首页    期刊浏览 2024年12月02日 星期一
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Speculating on the role of context in the outcomes of interpretive programs.
  • 作者:Powell, Robert B. ; Stern, Marc J.
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Interpretation Research
  • 印刷版ISSN:1092-5872
  • 出版年度:2013
  • 期号:July
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:National Association for Interpretation
  • 摘要:The interpretive equation suggests that successful interpretation requires that an interpreter must have knowledge of not only the resource, but also of their audience (Lacome, 2003). With this knowledge, interpreters can select and use appropriate techniques to make meaningful connections for visitors. In other words, interpretation is not a "one size fits all" prospect; selection and use of appropriate techniques depends upon the characteristics of the audience including their age, background, expectations, and motivations for attendance. Although not explicitly accounted for in the interpretation equation, setting and other context elements may also meaningfully influence interpretive programs and their outcomes (Larsen, 2003; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Moscardo, 1999). Some suggest that characteristics of the setting, attributes of the resource, and the collective characteristics of the audience form integral parts of the interpretive experience and should be accounted for in the planning and implementation phases (Larsen, 2003; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Moscardo, 1999).
  • 关键词:Interpretive programs (Parks and museums);National parks;National parks and reserves

Speculating on the role of context in the outcomes of interpretive programs.


Powell, Robert B. ; Stern, Marc J.


Introduction

The interpretive equation suggests that successful interpretation requires that an interpreter must have knowledge of not only the resource, but also of their audience (Lacome, 2003). With this knowledge, interpreters can select and use appropriate techniques to make meaningful connections for visitors. In other words, interpretation is not a "one size fits all" prospect; selection and use of appropriate techniques depends upon the characteristics of the audience including their age, background, expectations, and motivations for attendance. Although not explicitly accounted for in the interpretation equation, setting and other context elements may also meaningfully influence interpretive programs and their outcomes (Larsen, 2003; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Moscardo, 1999). Some suggest that characteristics of the setting, attributes of the resource, and the collective characteristics of the audience form integral parts of the interpretive experience and should be accounted for in the planning and implementation phases (Larsen, 2003; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Moscardo, 1999).

The other articles in this special issue explore which interpretive techniques are most strongly associated with visitor outcomes across a wide range of programs. But do certain techniques or approaches work better or worse in particular contexts and with certain audiences? To what extent does "context" influence visitor outcomes? This paper explores interactions between the duration, topic, type, and setting of programs, the nature of the interpreted resources, the size and age makeup of the audience, and visitor outcomes. The results of this study support the idea that context matters. We explore data collected from 272 programs across 24 diverse units of the U.S. National Park Service to build speculative hypotheses about which interpreter and program characteristics may be more or less important in producing positive visitor outcomes in different contexts.

Interactional theory

Interpretive programs and resulting visitor outcomes can be thought of as an interaction between the characteristics of the audience, the site/setting, the interpreter, and the interpretive program (Archer & Wearing, 2003; Mayer & Wallace, 2008; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Powell, Kellert, & Ham, 2009; Wearing & Wearing, 2001). This notion of interactions between humans and their social and physical environments influencing cognition and behavior is the main premise of interactional theory (Altman & Rogoff, 1987; Stokols & Altman, 1987). Through the lens of interactional theory, visitor outcomes associated with attending interpretive programs result from the interaction of the characteristics of the program, the interpreter, other audience members, and the setting in which the program occurs (Archer & Wearing, 2003; Arnould & Price, 1993; Falk & Deirking, 2000; Wearing & Wearing, 2001). This theoretical approach acknowledges that interpretive programs are complex and promotes a holistic view of the relationships between multiple factors that together produce experiential outcomes (Altman & Rogoff, 1987; Archer & Wearing, 2003; Brochu & Merriman, 2002; Wearing & Wearing, 2001).

Potential influences of context: Audience, program, and setting characteristics

Research and theory suggest that the makeup of the audience should influence the techniques that are used as well as the outcomes of a program (Ham, 2013; Larsen, 2003). Although it is assumed that audience size and the age ranges of an audience will influence the selection of interpretive techniques, few have examined which techniques work best for different audience makeups (from all children to all adults) or how audience makeup may influence outcomes. Coble and others (2013) provide one exception, finding that the presence of children in an audience reduced the formation of intellectual and emotional connections made by audience members in U.S. National Park Service interpretive programs.

The bulk of the research on the effects of group size comes from the formal education literature and suggests that smaller class sizes in formal settings tend to produce improved student outcomes (Boozer & Rouse, 2001; Finn & Achilles, 1999; Glass, 1982). In informal settings, such as in the case of interpretation and environmental education, there is less conclusive evidence. Powell and others (2009) examined visitors who received interpretation while rafting down the Colorado River through Grand Canyon National Park and found that group size was negatively associated with knowledge gain. Coble and others (2013) also found that as group size increased, intellectual connections decreased in attendees to NPS interpretation. However, Stern and others (2008) investigated the influence of group size at a residential environmental education center for elementary school children and found that larger groups were associated with improved awareness and interest in discovery and learning.

It is often assumed that the longer someone engages with an interpretive opportunity, whether an exhibit or a live interpretive program, the better the outcomes. While some empirical research supports this assertion, most have studied the influence of the number of interpretive programs attended or the number of days of a residential program and not the influence of duration of a single live interpretive program (Stern et al., in press). For example, Powell and others (2009), Stern and others (2008), Ballantyne and Packer (2005), and Coble and others (2013) have all found that greater exposure led to more positive outcomes. Museum and exhibit visitor studies also support the notion that the longer one engages an exhibit or collection of exhibits, the better (Falk, 2004).

We found few studies that examined whether particular types of interpretive programs were more or less effective in producing positive audience outcomes. Coble and others (2013) found that interpretive films were not as successful at producing intellectual connections as other interpretive program types such as live interpretation, illustrated programs, exhibits, and other conducted activities; no other trends were found. Van Winkle (2012) also examined the differences between electronic audio vs. live interpretation and found no differences in learning outcomes. We also examined whether particular interpretive techniques were more effective in programs interpreting natural resources vs. cultural resources and were unable to find prior research.

Other factors that may influence cognitive, affective, and behavioral outcomes include park setting, program location, and quality of the resource (Archer & Wearing, 2003; Mayer & Wallace, 2008; Merriman & Brochu, 2005; Powell et al., 2009; Wearing & Wearing, 2001). We refer to "park setting" in this study as a description of where the park unit that provided the interpretation program falls on the urban to remote spectrum. Different park units in different settings have different resources and may attract different visitors, each arriving with different motivations. However, it is still unclear if certain program practices work better in particular locations.

Natural environments, as opposed to built or indoor environments, are thought to enhance affective outcomes such as interests, emotions, and attitudes; cognitive outcomes such as learning; and psychological restoration (Crompton & Sellar, 1981; Kahn & Kellert, 2002; R. Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; R. Kaplan, Kaplan, & Ryan, 1998; Kellert, 2005; Stern, Powell, & Hill, in press). However, several reviews of the literature suggest that indoor settings can be more effective than outdoor settings and other non-traditional settings for producing certain student outcomes (Zelezny, 1999; Zink & Burrows, 2008). Therefore the influence of conducting live interpretation in indoor vs. outdoor locations and ascertaining which program practices work best in each may be more nuanced than previously thought.

Another aspect of the setting with potential to influence the outcomes of interpretation includes the quality of the resource and setting. Larsen (2003) suggests that the basis of most interpretation is a tangible resource, which has some iconic value that anchors the program. In fact, research suggests that some resources and settings with unique iconic or symbolic qualities may have powerful impacts on visitors affective, cognitive, and behavioral domains. For example, extreme aesthetic natural and built environments have been associated with peak, spiritual, extraordinary, and transformative experiences (S. Kaplan, 1993; Laski, 1961; Otto, 1958; Powell, Brownlee, Kellert, & Ham, 2012), increased feelings of satisfaction and enjoyment (Arnould & Price, 1993; Powell et al., 2012), enhanced ethical concern for nature and commitment to stewardship (Kellert, 1996; Powell et al., 2012), enhanced emotional and cognitive connections (Kellert, 2005; Powell et al., 2012), and feelings of awe and wonder (Kellert & Farnham, 2002; Powell et al., 2012). Expansive, grand, and austere landscapes also may promote feelings of humility, spirituality, and even fear (Brown & Raymond, 2007; Galagher, 1993; Heintzman, 2009; Heintzman & Mannell, 2003; Koecni, 2005; Powell et al., 2012; Williams & Harvey, 2001). Therefore it seems appropriate to examine whether the quality of a program's resource influences the participant's outcomes.

Finally, three intervening variables--the occurrence of accidents or other negative events; the occurrence of positive events, such as the sighting of a charismatic animal; and extreme weather--are also consider in this study because of their potential to influence the interpretive experience, and because they are considered largely outside the control of the interpreter and the audience (Powell et.al., 2009).

This study sought to better understand 1) the extent to which the context variables discussed above influence visitor outcomes and 2) whether certain forms of program delivery appear to work better or worse in particular contexts. These forms of program delivery are divided into interpreter characteristics and program characteristics and are described in detail in Stern and Powell (article 1, this issue).

Methods

We observed 376 live interpretation programs conducted by the NPS across 24 different park units. During these programs we recorded the occurrence and extent of a wide-range of characteristics pertaining to program practices, interpreter attributes, and context (audience, program, and setting). Program practices were drawn from an extensive literature review that identified recommended practices (Skibins, Powell, & Stern, 2012). Interpreter attributes were largely identified from a review of the communications and education literature, although many are also referenced in the interpretation literature (see Stern & Powell, article 1, this issue). For a complete list, see Stern and Powell (article 1, this issue).

Immediately after each interpretive program, we administered short questionnaires to attendees who were over the age of 15 to gauge the influence of these programs on three dependent variables (Table 1). The first dependent variable measured program attendees' level of satisfaction, using a single survey item that asked visitors to rate their overall level of satisfaction with the program they had just attended on a scale ranging from 0 ("terrible") to 10 ("excellent"). The second dependent variable, "visitor experience and appreciation," was composed of five survey items. The third dependent variable, "behavioral intentions," was composed of two survey items that gauged the program's influence on attendees' intentions to change future behaviors in the park and at home. The items comprising the two scales were measured using a five-point Likert-type scale, with answer choices: Not at all (1), A little (2), Somewhat (3), A moderate amount (4), and A great deal (5). Composite scores were created for each of the scales by taking the mean of all items.

From the 376 live interpretation programs, 64 were eliminated from analyses because of missing data or low response rates. We then divided the remaining 312 programs into those that served fewer than five people (n = 40) and those that served five or more (n = 272) because literature suggests that small programs are inherently different phenomenon than larger programs (Forist, 2003; McManus, 1987, 1988). We use the five-and-over sample in this paper because of the larger sample size, except for when examining the influence of group size. In this study, the interpretive program served as our unit of analysis. Therefore, all dependent variables were aggregated to the program level by calculating the mean score for each program (Table 1). For further information regarding sampling, data collection, data cleaning, dependent variable development procedures, program practices, and interpreter characteristics see Stern and Powell (article 1, this issue).

The audience, program, and setting characteristics under investigation included two continuous variables, four categorical variables, and two ordinal variables (Table 2). The two continuous variables included group size and program duration. The four categorical variables included the program topic, the program type, the park setting, and the location of the program. The two ordinal descriptors--the ratio of children to adults in the audience and quality of the resource--were recorded by the researchers in the field. Finally three intervening variables--the occurrence of extreme weather, the occurrence of accidents or other negative events, and the occurrence of positive events--were recorded because of their potential for influencing the interpretive experience. Table 2 provides a definition for each variable, an explanation of its measurement, and the mean or frequency depending upon the type of variable.

Results

How did context influence outcomes?

We first examine whether particular context variables are directly related to different outcomes. In other words, do certain contexts tend to produce different results? We also examine whether certain program characteristics or interpreter delivery styles are more prevalent in different contexts.

Group Size: The number of attendees to the 312 interpretive programs included in this analysis ranged from one person to approximately 600 people. The mean audience size was 48 and the median number of attendees was 17. When examining the correlation between the size of the audience and outcomes, we found no consistent relationships with satisfaction or the visitor experience and appreciation program outcomes. However, as audience numbers increased, programs tended to record greater audience intentions to change behaviors (r = .127; p = .031). As audience sizes increased, interpreters also tended to score higher in confidence (r = .237; p < .001), organization (r = .167; p = .002) of their programs, and humor quality (r = .213; p < .001). However, they also tended to be more formal (r = .346; p < .001) and provide less physical (r = -.140; p = .009) and verbal engagement (-.308; p < .001).

Ratio of children to adults: In programs with five or more attendees, 9% of the programs (n = 25) had mostly children present; 31% (n = 82) had roughly an equal mix of adults and children; 49% (n = 132) had mostly adults; and 11% (n = 29) had all adults. The higher the ratio of children to adults, the higher the behavioral intentions score (r = .182; p = 0.003). In other words, the more children present, the more likely adult participants were to report that the program had changed their behavioral intentions. Programs with higher ratios of children to adults were more commonly multisensory (r = .143; p = .019) and contained elements of novelty (r = -.133; p = .029). Interpreters were more likely to share their own personal stories (r = .151; p = 0.014) when more adults were present relative to children. Programs with all adults were more commonly solely fact-based than those where children were present (Pearson [chi square] = 7.6; p = .006).

Program duration: Advertised program lengths ranged from 15 minutes to four hours. Actual program lengths ranged from 10 minutes to three hours. The average program length was just under 49 minutes. No statistically significant relationships were observed between program duration and visitor outcomes.

Program focus: One-hundred and seventy (63%) of the programs focused primarily on cultural heritage; 70 (26%) had a primary focus on the natural environment. Twenty-nine (11%) had a dual focus. Behavioral intentions scores were statistically higher for nature-based programs (means: 3.05 vs. 2.84, t = 2.2, p = 0.026; Cohen's d = 0.33). No other statistically significant differences were noted in overall outcomes. In interviews prior to the programs, interpreters were more likely to express behavioral change as an intended outcome for nature-focused programs as opposed to culturally focused programs ([chi square] = 7.4; p = .007).

Program type: Programs included guided walks and tours (n = 161); talks, slide shows, and multi-media presentations (n = 98); demonstrations (n = 5); and activities (n = 8). Guided walks/tours and stationary talks made up 95% of the programs we observed. No statistically significant differences in outcomes between program types were observed.

Urban vs. remote: Within our sample of programs with five or more attendees, 91 (33%) programs took place in urban parks, 50 (18%) took place in urban-proximate parks, and 131 (48%) took place in remote parks. There were no significant differences in outcomes based upon proximity to urban centers.

Indoors vs. outdoors: Seventy-two percent (n=195) of programs took place outdoors; 20% (n = 55) took place indoors; and 8% (n = 22) used both indoor and outdoor settings. Visitor experience and appreciation scores tended to be greater following programs that took place entirely outdoors when compared to programs that took place entirely indoors (means: 4.45 vs. 4.33; t = 2.6; p = 0.011; Cohen's d = 0.36) or programs that had both indoor and outdoor components (means: 4.45 vs. 4.25; t = 2.1; p = 0.039; Cohen's d = 0.55). Indoor programs also tended to have larger audiences than programs conducted outdoors (means: 171.79 vs. 24.87; t = 8.8; p < .001; Cohen's d = 0.95).

Resource quality: We rated the quality of the resource where the program occurred. Forty-nine percent of program resources were rated as iconic or grandiose; 38% were rated as pleasant but not iconic; and 13% were rated as unimpressive or generic. The mean on the scale was 2.37 (s.d. = 0.69). The quality of the resource did not exhibit any consistent relationships with program outcomes.

Exceptional events: Thirty-four programs (13%) experienced negative events such as interruptions, technical difficulties, and accidents. Nine (3%) of the programs experienced notably bad weather. Only five programs (2%) experienced unexpected positive events, such as a rare animal sighting. We combined bad weather and negative events and conducted a means comparison between these programs and those without negative circumstances. Programs with negative circumstances (n=43) exhibited significantly lower satisfaction (means: 8.70 vs. 8.99; t = 2.8; p = 0.006; Cohen's d = 0.33) and visitor experience and appreciation scores (means: 4.25 vs. 4.44; t = 3.6; p < .001; Cohen's d = 0.43) than programs without these distractions. The small number of programs that experienced positive unexpected events precluded further analysis.

Which programmatic practices and interpreter attributes appear to work better in different contexts?

To examine whether different programmatic practices and interpreter attributes influence outcomes better in particular contexts and settings, we split the sample in the following ways: programs with larger and smaller proportions of children in the audience, culturally focused vs. environmentally focused programs, programs conducted in remote vs. urban parks, and indoor vs. outdoor programs. To ensure adequate sample sizes, we used the sample of programs with more than five attendees for each analysis.

We examined the relationships between interpreter and program characteristics and visitor outcomes within each context. We report only characteristics that show at least one statistically significant relationship with an outcome.

When a correlation coefficient for a particular program practice was significant in one context and not in another, we used Fisher r to z transformation to assess the significance of these differences. Fisher r to z transformation compares correlation coefficients of different groups, taking into account their respective sample sizes. The test yields a z-score and associated p-value. These statistics provide a more stringent criteria for distinguishing differences in correlation coefficients across the subsamples and helped us avoid Type I errors (cases in which a real relationship is assumed, but sufficient evidence is lacking to support it). We have bolded and shaded these significant differences (z-score at p < 0.05) in the subsequent correlation tables. To further evaluate differences in binary variables' relationships to outcomes, we only highlight instances where the mean score in one subsample is significant at p < 0.01 and the other is not statistically significant (p > 0.05). Our goal in these analyses is to take a conservative approach to identifying practices that appear to operate differently in different contexts. Because the sample sizes shrink rapidly as we split the data into subsamples, we acknowledge that the emergent patterns are speculative rather than definitive trends.

Adult audiences vs. audiences with children: Tables 3 and 4 summarize relationships between program and interpreter characteristics and visitor outcomes in programs with different ratios of children to adults in their audiences. The column labeled "adults" represents programs in which adults made up a clear majority of the audience (60% of [programs). The column labeled "children" represents programs with an equal or greater number of children compared to adults (40% of programs). Only characteristics showing at least one statistically significant relationship with an outcome are presented. While several program practices and interpreter attributes were consistently important irrespective of audience, there were several that appeared to be only significant for audiences with a large number of children and were significantly different from the mostly and all adult subsample. To determine which of these differences might be the most meaningful, we conducted Fisher r to z transformations to compare the correlation coefficients of different groups. We have bolded and shaded these differences in Table 3 (and subsequent correlation tables) that yielded a statistically significant z-score at p < 0.05.

These analyses reveal that four characteristics had stronger relationships to outcomes in programs with more children than they did in programs with little or no children. Confidence of the interpreter was more strongly linked with positive changes in behavior intentions in programs with more children (z = 2.01; p = 0.01). Appropriate for the audience was more strongly linked with behavioral intentions as well (z = 2.72; p < 0.01). Appropriate logistics and audibility were more strongly linked with satisfaction (z = 2.30; p = 0.01 and z = 1.71; p = 0.04, respectively) and visitor experience and appreciation (z = 2.88; p < 0.01 and z = 2.32; p = 0.01, respectively) in programs with more children. Humor quality (z = 2.40; p < 0.01) and humor quantity (z = 2.25; p = 0.01) were also more predictive of visitor experience and appreciation in programs with more children. Differences noted in t-tests did not meet our threshold.

In short, the results suggest that most of the key best practices identified in Stern and Powell (article 1, this issue) cut across contexts. However, certain program characteristics may be particularly beneficial with audiences dominated by children. These include exhibiting confidence, using humor, ensuring audibility, gearing program content and delivery style to the specific audience, and paying careful attention to appropriate logistics.

Natural vs. cultural focused programs: We ran a similar set of analyses for nature-focused vs. culture/history-focused programs (Tables 5 and 6). For this analysis, we removed programs with equally balanced nature-based and cultural-based content because of their small sample size (n=29). There were 70 nature-focused programs and 170 cultural/history-focused programs. The results suggest a consistent list of program elements that are significant in both natural and cultural programs. However, three interpreter characteristics appeared to have different influences on outcomes according to our criteria. Humor quantity was positively linked with satisfaction (z = 1.69; p = .04) and visitor enjoyment and appreciation (z = 2.03; p = .02) in cultural programs but not in nature-based programs. Making a false assumption about the audience was negatively related to visitor enjoyment and appreciation (z = -2.39; p < 0.01) in nature-based programs but not in cultural programs. Sarcasm (z = -1.97; p = .02) was negatively related to behavioral intentions in the nature-based programs but not cultural programs. Differences noted in t-tests did not meet our threshold.

In summary, it appears that making false assumptions about the audience and sarcasm may be more damaging to visitor outcomes in nature-focused programs than in cultural programs. Meanwhile, additional attempts at humor may have more positive influences on visitor outcomes in cultural programs as opposed to nature-based programs. Urban vs. remote parks: Within our sample of programs with five or more attendees, 91 programs took place in urban parks, 50 took place in urban-proximate parks, and 131 took place in remote parks. Because of the small number of programs within the urban-proximate park subsample, we dropped this group from the analysis. We thus explored only differences between programs occurring in urban and remote park units. When examining the relationship between location, outcomes and program and interpreter characteristics, certain variables appeared more predictive of outcomes in certain areas.

Tables 7 and 8 summarize relationships between program and interpreter characteristics and outcomes in both urban and remote parks. Again, most previously identified "best practices" (Stern & Powell, this issue) cut across park types. However, four interpreter delivery styles and two program characteristics displayed potentially meaningful differences in their relationships to outcomes. Sarcasm showed more positive relationships with satisfaction (z = 2.11; p = 0.02) and visitor experience and appreciation (z = 2.44; p < 0.01) in urban parks and a negative relationship with changes in behavioral intentions in remote parks (z = -1.94; p = 0.03). Surprise exhibited more positive relationships with changes in behavioral intentions in remote park units (z = 3.15; p < 0.01). Humor quantity was more positively linked with satisfaction (z = 2.82; p < 0.01) and visitor experience and appreciation (z = 3.26; p < 0.01) in urban settings. Multisensory engagement was positively linked to satisfaction in urban settings (z = 1.01; p = 0.04), and audibility was more positively linked to visitor experience and appreciation in urban settings (z = 3.15; p = 0.05). Moreover, t-tests revealed that appropriate pace was more positively related to visitor experience and appreciation in remote settings than in urban settings.

In summary, sarcasm appears to be significantly more effective with audiences who visit urban parks than those who visit remote parks. In fact, it actually exhibited positive relationships with attitudinal outcomes (satisfaction and visitor experience and appreciation) in urban settings and a negative relationship with behavioral intentions in remote settings. Meanwhile, the element of surprise may be more effective for audiences who visit remote parks. Maintaining an appropriate pace may also be a more relevant concern for programs in remote parks than in urban parks. Focusing more heavily on humor and multisensory engagement may be more effective in urban settings. Moreover, audibility may be more of a meaningful issue in urban settings than in remote settings.

Indoor vs. outdoor programs: We also compared programs that took place indoors vs. programs that took place outdoors (Tables 9 and 10). For this analysis, we removed programs that took place both indoors and outdoors because of the small sample size (n = 22). There were 55 programs that took place completely indoors and 195 programs that occurred solely outdoors. Six program and interpreter characteristics showed significantly different relationships with observed outcomes across the two contexts. Confidence (z = 1.65; p = 0.05), consistency (z = 2.76; p < 0.01), and organization (z = 2.59; p < 0.01) were each more strongly related to more positive visitor experience and appreciation in outdoor programs. Physical engagement exhibited a significant positive relationship with visitor experience and appreciation in outdoor programs and a significant negative relationship in indoor programs (z = 2.86; p < 0.01). Multisensory engagement showed a more positive relationship with behavioral intentions in outdoor settings than in indoor settings (z = 1.84; p = 0.03). T-tests revealed that appropriate pace was more positively related to both satisfaction and visitor experience and appreciation in outdoor settings.

In summary, confidence, consistency, organization, and pace may be more important drivers of outcomes in outdoor settings than in indoor settings, though confidence and organization appear to be clearly important in both. Indoor audiences may less commonly feel comfortable with higher degrees of physical engagement when compared to outdoor audiences. Multisensory engagement was also more positively linked with changes in behavioral intentions for outdoor audiences than for indoor audiences. Finally, maintaining an appropriate pace was a better predictor of attitudinal outcomes (satisfaction and visitor experience and appreciation) in outdoor programs than it was in indoor programs.

Discussion and Conclusion

This study sought to better understand 1) the extent to which context influences outcomes for interpretive program attendees and 2) which program practices and interpreter attributes may work best in particular contexts. We first explored the potential influence of context. We examined the size of the audience and its age makeup, program characteristics such as duration, topic, and type, and characteristics of the setting including proximity to urban centers, program location (indoor vs. outdoor), and resource quality by testing their relationship to three outcomes, satisfaction, visitor experience and appreciation, and behavioral intentions. In these analyses, there were several trends. First, we found that as group size increased, intentions to perform stewardship behaviors also increased. One explanation for this trend could be the exertion of normative pressure from peers or other audience members to change behaviors (see Ajzen, 1992; Ham et al., 2007). However, we did not test this hypothesis. Second, we found that as the number of children in an audience increased, intentions to change behaviors increased. One explanation for this trend may be that an audience with more children may foster intergenerational learning (Ballantyne, Fien, & Packer, 2001; Duvall & Zint, 2007). Also, programs that served audiences with more children tended to be less fact-based and were more commonly multisensory and novel. Theory and research on behavior change supports the notion that presenting facts, or attempting to increase knowledge, has little to do with whether someone will change their behavior (e.g., Ham, 2013; Stern & Powell, this issue). We also found that programs that occurred outdoors produced greater visitor experience and appreciation in their audiences. This finding supports the notion that outdoor settings may enhance more emotive and affective outcomes, such as enjoyment and appreciation in participants (e.g., Kahn & Kellert, 2002; R. Kaplan et al., 1998; Kellert, 2005). These outdoor programs also tended to have smaller audiences. This combination of a more intimate social environment coupled with an outdoor setting may further enhance outcomes.

To investigate and then develop hypotheses about whether certain practices might work better or worse in particular contexts, we split our sample of interpretive programs based on four contextual variables: programs with greater vs. lesser proportions of children in the audience; culturally focused vs. nature-focused programs; programs conducted in remote vs. urban parks; and indoor vs. outdoors programs. We compared relationships between program practices and interpreter attributes and outcomes within each subsample. We then examined these differences using more stringent thresholds to determine which might be indicative of a potentially meaningful trend warranting the development of a hypothesis. Several trends emerged across these four comparisons. First, a consistent list of programmatic practices and interpreter attributes appear important for achieving better visitor outcomes across most contexts. These include confidence, authentic emotion and charisma, organization, connection, verbal engagement, appropriate for audience, clear message, responsiveness, and fact-based messaging (negative). These findings largely corroborate the results of our analyses in Stern and Powell, articles 1 and 4 this issue, and Powell and Stern, article 2 this issue. Despite the consistent performance of some program practices across context, we did identify program characteristics that appeared to perform differently in particular contexts (Table 11).

While most program and interpreter characteristics performed similarly in programs containing different adult-to-child ratios, certain characteristics appeared to be more beneficial with younger audiences. These included confidence, using humor, ensuring audibility, gearing program content and delivery style to the specific audience, and paying careful attention to appropriate logistics. Similarly, few potentially meaningful differences surfaced between nature-focused and culturally focused programs in terms of the characteristics most strongly associated with outcomes. Making false assumptions about the audience met with less positive attitudinal visitor outcomes (satisfaction and visitor experience and appreciation) and using sarcasm exhibited a negative relationship with changes in behavioral intentions in nature-focused programs. Meanwhile, humor met with more positive attitudinal visitor outcomes in cultural programs.

We found similar trends with the relative influence of sarcasm and humor when comparing urban vs. remote parks. Each exhibited stronger positive links with attitudinal outcomes in urban parks and sarcasm was negatively related to behavioral outcomes in remote parks. Focusing more heavily on humor and multisensory engagement may be more effective in urban settings. Moreover, audibility may be more of a meaningful issue in urban settings than in remote settings. Our analyses suggest that maintaining an appropriate pace may not only be more important in remote settings as opposed to urban settings, but also in outdoor settings as opposed to indoor settings.

Confidence, consistency, organization, and pace may also be more important drivers of outcomes in outdoor settings than in indoor settings, though confidence and organization appeared to be clearly important in both. Physical engagement was positively linked to attitudinal outcomes in outdoor programs and negatively associated with the same outcomes in indoor programs. This suggests that audiences of indoor programs may have different expectations than audiences of outdoor programs and may not be as comfortable with physical engagement.

Overall, our analyses suggest that most of the "best practices" identified in the broader sample (Stern & Powell, this issue) are important regardless of context. However, some program and interpreter characteristics may operate differently in different settings and across contexts. However, we submit that all of the contextual differences explained herein are speculative and would require additional targeted investigation to validate. While we are confident that our overall sample represents a reasonable approximation of the diversity of interpretive programs across the NPS, we are less confident in the representativeness of each subsample. As our sample size is reduced, generalizability is weakened. As such, we suggest that the results of these contextual analyses should be thought of as hypotheses that could be further investigated to test their validity. The results, however, suggest that we can be confident in saying: context matters! Thus we urge researchers to design studies that can refine our understanding of how context influences outcomes, and which program practices and interpreter attributes work best in particular contexts.

References

Ajzen, I. (1992). Persuasive Communication Theory in Social Psychology: A Historical Perspective. In M. J. Manfredo (Ed.), Influencing Human Behavior (pp. 1-28). Champaign, IL: Sagamore Publishing, Inc.

Altman, I., & Rogoff, B. (1987). World view in psychology: Trait, interactionist, organismic, and transactionalist approaches. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology (pp. 1-40). New York: John Wiley.

Archer, D., & Wearing, S. (2003). Self, space, and interpretive experience: The interactionism of environmental interpretation. Journal of Interpretation Research, 8(1), 7-23.

Arnould, E. J., & Price, L. (1993). River Magic: Extraordinary Experience and the Extended Service Encounter. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1), 24-45.

Ballantyne, R., Fien, J., & Packer, J. (2001). Program Effectiveness in Facilitating Intergenerational Influence in Environmental Education: Lessons From the Field. The Journal of Environmental Education, 32(4), 8-15. doi: 10.1080/00958960109598657

Boozer, M., & Rouse, C. (2001). Intraschool variation in class size: Patterns and implications. Journal of Urban Economics, 50(1), 163-189.

Brochu, L., & Merriman, T. (2002). Personal interpretation: Connecting your audience to heritage resources. Fort Collins, CO: InterpPress.

Brown, G., & Raymond, C. (2007). The relationship between place attachment and landscape values: Toward mapping place attachment. Applied Geography, 27(2), 89-111. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apgeog.2006.11.002

Crompton, J. L., & Sellar, C. (1981). Do Outdoor Education Experiences Contribute to Positive Development in the Affective Domain? The Journal of Environmental Education, 12(4), 21-29. doi: 10.1080/00958964.1981.9942638

Duvall, J., & Zint, M. (2007). A Review of Research on the Effectiveness of Environmental Education in Promoting Intergenerational Learning. The Journal of Environmental Education, 38(4), 14-24. doi: 10.3200/joee.38.4.14-24

Falk, J. (2004). The director's cut: Toward an improved understanding of learning from museums. Science Education, 88(S1), S83-S96. doi: 10.1002/sce.20014

Falk, J., & Deirking, L. D. (2000). Learning from Museums. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.

Finn, J. D., & Achilles, C. M. (1999). Tennessee's class size study: Findings, implications, misconceptions. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 21(2), 97-109.

Forist, B. (2003). Visitor use and evaluation of interpretive media. A report on visitors to the National Park System. National Park Service Visitor Services Project.

Galagher, W. (1993). The power of place. New York: Poseidon.

Glass, G. V. (1982). School class size: Research and policy. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publication.

Ham, S. H., Brown, T. J., Curtis, J., Weiler, B., Hughes, M., & Poll, M. (2007). Promoting persuasion in protected areas: A guide for managers. Developing strategic communication to influence visitor behavior. Southport, Queensland, Australia: Sustainable Tourism Cooperative Research Centre.

Heintzman, P. (2009). Nature-Based Recreation and Spirituality: A Complex Relationship. Leisure Sciences, 32(1), 72-89. doi: 10.1080/01490400903430897

Heintzman, P., & Mannell, R. C. (2003). Spiritual Functions of Leisure and Spiritual Well-Being: Coping with Time Pressure. Leisure Sciences, 25(2-3), 207-230. doi: 10.1080/01490400306563

Kahn, P. H., & Kellert, S. R. (Eds.). (2002). Children and nature: Psychological, sociocultural, and evolutionary investigations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., & Ryan, R. L. (1998). With people in mind: Design and management of everyday nature. Washington DC: Island Press.

Kaplan, S. (1993). The role of natural environment aesthetics in the restorative experience. St. Paul, MN: U.S. Department of Agriculture, U.S. Forest Service.

Kellert, S. R. (1996). The Value of Life. Washington, D.C.: Island Press.

Kellert, S. R. (2005). Building for life: Designing and understanding the human-nature connection. Washington DC: Island Press.

Kellert, S. R., & Farnham, T. (Eds.). (2002). The Good in Nature and Humanity: Connecting Science, Religion, and Spirituality with the Natural World. Washington, DC: Island Press.

Koecni, V. (2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5, 27-44.

Lacome, B. (2003). The interpretation equation. In D. Larsen (Ed.), Meaningful interpretation: How to connect hearts and minds to places, objects, and other resources. Eastern National.

Larsen, D. (Ed.). (2003). Meaningful interpretation: How to connect hearts and minds to places, objects, and other resources. Eastern National.

Laski, M. (1961). Ecstasy: A study of some secular and religious experiences. London: Cressett Press.

Mayer, C. C., & Wallace, G. (2008). The interpretive power of setting: Identifying and protecting the interpretive postential of the internal and external setting at Copan Archaeological Park, Honduras. Journal of Interpretation Research, 13(2), 7-29.

McManus, P. M. (1987). It's the company you keep: The social determination of learning, Aerelated behaviour in a science museum. International Journal of Museum Management and Curatorship, 6(3), 263-270. doi: 10.1080/09647778709515076

McManus, P. M. (1988). Good companions: More on the social determination of learning, related behaviour in a science museum. International Journal of Museum Management and Curatorship, 7(1), 37-44. doi: 10.1080/09647778809515102

Merriman, T., & Brochu, L. (2005). Management of interpretive sites: Developing sustainable operations through effective management. Fort Collins, CO: InterpPress.

Moscardo, G. (1999). Making Visitors Mindful: Principles for Creating Quality Sustainable Visitor Experiences through Effective Communication. Champaign, IL: Sagamore Publishing.

Otto, R. (1958). The Idea of the Holy (J. W. Harvey, Trans. second ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Powell, R. B., Brownlee, M. T. J., Kellert, S. R., & Ham, S. H. (2012). From awe to satisfaction: immediate affective responses to the Antarctic tourism experience. Polar Record, 48(02), 145-156. doi: doi:10.1017/S0032247410000720

Powell, R. B., Kellert, S. R., & Ham, S. H. (2009). Interactional theory and the sustainable nature-based tourism experience. Society and Natural Resources, 28(8), 761-776.

Skibins, J. C., Powell, R. B., & Stern, M. J. (2012). Linking interpretation best practices with outcomes: A review of literature. Journal of Interpretation Research, 17(1), 25-44.

Stern, M. J., Powell, R. B., & Hill, D. Environmental education program evaluation in the new millennium: What do we measure and what have we learned? Environmental Education Research. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2013.838749

Stokols, D., & Altman, I. (Eds.). (1987). Handbook of Environmental Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Van Winkle, C. M. (2012). The effect of tour type on visitors' perceived cognitive load and learning. Journal of Interpretation Research, 17(1), 45-58.

Wearing, S., & Wearing, B. (2001). Conceptualizng the selves of tourism. Leisure Studies, 20, 143-159.

Williams, K., & Harvey, D. (2001). Transcendent experience in forest environments Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21(3), 249-260. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ jevp.2001.0204

Zelezny, L. C. (1999). Educational interventions that improve environmental behaviors: a meta-analysis. The Journal of Environmental Education, 31(1), 5-14. doi: 10.1080/00958969909598627

Zink, R., & Burrows, L. (2008). "Is what you see what you get?" The production of knowledge in-between the indoors and the outdoors in outdoor education. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 13(3), 251-265. doi: 10.1080/17408980701345733

Robert B. Powell

Department of Parks, Recreation and Tourism Management and School of Agricultural

and Forest Environmental Sciences, Clemson University

Marc J. Stern

Department of Forest Resources and Environmental Conservation, Virginia Tech
Table 1. Description and mean score of outcomes.

Outcomes                                            N    Mean   S.D.

Satisfaction                                       272   8.94   0.64

Visitor experience and appreciation (Cronbach's    272   4.41   0.32
[alpha] = .89)

* Made my visit to this park more enjoyable              4.55   0.30
* Made my visit to this park more meaningful             4.49   0.32
* Enhanced my appreciation for this park                 4.36   0.37
* Increased my knowledge about the program's             4.45   0.34
  topic
* Enhanced my appreciation for the National              4.27   0.36
  Park Service
Behavioral intentions (Cronbach's [alpha] = .94)   272   2.92   0.64

* Changed the way I will behave while I'm in             2.92   0.67
  this park
* Changed the way I will behave after I leave            2.92   0.61

  this park

Table 2. Description of context variables.

Context Variable, Definition, and       Mean or Frequency
 Measurement

Audience: Group size *                  Mean = 48
Number of total participants            Median = 17

Audience: Ratio of children to adults   Mostly Children = 25 (9%)
Categorized the ratio of children to    Even Distribution = 82 (31%)
adults in the audience using 4 point    Mostly Adults = 132 (49%)
scale: 1 = Mostly Children; 2 = Even    All Adults = 29 (11%)
Distribution; 3 = Mostly Adults; 4 =
All Adults.

Program: Duration                       Mean = 49 minutes
Duration of interpretation program
defined by time in minutes.

Program Topic                           Natural = 170 (63%)
Nature-focused, culturally-focused,     Cultural = 70 (26%)
or dual focus.                          Dual Focus = 29 (11%)

Program Type                            Guided Walk/Tour = 161 (59%)
Guided Walk/Tour, Activity,             Activity = 8 (3%)
Demonstration, or talk/slideshow/       Demonstration = 5 (2%)
presentation                            Talk/slideshow/presentation
                                        = 98 (36%)

Setting: Urban-Remote                   Urban = 91 programs (33%)
Parks were categorized as urban         Urban-proximate = 50
(within the limits of metropolitan      programs (18%)
areas with < 50,000 residents), urban   Remote = 131 programs (48%)
proximate (outside urban area, but
within a 60 mile radius), or remote
(60 miles or more from any
metropolitan area).

Setting: Location                       Indoors = 55 (20%)
Indoors, outdoors, or both.             Outdoors = 195 (72%)
                                        Both Inside and Outside = 22
                                        (8%)

Resource quality                        Mean = 2.37
Degree to which the resource where      Iconic or grandiose = 134
the program took place is awe           (49%)
inspiring or particularly iconic: 1 =   Pleasant but not iconic =
Unimpressive/generic; 2 = Pleasant     104 (38%)
but not iconic; or 3 =  Contextually    Unimpressive/generic = 34
iconic or grandiose.                    (13%)

Intervening Variable: Unexpected        Bad Weather = 9 (3%)
negative event                          Negative events = 34 (13%)
Any unexpected interruptions or
emergencies during the program, such
as a sudden change in weather,
medical emergency, technical
difficulties, or hazardous conditions
that detracted from the quality of
the program: 1 = Occurred; 0 = No
Issues.

Intervening Variable: Unexpected        Positive events = 5 (2%)
positive event
An unexpected experience that
occurred during the program, such as
seeing charismatic wildlife or other
unique phenomena that added
significantly to the quality of the
experience: 1 = Occurred; 0 = Did not
occur.

* Analyses pertaining to group size used all 312 valid programs.
Because we deemed programs with 5 or more attendees (n = 272) to be
different phenomena from programs with 5 or less attendees (n = 40),
all analyses pertaining to the other context variables used the
sample of programs with 5 or more attendees.

Table 3. Correlation coefficients for programs with mostly adult
audiences (n = 161) vs. those containing an equal or larger
proportion of children (n = 107).

                                                Visitor experience
Characteristic               Satisfaction        and appreciation

                           Children     Adult     Children   Adult
Interpreter
characteristics

Audibility                  .317 **    .104       .290 **    .005
Authentic emotion and       .450 **    .403 **    .410 **    .199 *
charisma

Confidence                  .523 **    .455 **    .386 **    .186 *
False assumption about     -.167      -.184 *    -.258 **   -.179 *
audience

Humor quality               .313 **    .263 **    .382 **    .099

Humor quantity              .184       .100       .236 *    -.043
Personal sharing            .097      -.001       .174      -.068
Responsiveness              .302 **    .195 *     .267 *     .208 **

Program characteristics

Appropriate for audience    .404 **    .267 **    .397 **    .313 **

Appropriate logistics       .317 **    .038       .396 **    .055
Clear message               .312 **    .229 **    .274 **    .101
Connection                  .403 **    .308 **    .350 **    .180 *
Consistency                 .374 **    .178 *     .316 **    .223 **
Multisensory engagement     .182       .240 **    .072       .169 *
Novelty                     .213 *     .080       .090      -.042
Organization                .380 **    .359 **    .278 **    .177 *
Physical engagement         .075       .078       .214 *     .029
Surprise                    .201 *     .101       .193 *     .116
Verbal engagement           .230 *     .227 **    .265 **    .192 *

                           Behavioral
Characteristic             intentions

                           Children   Adult
Interpreter
characteristics

Audibility                  .215 *     .034
Authentic emotion and       .203 *     .192 *
charisma

Confidence                  .336 **    .096
False assumption about     -.139      -.036
audience

Humor quality               .199 *     .135

Humor quantity              .099       .044
Personal sharing            .235 *     .101
Responsiveness              .000       .087

Program characteristics

Appropriate for audience    .365 **    .039

Appropriate logistics       .279 **    .104
Clear message               .302 **    .167 *
Connection                  .153       .141
Consistency                 .028       .064
Multisensory engagement     .107       .134
Novelty                    -.066       .085
Organization                .122       .167 *
Physical engagement         .187      -.001
Surprise                    .104       .142
Verbal engagement           .162       .170 *

** Significant at p < 0.01

*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 4. T-tests for programs with mostly children vs. mostly
adult audiences.

                                      Satisfaction

                              Children           Adult

                          Mean              Mean
Program characteristics   diff.      t      diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.52   -2.8 **   -0.25   -2.3 *
Appropriate pace           0.73    4.2 **    0.41    3.1 **

                          Visitor experience
                           and appreciation

                              Children           Adult

                          Mean              Mean
Program characteristics   diff.      t      diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.24   -2.5 *   -0.06    -1.1
Appropriate pace           0.25    2.6 *    0.18     2.9 **

                          Behavioral intentions

                          Children        Adult

                          Mean              Mean
Program characteristics   diff.     t       diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.21   -1.6     -0.07    -0.6
Appropriate pace           0.35    2.2 *    0.19     1.3

** Significant at p [less than or equal to] 0.01

*  Significant at p [less than or equal to] 0.05

Table 5. Correlation coefficients for natural (n = 170) vs. cultural
programs (n = 70).
                                                 Visitor experience
                                Satisfaction      and appreciation

Characteristic             Natural    Cultural   Natural   Cultural

Interpreter
characteristics
  charisma
Audibility                 .029       .221 **    .014      .190 *
Authentic emotion and      .440 **    .394 **    .294 *    .316 **

Confidence                 .503 **    .437 **    .297 *    .270 **
False assumption about    -.368 **   -.040      -.273 *   -.133
  audience
Humor quality              .202       .277 **    .150      .248 **
Humor quantity            -.024       .217 **   -.093      .198 **
Responsiveness             .207       .208 *     .319 **   .213 *
Program characteristics
Appropriate for the        .458 **    .355 **    .492 **   .351 **
  audience
Appropriate logistics      .286 *     .115       .222      .247 **
Clear message              .310 **    .243 **    .212      .201 **
Connection                 .335 **    .360 **    .311 **   .288 **
Consistency                .302 *     .271 **    .319 **   .253 **
Multisensory engagement    .282 *     .244 **    .245 *    .109
Novelty                    .261 *     .111       .147     -.069
Organization               .266 *     .431 **    .276 *    .247 **
Sarcasm                   -.068       .128      -.083      .074
Surprise                   .174       .130       .161      .134
Verbal engagement          .290 *     .212 **    .457 **   .177 *

/stop
                            Behavioral intentions

Characteristic              Natural    Cultural

Interpreter
characteristics
  charisma
Audibility                   .056       .120
Authentic emotion and        .291 *     .070

Confidence                   .330 **    .112
False assumption about      -.206      -.041
  audience
Humor quality                .204       .131
Humor quantity              -.033       .039
Responsiveness               .035       .015
Program characteristics
Appropriate for the          .269 *     .122
  audience
Appropriate logistics        .252 *     .156 *
Clear message                .186       .128
Connection                   .215       .090
Consistency                  .131       .045
Multisensory engagement      .183       .031
Novelty                     -.029      -.009
Organization                 .190       .128
Sarcasm                     -.322 **   -.049
Surprise                     .261 *     .041
Verbal engagement            .247 *     .089

** Significant at p < 0.01

*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 6. T-tests for cultural vs. natural programs.

                                  Satisfaction

                          Cultural         Natural

                          Mean             Mean
Program characteristics   diff.     t      diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.34   -2.6 *   -0.31   -2.1 *
Appropriate pace           0.52    3.8 **   0.46    2.4 *
Use of props               0.07    0.5      0.13    1.0

                          Visitor experience
                          and appreciation

                          Cultural          Natural

                          Mean           Mean
Program characteristics   diff.    t     diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.11   -1.9   -0.11   -1.3
Appropriate pace           0.17    2.5*   0.11    2.2 *
Use of props               0.01    0.1    0.17    2.2 *

                          Behavioral intentions

                          Cultural          Natural

                          Mean            Mean
Program characteristics   diff.     t     diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      0.01    0.1     -0.30   -1.9
Appropriate pace          0.29    2.1 *    0.11    0.5
Use of props              0.02    0.2     -0.04   -0.2

** Significant at p < 0.01

*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 7. Correlation coefficients for programs that took place in urban
(n = 91) vs. remote parks (n = 131).

                                                  Visitor experience
Characteristic               Satisfaction         and appreciation

                             Urban     Remote     Urban     Remote
Interpreter characteristics

Audibility                  .238 *     .159       .267 *     .043
Authentic emotion and       .415 **    .432 **   -.352 **    .280 **
  charisma
Confidence                  .453 **    .519 **    .264 *     .294 **
False assumption about     -.096      -.308 **   -.189      -.259 **
  audience
Formality                  -.0 46     -.132      -.259 *    -.086
Humor quality               .373 **    .275 **    .355 **    .207 *

Humor quantity              .355 **   -.019       .372 **   -.061
Personal sharing           -.027       .060       .073       .044
Responsiveness              .230       .235 **    .213       .304 **

Program characteristics

Appopriate for the          .371 **    .366 **    .391 **    .344 **
  audience
Appropriate logistics       .186       .162       .307 **    .240 **
Clear message               .285 **    .250 **    .267 *     .201 *
Connnction                  .394 **    .264 **    .270 *     .285 **
Consistency                 .385 **    .300 **    .347 **    .353 **

Multisensory engagement     .316 **    .076       .076       .066
Novelty                     .276 **    .084       .127      -.082
Organization                .466 **    .307 **    .239 *     .245 **

Sarcasm                     .290 **    .007       .259 *    -.070

Surprse                     .109       .197 *     .068       .190 *
Verbal engagement           .285 **    .190 *     .279 **    .199 *

                             Behavioral
Characteristic               intentions

                             Urban    Remote
Interpreter characteristics

Audibility                    .163      .000
Authentic emotion and         .069      .262 **
  charisma
Confidence                    .191      .265 **
False assumption about       -.039     -.176 *
  audience
Formality                     .100     -.039
Humor quality                 .198      .141

Humor quantity                .163     -.027
Personal sharing             -.024      .107
Responsiveness                .123      .120

Program characteristics

Appopriate for the            .165      .233 **
  audience
Appropriate logistics         .233 *    .167
Clear message                 .107      .202 *
Connnction                    .080      .154
Consistency                   .095      .022

Multisensory engagement       .047      .194 *
Novelty                      -.025     -.077
Organization                  .178      .148

Sarcasm                       .051     -.214 *

Surprse                      -.150      .278 **
Verbal engagement             .047      .147

** Significant at p < 0.01
*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 8. T-tests for programs that took place in urban vs. remote parks.

                          Satisfaction

                               Urban             Remote

                          Mean              Mean
Program characteristics   diff.      t      diff.      t

Fact-based messaging      -0.57   -3.5 **   -0.35   -3.0 **

Appropriate pace           0.46    2.2 *     0.43    3.4 **

                          Visitor experience
                          and appreciation

                               Urban           Remote

                          Mean              Mean
Program characteristics   diff.      t      diff.      t

Fact-based messaging      -0.23   -2.5 *    -0.10   -1.5

Appropriate pace           0.19    1.8       0.23    3.2 **

                          Behavioral intentions

                              Urban          Remote

                          Mean           Mean
Program characteristics   diff.    t     diff.    t

Fact-based messaging      -0.06   -0.4   -0.21   -1.8

Appropriate pace           0.39    1.9    0.14    1.1

** Significant at p < 0.01

*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 9. Correlation coefficients for indoor (n = 55) vs. outdoor
(n = 195) programs.

                                                   Visitor experience
Characteristic                     Satisfaction     and appreciation

                               Indoor    Outdoor   Indoor    Outdoor

Interpreter characteristics
Audibility                      .052      .236 **    .254      .152 *
Authentic emotion and           .284 *    .442 **    .221      .266 **
charisma

Confidence                      .273 *    .551 **    .093      .337 **
False assumption about         -.278 *   -.163 *    -.302 *   -.189 *
audience
Humor quality                   .145      .330 **    .092      .222 **
Responsiveness                  .284      .194 **    .183      .195 **

Program characteristics
Appropriate for the audience    .330 *    .375 **    .214      .368 **
Appropriate logistics           .284 *    .118       .427 **   .148 *
Clear message                   .345 *    .217 **    .124      .116

Consistency                     .125      .290 **   -.080      .338 **
Connection                      .286 *    .332 **    .117      .242 **

Multisensory engagement         .145      .196 *    -.188      .113
Novelty                         .045      .192 **   -.164      .068

Organization                    .273 *    .385 **   -.098      .297 **

Physical engagement            -.266 *    .120      -.296 *    .141 *
Sarcasm                         .068      .098      -.078      .043
Surprise                        .063      .174 *    -.013      .179 *
Verbal engagement               .025      .228 **   -.008      .182 *

                               Behavioral
Characteristic                 intentions

                               Indoor   Outdoor

Interpreter characteristics
Audibility                      .134      .097
Authentic emotion and           .119      .180 *
charisma

Confidence                      .017      .199 **
False assumption about         -.049     -.103
audience
Humor quality                   .115      .132
Responsiveness                  .049      .037

Program characteristics
Appropriate for the audience    .149      .112
Appropriate logistics           .190      .126
Clear message                   .279 *    .131

Consistency                    -.099      .041
Connection                      .248      .055

Multisensory engagement        -.107      .178 *
Novelty                        -.054      .024

Organization                    .001      .142 *

Physical engagement            -.125      .080
Sarcasm                        -.003     -.210 **
Surprise                        .047      .141 *
Verbal engagement               .023      .139

** Significant at p < 0.01
*  Significant at p < 0.05

Table 10. T-tests for indoor (n = 55) vs. outdoor (n = 195) programs.

                                  Satisfaction

                          Indoor           Outdoor

                          Mean             Mean
Program characteristics   diff.     t      diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.58   -2.5 *   -0.18   -1.7

Appropriate pace          .0.36    1.3      0.61    5.2 **

                          Visitor experience
                          and appreciation

                             Indoor         Outdoor

                          Mean           Mean
Program characteristics   diff.    t     diff.      t

Fact-based messaging      -0.20   -1.7   -0.01    -0.3

Appropriate pace           0.14    0.9    0.22     3.9 **

                          Behavioral intentions

                          Indoor         Outdoor

                          Mean           Mean
Program characteristics   diff.    t     diff.     t

Fact-based messaging      -0.32   -1.6   -0.03   -0.3

Appropriate pace          -0.1    -0.3    0.25    2.1 *

** Significant at p < 0.01

* Significant at p < 0.05

Table 11. Program and interpreter characteristics with different
relationships to outcomes in different contexts.

                                     Visitor          Behavioral
Context            Satisfaction      Experience and   Intentions
                                     Appreciation

More children in   Appropriate       Appropriate
the audience       logistics (+)     logistics (+)
                   Audibility (+)    Audibility       Confidence (+)
                                     (+)
                                     Humor quality    Appropriate
                                     (+)              for audience
                                     Humor            (+)
                                     quantity (+)

Nature-focused     False             False            Sarcasm (-)
programs           assumption        assumption
                   about the         about the
                   audience (-)      audience(-)

Culturally-        Humor quantity    Humor
focused programs   (+)               quantity (+)

Urban parks        Audibility (+)    Audibility
                   Sarcasm (+)       (+) Sarcasm
                   Humor quantity    (+) Humor
                   (+)               quantity (+)
                   Multisensory
                   (+)

Remote parks                         Appropriate      Surprise (+)
                                     pace (+)         Sarcasm (-)

Indoor programs    Physical          Physical
                   engagement(-)     engagement(-)

Outdoor programs   Physical          Confidence (+)   Multisensory
                   engagement (+)    Consistency      (+)
                   Appropriate       (+)
                   pace (+)          Consistency
                                     (+)
                                     Organization
                                     (+)
                                     Appropriate
                                     pace (+)
                                     (+)
                                     Physical
                                     engagement
                                     (+)
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有