Mutual fund performance: luck or skill?
Bhootra, Ajay ; Drezner, Zvi ; Schwarz, Christopher 等
I. INTRODUCTION
Fundamental finance principles, along with common sense, dictate
that investors take account of both risk and return, yielding some
measure of positive risk-adjusted returns that also account for all
transaction costs and/or management fees. In other words, investors seek
positive alphas ([alpha]'s), i.e., risk and cost-adjusted positive
returns. Many investors turn to professionals in search of such gains.
According to the Investment Company Fact Book (2011), almost 30% of
world's wealth is invested in mutual funds: $25 trillion of the $85
trillion total world-wide investible wealth. About 44% of individual
wealth in the U.S., $12 trillion, is invested in mutual funds. Mutual
funds charge management fees that appear to reduce total return to
investors. Many mutual funds either do not beat the market or fail to
outperform their chosen benchmarks. Indeed, while Fama and French (2010)
note "the aggregate portfolio of actively managed U.S. equity
mutual funds is close to the market portfolio, but the high costs of
active management show up intact as lower returns to investors,"
they document existence of genuine inferior and superior performance at
the negative and positive extremes.
Results herein mirror those of Fama and French (2010), though a
probability approach leads to the results herein versus the bootstrap
method used by them. Experiments involving many successive trials with
multiple subjects, demonstrate a positive probability that a subject
will win several times in a row merely due to luck, not skill. For
example, while the probability that a person flips a coin and ends up
with 10 heads in a row is less than one in a thousand (1/1032 = 0.098%),
such strings of luck do occur, and when they do, it is not skill on the
part of the person flipping the coin. Some of the thousands of
investment funds may end up the "winners" for prolonged
periods of time. Naturally, investors need to know whether streaks of
superior performance are due to skill. The analysis herein demonstrates
that such streaks exist within the family of mutual funds and are due to
skill, not luck.
The main contention of this paper relies on detecting the existence
of skill with the generalized binomial distribution, using an
established analogy with winning in sports. Money managers are usually
well-educated and even skilled, as are professional athletes.
Competition alone in sports dictates that not all professional teams can
win; though as noted in more detail below, the most highly skilled teams
appear to win more often. In contrast, a good deal of prior evidence
indicates that a majority of actively managed mutual funds fail to
outperform passively managed index funds.
According to Berk and Green (2004), the apparent lack of investment
skill has "led researchers to raise questions about the rationality
of investors who place money with active managers despite their apparent
inability to outperform passive strategies and who appear to devote
considerable resources to evaluating past performance of managers."
This paper contributes to this important debate about luck vs. skill.
Using a novel statistical approach based on the Generalized Binomial
Distribution (Drezner and Farnum, 1993), this paper investigates whether
some mutual funds provide positive [alpha]'s over time as a result
of skill. The approach demonstrates, in contrast to a large degree of
the extant evidence, that skilled fund managers do exist--persistence in
superior performance cannot be attributed to luck.
This paper's results suggest important practical implications.
The lack of convincing evidence that mutual funds provide superior
performance dramatically influences the investing habits of individuals.
The lack of such evidence along with such straightforward factors as the
ease of trading ETFs and lower management costs for ETFs have most
likely led to the growth of ETFs and a relative decline in mutual funds
in recent years. For example, from 2005-10 the number of ETFs grew from
204 to 950, a growth of nearly 400%, with Weinberg (2012) reporting a
total of over $1.4T invested in ETFs by 2012 from a zero base in 1994.
Between 2005 and 2010 the number of mutual funds grew from 8,449 to
8,545, a growth of just over 1% with the apparent peak of 8,884 mutual
funds in 2008 according to the Investment Company Fact Book (2011).
Indeed, from the peak in 2008 to the more recent number of 8,545, the
decline in the number of mutual funds is almost 4%. The research herein
focuses on the relative numbers of these types of funds a as symptom of
broader movements in the investment world and refrain from a more in
depth analysis of such funds, given the aim of addressing the luck vs.
skill issue.
Doubt surrounding the genuine performance of mutual funds is
probably a factor in the relative lack of growth of mutual funds in
recent years. However, if the (higher) risk-adjusted performance of some
mutual funds is due to skill, then investors will not abandon them
altogether. Aside from the crucial impact on financial theory, the
longterm fate of a $25 trillion industry rests in part on whether that
industry succeeds on the basis of skill or luck. Results herein provide
one additional reason for believing that skill is involved, and thus
that the mutual fund industry should have a secure future.
While the paper shines a spotlight on one particular area of
contention in the broader debate, the analysis of mutual fund
performance remains enveloped in controversy. At least four substantial
debates lie at the center of any such controversy about the risk-return
tradeoff. The perspective offered herein does not aim to resolve these
debates, rather it indicates which approach to these debates keeps the
spotlight on the paper's central contribution. The paper's
secondary contribution lies is delineating the major debates, given the
overall complexity of differentiating skill from luck. The aspiration to
identify the nature of the skill involved in achieving extraordinary
returns with a mutual fund investment remains. The conclusion addresses
this natural aspiration.
The first debate centers on how to measure risk appropriately. It
is largely related to the validity of CAPM or its alternatives. The
standard approach in the analysis of mutual fund performance side-steps
these controversies by measuring performance given any reasonable
metric, i.e., the Fama-French 3-factor and the Carhart 4-factor models.
In addition to the choice of a one-period measure of risk, Welch and
Levi (2012) propose that longer-run risk measures have no predictive
power. While acknowledging the importance of measuring risk, the
analysis herein utilizes the standard approach.
A second debate, tied to the issue of measuring risk, concerns the
appropriate time period for measuring risk-adjusted returns. Fama and
French (2010) note this issue by distinguishing their use of "long
histories of individual fund returns" from the alternative approach
of persistence tests such as those conducted by Grinblatt and Titman
(1989) and Carhart (1997), i.e., "that is, whether past winners
continue to produce high returns and losers continue to
underperform." The analysis herein fits within the
"persistence" tests category.
The third debate concerns the choice of appropriate comparison
samples. An instructive example is the Wall Street Journal's
"Investment Dartboard Contest," which has been held for almost
50 years. The contest appears to addresses the "luck vs.
skill" debate. Can luck, disguised as stock portfolios
"chosen" by darts, outperform the skill of individual
investors making their own reasoned portfolio choices? Ensign (2012)
reports that in this well-received contest, luck wins out, in which
darts beat "the readers, with 28 wins out of 46 contests."
However, such contests use samples that are inappropriate for answering
the question posed, about whether superior mutual fund performance is
due to skill or luck. Simply, investment professionals are not permitted
to select the portfolios submitted by readers. Hence, the WSJ contest is
really luck vs. the uninformed, not luck vs. skill. Their contest is
fun, sells papers, and provides some useful information to the casual
investor, though it remains silent about whether superior mutual fund
performance is due to skill.
How to choose an appropriate sample may remain a concern for years
to come, given that minimally, performance may be pegged to the: (1)
mutual fund itself, (2) manager of the fund, including appropriate
training or education, (3) manager and team, (4) mutual fund company
which selects managers, and even (5) the stock picking and investment
procedures used or permitted (e.g., whether funds allow short-selling).
This paper focuses on (1), the performance of mutual funds themselves.
The fourth debate focuses on the statistical approach and/or
methodology used to distinguish luck from skill. Naive and perhaps even
sophisticated statisticians or econometricians are likely to agree that
statistics should be able to provide a definitive approach. Yet, no
consensus has yet been achieved. Fama and French (2010) use
bootstrapping. Barras, Scaillet and Wermers (2010) refine a standard
approach by employing a control for zero-alpha funds. The primary
contribution herein centers on using the Generalized Binomial
Distribution (GBD) to provide a new and important perspective about the
luck versus skill mutual fund debate. Drezner and Farnum (1993) first
used the GBD to analyze an analogous debate about of skill vs. luck in
sports and in student success.
The data section describes the procedures for creating the sample
of monthly returns for 981 mutual funds for a 15-year period from 1995
to 2009. Intuitively, the analysis examines how many funds consistently
performed "at the top" across the whole sample period. For
example, a natural and important question is, how many funds
consistently performed within the top 25% of all mutual funds over the
whole sample period of 15 years? The funds in this top 25% are the
winners. The basic argument is that the GBD technique implies that they
are not winners by chance (luck). They win as a result of skill. The
results provide an estimate of the number of funds expected to win as a
result of skill using the standard binomial distribution. If fewer such
funds exist than expected, perhaps because funds regularly move into and
out of a winning position, then mutual fund performance cannot be
distinguished from luck. However, if significantly more such funds exist
in the sample than expected, skill is involved. The number of such funds
must be significantly higher by an appropriate statistical test to reach
such a conclusion.
The percentage constraint is p in the analysis (the percentage of
funds in the top p of all funds across the sample period), which can be
set to any level of probability. Percentages used include p = 0.25, 0.5
and 0.75 which means that the mutual fund's rate of return was at
the top one quarter of the returns, at the top one half, or the top 75%
(alternatively not in the bottom 25%) for the entire period. The results
over 180 months display statistical significance at an extreme level.
I. DATA
The data analyzed in this paper draws on the data employed and
described in detail in Schwarz (2011). Briefly, three sources combine to
generate the data. First is the Mutual Fund database as of 2007 from the
Center for Research in Security Prices (CRSP), which provides the
standard information about mutual funds, most importantly the type of
fund, NAV, returns, and asset data. Second, the Thomson Financial Mutual
Funds Holding Dataset (TFDS) as of 2007 is used to filter the first set
of funds to permit only stock funds (50% or more of holdings in stocks)
and those with total assets of at least $10 million are in the final
dataset. Finally, the analysis requires linking the dataset of mutual
funds to the CRSP Mutual Fund dataset using the MF Links files available
via WRDS. This link file focuses largely on equity funds. Returns in the
final set of mutual funds may be calculated beyond the set
"cutoff' date of 2007, which is why returns extending to
December 2009 are indicated below.
The basic dataset generates rates of return for 5,134 mutual funds
over the 180 month period from January 1995 to December 2009, including
553,973 return "observations." The GBD based approach requires
that only funds with all available observations over the entire sample
period be included in the analysis. Many of the mutual funds did not
operate over the whole period of 15 years and thus were dropped from
consideration. The sample includes only those mutual funds that operated
throughout the whole period, yielding a final set of monthly returns for
981 mutual funds over 180 months, from January 1995 to December 2009. On
average, these "survivor" funds are likely to be more skilled
as compared with the ones that did not survive the entire period.
However, not all funds within this group are equally skilled. As
demonstrated later, the statistical approach based on GBD allows for
identification of a small number of skilled funds within these
survivors. As a result, the analysis herein does not suffer from
survivorship bias as documented by Brown, Goetzmann, Ibbotson, and Ross
(1992) and others.
Finally, time-series regressions of monthly fund returns on
contemporaneous risk factors each year (using 12 observations for each
year in the regressions) produce the risk-adjusted returns (alphas
[equivalent to] as) for all funds. The regressions employ the three
standard risk-adjustment approaches: (1) CAPM, (2) the Fama-French
3-factor model and (3) the Carhart 4-factor model, yielding three sets
of 14,715 annual [alpha]s (15 annual a observations for each of the 981
sample funds). For details on calculating the alphas, see Appendix. All
three approaches yield consistent and robust results, though the
discussion below focuses only on as resulting from the Carhart 4-factor
model.
Before the central analysis proceeds, briefly consider the
descriptive statistics for the annual fund as. As noted above, the data
set includes 14,715 as consisting of the excess annual returns for 981
mutual funds over 15 years. Figure 1 displays a histogram of the average
excess return by fund across all 15 years. As expected, the graph
approaches the shape of a normal curve, with a slight negative skew.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Next, consider the all fund (aggregate) average annual alpha by
year, largely to explore whether any obvious "trend," or lack
thereof, exists. The graph in Figure 2 above compares the average annual
mutual fund [alpha] to the average annual return of the S&P 500. It
should be noted that while none of the sample mutual funds are index
funds, one would expect that in the aggregate their performance matches
the whole market, while also keeping in mind that one cannot calculate
[alpha]s for the S&P 500. Figure 2 displays the relatively random
nature of annual (aggregate) mutual fund as.
II. THE GENERALIZED BINOMIAL DISTRIBUTION
Suppose that a sequence of n Bernoulli events (the result of each
event is either "success" or "failure") is observed.
Each outcome has a probability p of success and 1 p of failure. If the
events are random and not correlated, the distribution of the number of
successes is a Binomial distribution. There are many situations where
the events are related. Drezner and Farnum (1993) proposed the
Generalized Binomial Distribution (GBD) to model such related events.
Examples of such situations are grades on a multiple choice exams,
standing of teams at the end of a baseball season (Drezner and Farnum,
1993) or the standing at the end of the NBA season (Drezner, 2006). A
strong analogy exists between skill in sports and the current case of
whether mutual fund performance displays skill over time.
In all these cases it is quite evident that skill plays a role in
longer term (repeated) outcomes. If a student answers the first question
on an exam correctly, the probability that he/she answers the second
question correctly increases; while if he/she answers the first question
incorrectly, the probability that he/she answers the second question
correctly decreases. The same reasoning holds in sports where a first
game win increases the probability of winning the second game due to
skill. The reason for this observation is that skill is involved and the
outcome is not random.
Consider a correlation factor 9 describing the skill level. The
value of [theta] is determined by fitting the resulting frequency
distribution to the data by a [chi square] goodness of fit test. Suppose
that in the first k events there are r successes recorded. The
"rate" of success is r/k. The probability of success for the
next event equals:
(1 - [theta])p + [theta] r/k (1)
When events are independent and no skill is involved, then [theta]
= 0 and the probability of success is p regardless of the history. When
[theta] = 1, then if the first event is a success, all events are
successes; and if the first event is a failure, then all k events fail.
An increasing 9 between these two extremes depicts an increasing level
of skill. Specific values of 9 for exam grades (0.5921), baseball games
(0.397) and NBA games (0.5765) were derived so that the GBD fits the
actual results of these data sets very well, while the binomial
distribution does not fit the actual data.
Drezner and Farnum (1993) provide proofs for the properties of the
GBD. Two important properties for the distribution exist:
Property 1:
Mean = np (2)
Property 2:
Variance = (1 - p) 1 - [[subset or equal to].sup.n-1.sub.k=0]k +
2[theta]/k+1/1 - 2[theta] (3)
In the case of mutual fund performance, values for p, such as p =
0.5, define the "success" that occurs for a mutual fund when
it performs in the top 50% of all mutual funds in the specified period.
If skill is involved in the performance of mutual funds the distribution
of number of successes should differ from a binomial distribution
because success in one period increases the probability of success in
the next period.
The [chi square] goodness of fit test indicates whether the
distribution of the returns of mutual funds is significantly different
from a binomial distribution. Suppose that data about m periods for a
given proportion p is given. The range of the binomial distribution is
between 0 and m. The range [0, m] is divided into 10 ranges so that the
binomial probability for each range is about 10%. This yields 10
expected values, one for each range. The actual counts in each range
constitute the actual data and the [chi square] statistic with 9 degrees
of freedom is found with the corresponding p-value for the goodness of
fit test. Low p-values, when comparing the data to the binomial
distribution, indicate deviation from randomness or, i.e., skill. When
comparing the data to the GBD, high p-values suggest no deviation from
GBD. Minimizing the [chi square] statistic by employing the solver in
Excel provides the best fitting value of 9.
III. ANALYSIS OF THE DATA RESULTS USING as
To adjust the data for different types of mutual funds the rates of
return were adjusted to provide annual as for 15 years for these 981
mutual funds. As noted above, the only results presented rely on the as
as calculated using the Carhart 4-factor model. Table 1 summarizes the
statistical results, and Figures 3-5 display graphs of the three cases.
The analysis shows that to be and remain at the top 25% of mutual fund
performance over time (or avoiding the bottom 25%) clearly requires
skill, while to be among the top 50% requires minimal skill. Hence the
results herein clearly demonstrate that persistently good mutual fund
performance requires skill. Performance placing a manager in the top 50%
is not statistically significant (the binomial p-value = 0.059), because
being slightly above or below the median is a matter of luck, which
affects the count of instances below and above the median. However, the
cut-off for being in the top 25% or the top 75% (or avoiding the bottom
25%) does not result in many instances in the "gray area" that
are the result of luck.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
IV. PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS
Results demonstrate that mutual funds that consistently outperform
do so as a result of skill. One remaining question is whether it is
possible to translate this knowledge into winning strategies for
individuals investing in mutual funds. Many such strategies may exist.
One rather intuitive approach follows.
Simply, individuals may identify funds that remain in the top 25%
of all funds for five consecutive years (using the techniques described
herein). Recall that the motivating factor is that such funds
"won" as a result of skill, and that skill remains for a
period of time. In sports, team owners and sports fans rely on the
persistence of skill. By analogy and as shown by the analysis herein,
the same holds true for mutual fund performance. Out of the 981 mutual
funds, five remained in the top 25% for the 1995-99 period, and four of
these five outperformed the market. Their average was higher than the
average for the remaining 976 returns with a p-value of 0.08. Note also
that the expected number of funds in the top 25% for the first five
years is 981/1024 < 1. The fact that five exist indicates skill, not
luck.
To explore the subsequent performance of the winners in more
detail, consider whether the top performing funds in an early period
maintain that superior performance throughout the sample period. Table 2
presents the (statistical) significance of that performance, given the
"winners" during various years. At the end of one year, the
top 25% (245 funds) of the 981 funds are automatically the top
performers. After two years, only 50 funds remain in the top 25% for
both years, and so on as indicated in row two.
The results presented in Table 2 reinforce the primary conclusion
and constitute the main contribution to the literature, i.e., some
superior mutual fund performance occurs through skill. Indeed, it is
likely that individual investors who invest in managed mutual funds with
appropriate superior performance make rational investment decisions.
V. CONCLUSION
This paper demonstrates that skill is involved in a mutual fund
remaining among the top 25% of performers of all mutual funds over a 15
year period. Since the analysis herein does not attempt to identify the
source of that skill, the exact nature of that skill remains elusive.
The desire to identify the nature of investment skill appears perfectly
rational. If the needed skill can be defined and "packaged,"
then more funds would be winners.
However, with more "winners," the perceived gains from
investing with some skilled investment approach end up being arbitraged
away, especially when the skill becomes widely known, accessible, and
implemented. An analogy with sports teams provides insight again.
Consider a professional football team that devises an innovative
strategy for defense and ends up with a perfect season. As long as other
teams can eventually replicate the strategy and the skill spreads, the
success of the new team remains limited. Eventually, the competitive
nature of sports eliminates the advantage arising from the new strategy.
Of course, the fundamental principles of arbitrage and efficient markets
enforce the same basic result for investment strategies. If a winning
investment strategy can be replicated, it will not long remain a winner.
Longer-term winning strategies also require skill at concealing the
winning strategy. Thus, the public identification of winning investment
strategies is self-defeating.
In addition, the small percentage of winning investment strategies
indicates that the attempt to discover some immutable nature of the
skill involved cannot be successful. There are likely to be several
distinct skilled strategies such that identifying one or even several
winning strategies from among a small sample of them is doomed to
failure given that meaningful statistically significant results could
not be achieved (due to the small sample).
Nonetheless, the persistence of successful mutual fund performance
is due to skill, and this conclusion has important ramifications for the
worldwide multi-trillion dollar mutual fund industry. While not a direct
confirmation nor an endorsement of such measures, the primary result of
this paper lends some support to guides such as Morningstar's
five-star rating system for mutual funds.
APPENDIX
Alphas from factor models
Alphas are obtained from time-series regressions of fund excess
returns on factors, as specified below. The factors are obtained from
Ken French's website at
http://mba.tuck.dartmouth.edu/pages/faculty/ken.french/data_library.html.
CAPM: [r.sub.it] - [r.sub.ft] = [[alpha].sub.i] +
[[beta].sub.i,mkt]([r.sub.mt] - [r.sub.ft]) + [[epsilon].sub.it]
Fama-French: [r.sub.it] - [r.sub.ft] = [[alpha].sub.i] +
[[beta].sub.i,mkt]([r.sub.mt] - [r.sub.ft]) +
[[beta].sub.i,SMB][SMB.sub.t] + [[beta].sub.i,HML][HML.sub.t] +
[[epsilon].sub.it]
Carhart: [r.sub.it] - [r.sub.ft] = [[alpha].sub.i] +
[[beta].sub.i,mkt]([r.sub.mt] - [r.sub.ft]) +
[[beta].sub.i,SMB]SM[B.sub.t] + [[beta].sub.i,HML]HM[L.sub.t] +
[[beta].sub.i,MOM]MO[M.sub.t] + [[epsilon].sub.it]
where:
[[alpha].sub.i] = alpha of fund i,
[r.sub.it] = return of fund i in month t,
[r.sub.mt] = return on market portfolio in month t,
[r.sub.ft] = risk-free rate in month t,
[SMB.sub.t] = size factor in month t,
[HML.sub.t] = book-to-market factor in month t,
[MOM.sub.t] = momentum factor in month t, and
[beta]'s are regression coefficients on market, SMB, HML, and
MOM factors.
REFERENCES
Barras, L., O. Scaillet, and R. Wermers, 2010, "False
Discoveries in Mutual Fund Performance: Measuring Luck in Estimated
Alphas," Journal of Finance 65(1), 179-216.
Berk, J.B., and R.C. Green, 2004, "Mutual Fund Flows in
Rational Markets," Journal of Political Economy 112, 69-1295.
Brown, S.J., W. Goetzmann, R.G. Ibbotson, and S.A. Ross, 1992,
"Survivorship Bias in Performance Studies," Review of
Financial Studies 5, 553-580.
Carhart, M., 1997, "On Persistence in Mutual Fund
Performance," Journal of Finance 52(1), 57-82.
Drezner, Z., and N. Farnum, 1993, "A Generalized Binomial
Distribution,"
Communications in Statistics: Theory and Methods 22, 3051-3063.
Drezner, Z., 2006, "On the Limit of the Generalized Binomial
Distribution," Communications in Statistics: Theory and Methods 35,
209-221.
Ensign, R.L., 2012, "Darts Top Readers Again, Extend Overall
Lead," The Wall Street Journal, July 14.
Fama, E.F. and K.R. French, 2010, "Luck versus Skill in the
Cross-Section of Mutual Fund Returns," Journal of Finance 65(5),
1915-1947.
Fulkerson, J.A., 2013, "Is Timing Everything? The Value of
Mutual Fund Manager Trades," Financial Management 42(2), 243-261.
Grinblatt, M., and S. Titman, 1989, "Mutual Fund Performance:
An Analysis of Quarterly Portfolio Holdings," Journal of Business
62(3), 394-415.
Investment Company Institute. 2011 Investment Company Fact Book.
Morningstar, morningstar.com
Schwarz, C.G., 2012, "Mutual Fund Tournaments: The Sorting
Bias and New Evidence," Review of Financial Studies 25(3), 913-936.
Welch, I., and Y. Levi, 2012, "The Case for Risk-Neutral
Capital Budgeting," Working Paper, UCLA.
Weinberg, A., 2012, "What ETFs' Next Act Will Look
Like," The Wall Street Journal, October 23.
Ajay Bhootra (a), Zvi Drezner (b), Christopher Schwarz (c), Mark
Hoven Stohs (d)
(a) Mihaylo College of Business and Economics, California State
University Fullerton Fullerton, California 92831 abhootra@fullerton. edu
(b) Mihaylo College of Business and Economics, California State
University Fullerton Fullerton, California 92831 zdrezner@fullerton. edu
(c) Paul Merage School of Business, University of California at
Irvine Irvine, California, 92697 cschwarz@uci. edu
(d) Mihaylo College of Business and Economics, California State
University Fullerton Fullerton, California 92831
[email protected]
Table 1
Goodness of fit
Binomial
p [chi square] p - value
0.25 220.46 1.7 x [10.sup.-42]
0.50 16.40 0.059
0.75 121.82 5.7 x [10.sup.-22]
Generalized Binomial Distribution
[theta] [chi square] p - value
0.245 10.54 0.308
0.064 7.42 0.594
0.262 4.55 0.872
Notes: Table 1 presents statistical results of the analysis. p is
the probability that a mutual fund performs in the top p % of all
funds during the sample period. As noted above, low p-values, when
comparing the data to the binomial distribution, indicate that
skill is involved in remaining among the top mutual fund
performers. When comparing the data to the GBD, high p-values
suggest no deviation from GBD. Row 1 (p = 0.25) corresponds to
Figure 1 below, Row 2 to Figure 2 and Row 3 to Figure 3.
Table 2
Top mutual fund performance
# of Years
in Top 25% 1 2
# of Mutual 245 50
Funds in Top 25%
Statistical 2.7 x [10.sup.-07] 2.9 x [10.sup.-06]
Significance
# of Years
in Top 25% 3 4 5
# of Mutual 24 11 5
Funds in Top 25%
Statistical 6.3 x [10.sup.-04] 6.6 x [10.sup.-05] 0.08
Significance
Notes: This table shows the number of mutual funds (row 2) in the
top 25% of all fund performance for the number of years given in
row one. For example, after the first year's performance, 245 funds
out of 981 funds total performed among the top 25. The statistical
significance of their superior performance (based upon the average
[alpha]), compared to the lower performing group for the remainder
of the sample period, is given in row three.