Comparative effectiveness of phonological awareness and oral language intervention for children with low emergent literacy skills.
Fielding-Barnsley, Ruth ; Hay, Ian
Introduction
Early identification of reading difficulties is paramount if
teachers are to help change the current situation where 16% of children
are falling into the category of 'disabled readers' in
Australia. (Louden et al., 2000). In the United States, The National
Assessment for Educational Progress (NAEP, 1998) identified 60% of
students scoring below proficient in fourth grade. Early identification
and the resulting remediation of reading difficulties have many
advantages over leaving children to 'grow out' of their
problems (Pressley, 2006; Torgesen, 1998). One such advantage is
reducing the children's negative feelings associated with their
constant failure in the classroom (Hay, 2000; Muter, 2003). Muter
maintained that early identification also allowed for a
'purer' child profile to be established, because older
children often acquired compensatory and avoidance behaviours that can
mask their reading difficulties.
It has been demonstrated that there is a strong link between
phonological awareness and early reading. Children's awareness of
the phonological units of speech, particularly rhyme (Bradley &
Bryant, 1983) and alliteration (Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1995)
have been found to have significant effects for early reading. Knowledge
of the alphabet is also one of the best predictors for a successful
transition into early reading (Adams, 1990).
Whilst there has been a plethora of research on the benefits of
phonological awareness intervention for struggling readers (see Lovett,
Steinbach & Frijters (2000) for a summary), there has been
significantly less on the effects of language intervention (Muter,
Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004). The NICHD (2005) suggests that
there is a vital link between early oral language and decoding skills.
While phonological awareness, the understanding of the sound structure
of language, is necessary, it alone is not sufficient for the successful
acquisition of reading (Byrne, Fielding-Barnsley & Ashley, 2000).
Hay, Elias, Fielding-Barnsley, Homel, and Frieberg, (2007) demonstrated
that the inclusion of a structured language programme along with a
shorter phonological awareness programme produced significant gains in
children's reading ability; particularly if those children came
from low (socio-economic status) SES homes. Children who fail to respond
to reading intervention are often those with weak oral language (Al
Otaiba & Fuchs, 2006). Hart and Risley (1995) made the link between
weak oral language and low SES in their seminal study where it was found
that children from high socio-economic status (SES) families heard
around 487 utterances per hour, compared to 178 utterances per hour for
children from families on welfare. By the time they were aged 4 years,
the high SES children had been exposed to around 44 million utterances,
compared to 12 million utterances for the lower SES children.
In particular, the evidence suggests that many children with
significant reading and learning difficulties/disabilities have deficits
in both phonological awareness and language skills (Saada-Robert, 2004;
Snowling, 2005). Whilst language delays are considered a cause of
reading delays, the children's lack of reading skills also have an
ongoing negative influence on the children's vocabulary and
language development (Catts & Kamhi, 2005). This reciprocal
relationship between language and reading has significant implications
for the type and range of screening and interventions teachers provide
to children in the beginning school years.
Several researchers have reported on the beneficial effects of
language instruction on measures of comprehension (Oakhill, Cain, &
Bryant, 2003; Muter et al., 2004) and more recently Bowyer-Crane et al.
(2008) have reported differential effects between an oral language and
phonology with reading based intervention. In the Boyer-Crane et al.,
study the Phonology+Reading group showed an advantage over the Oral
Language group on measures of literacy and phonology and the Oral
Language group showed an advantage on measures of vocabulary and
grammatical skills.
Whilst Hay et al. (2007) have demonstrated that a language plus
phonological awareness intervention has enhanced decoding (reading)
scores we do not know if the intervention has the same effect on
measures of encoding (spelling). A recent study concluded that whilst a
language and phonological awareness intervention delivered in pre-school
demonstrated positive short term effects it did not generalise to
measures of encoding (spelling) in Grade 2 (O' Connor, Arnott,
McIntosh & Dodd, 2009).
In the following study the terms 'decoding' for early
reading and 'encoding' for spelling are used interchangeably.
The term 'decoding' is understood to describe the process of
changing written letters into spoken sounds to make meaning of written
text; or 'breaking the code'. 'Encoding' is the
exact mirror image of this process and is understood to describe the
process of changing spoken language into the symbols of written
language. Both processes are used to discover the alphabetic principle
and therefore valid measures of early reading.
Research questions
The research questions are driven by the theory, particularly by
the concept that phonological awareness may be underpinned by oral
language skills (Burgess & Lonigan, 1998) and phonological
processing skills, such as encoding (spelling) may be largely dependent
on the development of good oral language skills (Storch &
Whitehurst, 2002). This research therefore asks the question, will an
oral language intervention result in similar gains to phonological
intervention on a measure of spelling?
The second research question aims to build on the research by Hart
and Risley (1995) who link low oral language with SES and Hay et al.
(2007) who demonstrated that a phonological awareness with oral language
intervention resulted in increased literacy growth for children from low
SES backgrounds. The research aims to answer specifically the influence
of parents' educational and family literacy levels on outcome
measures of early literacy, namely spelling.
Method
Participants
Participants were 457 students from Year One in 9 schools varying
in Socio Economic Status (SES) in the Brisbane region of Queensland,
Australia. Two schools were classified as high SES, five schools as
average (middle) SES and two schools as low SES as determined by
Australian Government income census data. Two hundred and thirteen
students were female and 244 were male. Their mean chronological age in
months at the time of screening was 70.6 (SD = 4.01). Children with a
known intellectual disability were excluded from the study.
Approval for this study was received from the relevant university
ethics committee, school authorities, as well as the principals,
children's teachers, parents and caregivers.
Design and procedure
All 457 children received a battery of tests in the second term of
their first year of formal schooling (Year 1 in Queensland). As a result
they had received some literacy instruction. The children were settled
and familiar with classroom routines. Children were tested individually
in their schools, in a quiet environment, by trained research assistants
who were also registered teachers. During the 1st round of screening all
children received a letter knowledge and rhyme detection test in one
session lasting approximately 30 minutes. Measures at the level of the
phoneme were not administered due to floor effects in a pilot test. The
tests were administered in the same order for each child.
During the 2nd round of screening, from the population of N = 457,a
second cohort of 141 children was selected based on their alphabet and
rhyme scores and the need to representatively sample across all of the
schools involved in the project. The 141 of children (31%) who scored at
or below the cut off scores of < (less than) or= to 15/26 for
alphabet recognition and <or = to 5/10 for rhyme detection were then
tested with an expressive and receptive vocabulary test in one session
lasting approximately 45 minutes. The cut off scores were selected to
identify children potentially at risk of developing difficulties in
early literacy acquisition. The rhyme detection test was constructed in
a way that allowed for children to score 3 correct answers by chance
therefore a score of 5/10 was actually very low. The tests were
administered in the same order for each child, with speed of word naming
collected while completing the expressive vocabulary measure. The
parent/s of each child also completed a family reading survey relating
to each child in the study.
The sample of children selected for intervention was further
reduced by taking their expressive and receptive language scores into
consideration. Only those children who scored <or = to 70/100 on the
HPNT or <or = to 70 (Standardised score) on the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test (PPVT) were included in the intervention programme.
Therefore only children who were identified as being at risk in both
phonological awareness and language were included in the intervention
sample. This resulted in a total of 96 children, 21% of the original
sample of 457 children. The rationale for the cut off points for all
pre-test measures was to have a cohort who would be an approximate
representation of the Louden et al. (2000) study where 16% of the sample
fell into the category of 'disabled readers'.
A group of 29 at-risk children acted as a classroom control and
received no intervention. A group of 31 not at-risk children acted as
class controls, 2 children were randomly selected from each class. This
resulted in a total of 156 participants in the study who received all
pre and post test measures. In addition 301 children from the original
sample were included in the measure of spelling.
A measure of encoding (spelling) was selected as a proxy for early
reading based on the findings of Morris and Perney (1984) who
illustrated the strong relationship between early encoding (spelling)
and reading. Also we gained much richer data from a partial scoring
system adapted from a version by Liberman, Rubin, Duques and Carlisle
(1985). Whereas a measure of reading is usually scored as being either
correct or incorrect, this measure of encoding (spelling) awarded scores
for partially correct encoding. (Please refer to the post testing for a
full description of this test.)
Instruments
Tests Administered to all Children (N= 457)
Screening 1st round--The Phonological Abilities Test (PAT) (Muter,
Hulme & Snowling, 1997).
The Phonological Abilities Test (PAT) consists of a number of
phonological awareness subtests of which rhyme detection and a test of
letter knowledge were selected for this study due to floor effects at
the level of the phoneme.
Letter Knowledge: In this test the children were asked to supply
either the name or the sound of each of the 26 lowercase letters of the
alphabet, which were presented in random order on individual cards.
Scoring was based on the total number of letters identified, whether by
name or by sound.
Rhyme detection: The 10-item rhyme detection subtest from the PAT
was used to measure children's ability to detect rhyme in spoken
words. There were two practice items and corrective feedback was given
for the first four items. For the ten items with pictures the children
were required to indicate which of three words rhymed with a named
stimulus word (e.g., fish, gun, hat) with (e.g., cat).
Tests administered to at risk children
Vocabulary--Receptive
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn,
1981). The PPVT- R is a standardised measure of receptive vocabulary
skills. Participants are presented with an easel page that contains four
numbered pictures. The goal of this task is to select the picture that
matches the word that was orally presented by the administrator. The
test manual reports internal consistency coefficients that range from
0.67 to 0.88 (median = 0.80) for Form L and from 0.62 to 0.86 (M = 0.81)
for Form M. Immediate retest alternate from reliability coefficients
range from 0.73 to 0.91 (M = 0.82) and delayed retest alternate form
reliability coefficient range from 0.52 to 0.90 (M = 0.78).
Vocabulary--Expressive
Hundred Picture Naming Test (HPNT) (Fisher & Glennister, 1992).
The HPNT was used to measure expressive vocabulary. This test consists
of 100 line drawings of everyday objects such as: butterfly, scissors
and kite. The participants are asked to name each of the items and this
test is also timed. Test occasion correlation = 0.83.
Stony Brook Family Reading Survey (Whitehurst, 1993). This adapted
questionnaire (Fielding-Barnsley & Purdie, 2002) consists of 6
questions relating to each family's reading related activities in
the home and 2 questions related to each parent's level of
education. Examples of reading related questions are: How many picture
books do you have in your home and how often does your child ask to be
read to? Each question has a range of five possible answers.
Intervention
The intervention sessions consisted of 16 x 30 minute lessons over
a period of 8 weeks. The lessons were conducted by trained research
assistants in small groups of 4 to 5 children outside of their regular
classrooms.
Phonological awareness was taught using Hatcher's (2000)
'Sound Linkage', an integrated programme for overcoming
reading difficulties and Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley's (1991)
'Sound Foundations' Programme.
Forty-nine children at 5 of the 9 schools received this
intervention. The intervention was comprised of systematic and explicit
instruction in phonological awareness beginning with understanding the
terms beginning, middle and end, followed by learning about syllables,
rhyme and phonemes. These activities enable the instructors to work
directly on improving children's ability to analyse the sounds of
speech, and to relate this knowledge to the process of learning to read
and spell. 'Sound Foundations' was used to complement
instruction in phonemic awareness. This kit consists of large posters
depicting words beginning or ending with the same sound, for example the
beginning /s/ poster depicts the sun, sea, seal, seahorse etc., and the
ending /s/ poster depicts a bus, glass, octopus etc. The snap and domino
card games included in Sound Foundations were also used to reinforce
this learning. The alphabet was not included in this instruction as this
was being taught in the classroom.
Forty seven children at 5 of the 9 schools received Marion
Blanks' four levels of dialogue instruction. Blank's four
levels of dialogue are: (i) dealing with directly supplied information
(matching the experiences); (ii) classification (selective analysis of
the experience); (iii) reorganisation (reordering the experience); and
(iv) abstraction and inference (reasoning about the experience. Marion
Blank and her colleagues (Blank & Franklin, 1980; Blank, Rose, &
Berlin, 2003) have proposed four levels of dialogue complexity, where
the children are active participants in the learning interchanges. In
such a communicative context the teacher initiates and shapes the
dialogue so that the students respond at a more appropriate and
advancing level of linguistic complexity.
Referring to Table 1 which identifies the four levels and the
sub-elements, at the lowest level of complexity or first level, the
student is required to respond to language concerning salient
perceptions (e.g., to the question, what is this?), with a focus on
vocabulary development, moving to the second level of the organisation
of information stage where the key questions investigate how objects,
events, or issues are classified. This organisation and classifying of
the information particularly helps facilitate students' encoding
and retrieval of information into and from their long-term memory. This
skill is particularly relevant to the current study where the children
are demonstrating their ability to retrieve information in order to
encode (spell) words. The third level is focussed on reorganising or
adding to the information, based on what an individual already knows of
the topic, which is the linking of the information to higher order
reasoning (e.g., what else do you know about?). This level of dialogue
relates to the current study where the children use their knowledge of
orthography to recognise similarities in word families. The fourth level
deals with the abstract, and at this level the language demands involve
reflecting upon or restructuring perceptions (e.g., the question, what
do you think will happen if ...?). Again this level of dialogue assists
the children in the current study to utilise their knowledge of phonemic
awareness in for example deleting the /s/ sound in nest to produce the
word net.
Post intervention testing
A total of 156 children received the full Phonological Abilities
Test (PAT) and the Hundred Picture Naming Test (HPNT). In addition, all
children plus the remaining 301 children in all classes received the
Fielding-Barnsley/ Hay spelling test. The 156 children comprised 49 from
the phonological awareness intervention, 47 from the oral language
intervention, 29 classroom at-risk controls and 31 not at-risk classroom
controls.
The PAT consists of six subtests: four tests of phonological
awareness (two rhyming and two segmentation tasks), a test of speech
rate and a test of letter knowledge. The six tests are:
1. Rhyme detection which requires the child to select the word
which rhymes with the stimulus word for an array of three choices.
2. Rhyme production in which the child is given 30 seconds in which
to produce as many rhyming responses as possible to a stimulus word.
3. Word completion (syllables and phonemes) in which the child
'finishes off' the final syllable or phoneme in a series of
one or two syllable words.
4. Phoneme deletion (beginning and end sounds) in which the child
removes or deletes either the first or final phoneme of a single
syllable word.
5. Speech rate which requires the child to repeat the word
'buttercup' 10 times as quickly as possible while the examiner
records the time taken.
6. Letter knowledge in which the child is asked to supply either
the name or sound for each of the twenty- six letters of the alphabet.
The Fielding-Barnsley/ Hay (2006) spelling test and scoring system
are amended versions of the Liberman, Rubin, Duques, and Carlisle (1985)
spelling test. The children were asked to spell 10 words in writing
(dog, man, one, said, blue, come, plug, went, limp, tree) and 4
pseudowords (ig, sut, frot, yilt) presented in the same order. The words
were said once, repeated in a sentence, and spoken a third time. The
pseudowords were spoken three times. A correct spelling, such as
'come' earns the maximum of 6 points. Letter reversals were
ignored. The rest of the scoring system is based on the number of
phonemes represented; the scoring system values accurate segmentation. A
spelling that represents all phonemes with conventionally acceptable
letters, such as 'cum' or 'kum' for come earns 5
points, and one that represents all phonemes but includes more distantly
related letters such as 'cam', is given 4 points. If the
attempt includes more than one but not all phonemes, such as
'cm' or 'ku' for 'come' or 'pug'
for 'plug', earns 3 points. One phoneme with a conventional
letter, such as k, is given 2 points, and one phoneme with a related
letter, such as g or n for come, earns 1 point.
Results
The full cohort of children was first investigated for gender
differences in rhyme and alphabet knowledge, with outcomes not
significantly influenced by gender (rhyme F (1,442) = 1.58, sig = 0.209;
alphabet F (1,450) = 0.162, sig = .687).
Correlations of language and early indicators of reading
Investigating the relationship between the early literacy
variables, the highest correlation was r = 0.68 between expressive and
receptive language (HPNT and PPVT), see Table 2. There are also high
inter-correlations between the children's alphabet knowledge and
their expressive and receptive language scores. Unlike alphabet
knowledge, rhyme did not correlate with expressive and receptive
language, however, there was a significant moderate correlation between
children's alphabet and rhyme knowledge, r = 0.32. The
children's speed of word naming on the HPNT was negatively
correlated with both alphabet knowledge and the language measures,
demonstrating that children who achieved higher scores on alphabet and
language measures took less time to respond to the HPNT pictures, that
is they had a higher level of automaticity associated with the naming of
objects. For example, based on the obtained plotted regression square
line of 0.05 an alphabet score of 19 equals 200 seconds, and an alphabet
score of 5 equals 800 seconds. This finding demonstrates that if a word
is in the child's lexicon or long term memory it is retrieved more
quickly.
Home reading environment
Children's abilities to do the rhyme tasks were uncorrelated
with their home reading behaviours. In contrast, children's
alphabet knowledge was highly associated with home book reading as was
children's receptive and expressive language development. The
highest correlations were associated with the children's alphabet
knowledge, the amount of home reading, and the years of education of the
children's primary caregivers, see Table 3.
Intervention outcome using the Fielding-Barnsley/ Hay (2006)
spelling results
In the first of the following set of analyses of variance (ANOVAs),
a measure of children's spelling results was entered as the outcome
variable, and type of intervention (phonological, or language) plus two
relevant control groups (class control not at risk or at-risk control)
was entered as the predictor variable.
The ANOVA showed a significant main effect for group (F(3,
115)=4.757, p<0.01). Group differences in relation to the spelling
outcome are shown in Table 4.
As indicated in Table 4, the phonological intervention group did
better than the language experimental group and significantly better
than the at-risk control group (p<0.01).
A series of follow-up ANCOVAs (analysis of covariance &
variance) was utilised to examine the effect of various pre-existing
conditions that might be deemed to be influential. This analytic
approach is equivalent to combining analysis of variance and regression
approaches within a single procedure. The question of interest is
whether type of intervention is still statistically influential under
these conditions.
The first of these ANCOVAs included pre-test measures of rhyming
knowledge and alphabetical knowledge as covariates. Under these
conditions, alphabet knowledge (F(1,113)=32.269) and group
(F(3,113)=2.845, p<0.5) both predicted FBH spelling outcomes at
statistically significant levels, while the effect of rhyming knowledge
was statistically non-significant.
With pre-existing rhyming and alphabet knowledge taken into
account, the phonological intervention surpassed other treatment groups
and did significantly better than the at-risk control group (p<0.05).
Family background
The effect of family background was examined by including two
Family Reading Survey (FRS) items that measured parents' years of
schooling (Secondary, Post-secondary) completed by the two spouses, and
school SES as covariates in an ANCOVA. The outcomes suggested that
neither years of school nor SES predict spelling outcomes. Under these
conditions, the effect group was marginally significant (p=0.062).
The effect of home reading was examined by an ANCOVA that included
a covariate that combined four FRS items related to the frequency with
family members read picture books to the child, how often the child
asked to be read to, how often that child looked at books by
him/herself, and how often the adult accompanied the child to the
library. As with family background, this measure of pre-existing
literacy predicted spelling outcomes at non-significant levels, in
contrast with type of intervention (F(2,49)=3.939, p<0.05), where
either phonological or experimental language participants did better
than those in the at-risk control group (p<0.05).
Discussion
The overall finding is that the most significant differences in
measures of spelling are related to the type of intervention rather than
factors related with family literacy practices or parents'
education levels. This concurs with the research conducted by Noble,
Farah and McCandliss (2006) who stated that 'regardless of
background children with higher phonological awareness abilities are
decoding successfully' (p. 140) and Van Steensel (2006) and White
(1982) who both argued that it was the social, language and literacy
enhancement associated with higher SES homes that were the important
factors; not the parental occupations or family income. This finding has
important ramifications for early childhood educators who may need to
adjust their teaching programmes to cater for these differences.
The low literacy levels at school entry reported in the current
study are certainly cause for concern and should alert teachers to the
fact that these children will require systematic and explicit teaching
of phonological awareness and language in order to overcome any
potential risks associated with low levels of pre-literacy skills at
school entry. The message is that these children are capable of closing
the gap between being a poor reader and a good reader, or in this case
between being poor spellers and good spellers. It must be emphasised
that it is the intervention that has the most significant effect rather
than the family background or family literacy practices.
It is critical that teachers are aware of the important factors,
such as knowledge of the alphabet, that do have a significant impact on
measures of literacy (Adams, 1990). The current study revealed that
alphabet knowledge was significantly correlated with the measures of
spelling. However, knowledge of rhyme was not significantly correlated
with measures of spelling. Other studies have also reported
non-significant results for knowledge of rhyme and outcome measures of
reading (Muter et al., 2004).
The current study supports other studies (Byrne &
Fielding-Barnsley, 1995) in the finding that phonological awareness
intervention supports early measures of reading and spelling but what is
not so commonly known is the effect of early language intervention on
measures of early literacy such as spelling. The current study supports
the findings of Burgess and Lonigan (1998) and Storch and Whitehurst
(2002) that phonological awareness should be combined with early
language intervention. We know that phonological awareness and language
support each other (Hay et al., 2007; Nation & Snowling, 2004) and
that both are necessary in order to support early readers; particularly
those at risk due to entering school with low levels of language. It has
also been noted that oral language supports 'learning to
learn' (NICHD, 2005) and this could explain why the oral language
intervention in this study was almost as effective as the phonological
awareness intervention. It could be hypothesised that some children
cannot access the language related to instruction in phonological
awareness such as, 'what is the beginning sound in this word?'
The potential interference of semantic complexity of instruction is
clarified in Marion Blank's oral language intervention. Speece and
Cooper (2002) have provided evidence that language training predicts
growth of phonological skills in kindergarten children. They concluded
that the ability to understand the meaning component of language is a
significant indicator of single-word reading in first grade. The
emphasis on vocabulary growth in the oral language component of the
current study supports the hypothesis that oral language supports the
development of early literacy skills such as spelling. A similar study
(Hill & Launder, 2010) failed to demonstrate a relationship between
oral vocabulary and reading achievement but it could be suggested that
they may have found a correlation had they used a measure of spelling as
demonstrated in the current study.
Whilst there remain several questions as to the importance of oral
language and its relationship to beginning reading we know that oral
language is a better predictor of comprehension in later years and that
phonological awareness predicts decoding (Bishop & Snowling, 2004).
It can therefore be deduced that both skills are important to the
ultimate goal of reading for understanding and should be part of the
early teaching repertoire.
Limitations
One of the limitations of this study was that the 2 interventions
were not combined, that is that each intervention group received either
the phonological awareness intervention or the language intervention so
we are unable to confirm that phonological awareness and language may
have a multiplicative effect on outcome measures of spelling.
Another limitation is that the in-class control group did not
receive any intervention and therefore this could have resulted in a
Hawthorne effect for both intervention groups.
Summary
This study has highlighted the importance of explicit and
systematic instruction in phonological awareness and oral language for
children at the beginning stages of reading. Whilst this research
highlighted a group of children identified with low language and
phonological skills at school entry it does not preclude teaching these
skills to all children. Measures of spelling, especially those that
credit approximation, may be a better indicator of children's
acquisition of the alphabetic principle than reading.
Acknowledgement
This article is based on research funded by the Australian Research
Council Discovery grant DP0666577 to Professor Ian Hay, Associate
Professor Ruth Fielding Barnsley and Professor Adrian Ashman.
References
Adams. M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about
print. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2006). Who are the young children
for whom best practices in reading are ineffective: An experimental and
longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(5), 414-431.
Bishop, D., & Snowling, M. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and
specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin,
130 (6), 858-886.
Blank, M., & Franklin, E. (1980). Dialogue with preschoolers: A
cognitive-based system of assessment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, (2)
122-150.
Blank, M., Rose, S.A., & Berlin, L.J. (2003). Preschool
Language Assessment Instrument: The language of learning in practice
(2nd Ed.). Austin, TX: Pro Ed.
Bowyer-Crane, C., Snowling, M.J., Duff, F.J., Fieldsend, E.,
Carroll, J.M., Miles, J., Gotz, K., & Hulme, C. (2008). Improving
early language and literacy skills: Differential effects on an oral
language versus phonology with reading intervention. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 422-432.
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P.E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and
learning to read--a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421.
Burgess, S.R., & Lonigan, C.J. (1998). Bidirectional relation
of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a
preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70(2),
117-141
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a
program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2- and 3-year
follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology,
87(3), 488-503.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Sound Foundations.
Sydney, Australia: Peter Leyden Educational Publishers.
Byrne, B., Fielding-Barnsley. R., & Ashley, L. (2000). Effects
of preschool phoneme identity training after six years. Outcome levels
distinguished from rate of response. Journal of Educational Psychology,
92(4), 659-677.
Catts, H.W., & Kamhi, A.G. (2005). The connection between
language and reading disabilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test,
Version 3. Circles Pines, MI: American Guidance Service
Fielding-Barnsley, R., & Purdie, N. (2002). Developing
pre-literacy skills via shared book reading: The assessment of a family
intervention program for preschool children at risk of becoming reading
disabled. Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7(3), 13-19.
Fisher, J.P., & Glennister, J.M. (1992). The Hundred Pictures
Naming Test. Hawthorn: The Australian Council for Education Research.
Hart, B.H., & Risley, T.R. (1995). Meaningful differences in
the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul
H. Brookes.
Hatcher, P.J. (2000). Sound Linkage: An integrated programme for
overcoming reading difficulties (2nd ed.). London: Whurr.
Hay, I. (2000). Gender self-concept profiles of students suspended
from high school. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
Disciplines, 41, (3) 345-352.
Hay, I., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (2006). Enhancing the early
literacy development of children at risk for reading difficulties.
Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 11(3), 117-124.
Hay, I., Elias, G., Fielding-Barnsley, R., Homel, R., &
Frieberg, K. (2007). Language delays, reading delays and learning
difficulties: Interactive elements requiring multidimensional
programming, Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40 (5), 400-409.
Hill, S., & Launder, N. (2010). Oral language and beginning to
read. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 33 (3), 240-254.
Liberman, I.Y., Rubin, H., Duques, S., & and Carlisle, J.
(1985). Linguistic abilities and spelling proficiency in kindergartners
and adult poor spellers. In D.B. Gray & J.F. Kavanagh (Eds.),
Biobehavioral measures of dyslexia (pp. 163-176). Parkton, MD: York
Press.
Louden, W., Chan, L., Elkins, J., Greaves, D., House, H., Milton,
M., Nichols, S., Rohl, M., Rivalland, J., & Van Kraayenoord, C.
(2000). Mapping the territory. Canberra, Australia: Commonwealth
Department of Education, Training and Youth Affairs.
Lovett, M.W., Steinbach, K.A., & Frijters, J.C. (2000).
Remediating the core deficits of developmental reading disability: A
double deficit perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4),
334-342.
Morris, D., & Perney, J. (1984). Developmental spelling as a
predictor of first-grade reading achievement. The Elementary School
Journal, 84 (4), 440-457.
Muter, V. (2003). Early reading development and dyslexia. London:
Whurr Pub
Muter,V., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (1997). The Phonological
Abilities Test. (PAT). London. The Psychological Corporation. Harcourt
Brace & Company.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004).
Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of
early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study.
Developmental Psychology, 40 (5), 665-681.
National Assessment for Educational Progress (NAEP) (1998). Reading
report card for the nation and the states. National Center for Education
Statistics, Washington, DC.
Nation, K., & Snowling, M.J. (2004). Beyond phonological
skills: broader language skills contribute to the development of
reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27 (4) 342-356.
NICHD. Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Pathways to
reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading.
Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 428-442.
Noble, K.G., Farah, M.J., & Mc Candliss, B.D. (2006).
Socio-economic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships
in reading. Cognitive Development, 21(3), 349-368.
Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P.E. (2003). The dissociation
of word reading and text comprehension : Evidence from component skills.
Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(4) 443-468.
O'Connor, M., Arnott, W., McIntosh, B., & Dodd, B. (2009).
Phonological awareness and language intervention in preschoolers from
low socio-economic backgrounds: A longitudinal investigation. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(4), 767-782.
Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for
balanced teaching. (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Roth, F.P., Speece, D.L., & Cooper, D.H. (2002). A longitudinal
analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. The
Journal of Educational Research, 95 (5), 259-272.
Saada-Robert, M. (2004). Early emergent literacy. In T. Nunes,
& P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children's literacy (pp.
578-598). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
Snowling, M.J. (2005). Literacy outcomes for children with oral
language impairments: Developmental interactions between language skills
and learning to read. In H.W. Catts, & A.G. Kamhi (Eds.), The
connection between language and reading disabilities (pp. 55-75).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Storch, S.A., & Whitehurst, G.J. (2002). Oral language and
code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal
structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934-947
Torgesen, J.K. (1998), Catch them before they fall: Identification
and assessment to prevent reading failure in young children. American
Educator, 22, 32-39.
Van Steensel, R. (2006). Relations between socio-cultural factors,
the home literacy environment and children's literacy development
in the first years of primary education. Journal of Research in Reading,
29,4,367-382.
White, K.R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and
academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461-481.
Whitehurst, G.J. (1993). The Stony Brook Family Reading Survey.
Stony Brook. NY: Author.
Ruth Fielding-Barnsley & Ian Hay
University of Tasmania
Associate Professor Ruth Fielding-Barnsley heads the literacy team
at The University of Tasmania. Previously Ruth has lectured at
University of New England, Armidale and the Queensland University of
Technology. Ruth's research interests are in the areas of family
literacy, the acquisition of reading, teachers' knowledge of
metalinguistics, dyslexia and asperger's syndrome. She was an
advisor for the new Australian Curriculum: English.
Professor Ian Hay is the Dean of the Faculty of Education,
University of Tasmania, Australia. He has published over 100 book
chapters, refereed journal articles and other related articles, and
delivered over 100 conference presentations. His research interests
include children's reading development, students with literacy and
academic difficulties, and the role of motivation in learning.
Table 1
Four levels of language complexity and proficiency related to teacher
discourse and questioning (Blank et al., 2003)
Level of Language Complexity to the Example of Teacher
Complexity & experience Discourse
Proficiency
1 Directly Supplied Information What do you see?
(Matching experiences)
Classification
2 (Selective analysis of What colour is that?
experience)
3 Reorganisation Re-tell me the story
4 Abstraction and Inference What made it happen?
(Reasoning about experience)
Table 2
Correlations between children's rhyme, alphabet, receptive and
expressive language and speed of word naming
Rhyme Alphabet PPVT HPNT Speed
Rhyme 1
Alphabet .32 ** 1
PPVT (Receptive
Language) .02 .40 ** 1
HPNT (Expressive
Language) .11 .58 ** .69 ** 1
Speed of word
naming .07 -.24 ** -.31 ** -.33 ** 1
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
Table 3
Correlation between children's home reading environment and children's
ability with rhyme, alphabet, expressive and receptive language
Rhyme Alphabet PPVT HPNT
Level of home book reading
to child .13 .48 ** .43 ** .38 **
Started reading to your child .08 .21 * .33 ** 27 **
Number of picture books in
the home .11 .42 ** .47 ** .36 **
Level of child asking to be
read to .02 .31 ** .40 ** .30 **
Level child looks at books
by him/herself .02 .25 ** .17 .06
Years of schooling of primary
care giver .05 .48 ** .38 ** .33 **
Years of schooling of spouse .07 .38 ** .34 ** .22
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
Table 4
Effect of type of intervention on a measure of spelling outcome
(ordered by magnitude)
Dependent Variable: Mean Std Error Lower 95%CI Upper 95%CI
FBH Spelling test
results
Class control group 42.10 1.32 39.49 44.71
Phonological
experimental group 39.95 1.14 37.69 42.21
Language experimental
group 38.66 1.28 36.13 41.18
At-risk control group 34.00 1.75 30.53 37.47
Table 5
Effect of type of intervention on spelling outcomes with rhyming
and alphabet knowledge taken into account
Dependent Variable: Mean Std Error Lower Upper
FBH Spelling test results 95%CI 95%CI
Phonological experimental group 40.99 1.13 38.76 43.22
Language experimental group 40.02 1.16 37.73 42.32
Class control group 38.24 1.63 35.00 41.48
At-risk control group 35.80 1.62 32.60 39.00