transportation to CUIS 92.1 96.7 97.8 95.3
(% riding NPS ferry)
Any National Park Visit 60.8 55.6 54.0 57.1
(% visiting in past year)
Any National Park Visit 85.5 86.4 82.9 85.1
(% visiting in past 5 years)
Consumptive Outdoor 42.4 38.6 47.8 42.7
Recreation Participant
(% participating)
Member of Conservation Org. 15.4 15.6 13.2 14.8
(% members)
Invasive Species in Media 52.0 54.8 49.2 52.1
(% exposed to media info on
invasive species in past year)
Variable Diff. Stat.
N
Gender [chi square](2)=1.4,
(% male) p=0.509
Age [chi square](2)=6.0,
(% under 40) p=0.049
Race/Ethnicity [chi square](2)=1.1,
(% White) p=20.585
Education [chi square](2)=2.8,
(% w/ advanced degree) p=20.252
CUIS Length of Stay [chi square](2)=16.8,
(% day use only) p<0.001
Transportation to CUIS [chi square](2)=15.0,
(% riding NPS ferry) p=0.001
Any National Park Visit [chi square](2)=3.9,
(% visiting in past year) p=20.144
Any National Park Visit [chi square](2)=1.7,
(% visiting in past 5 years) p=20.423
Consumptive Outdoor [chi square](2)=6.0,
Recreation Participant p=0.051
(% participating)
Member of Conservation Org. [chi square](2)=1.7,
(% members) p=0.423
Invasive Species in Media [chi square](2)=2.1,
(% exposed to media info on p=0.349
invasive species in past year)
Table 2. Percentage of Cumberland Visitors in Each Assigned
Treatment Group Acknowledging Exposure to Invasive Species
Information from Different Sources During Their Visit
Assigned Treatment Group
Variable Control Flyer Talk
(%) (%) (%)
Information from Any Source 29.8 69.1 70.3
Flyer 2.0 54.3 0.6
Talk 2.4 1.1 56.6
Other Ranger Talk 26.8 13.8 12.2