Toward a management strategy for optimal recruiting: potential applicant concerns on goodness of fit in the corporate culture.
Buntzman, Gabe ; Parker, Richard D.
ABSTRACT
Over the years a large body of literature has developed regarding
cultural difference using Hofstede's cultural dimensions. In the
course of studies regarding this literature attention has been placed on
the differences between management and employees from a management
perspective, yet little attention has been focused in the opposite
direction. This study uses an in-basket laboratory experiment to
determine if US students would prefer to work for a boss who shared
cultural traits. The literature, methods and findings are followed by a
discussion of the results and implications for entrepreneurs.
INTRODUCTION
Buntzman and Parker (2004) recently applied the Hofstede value
system to an analysis of MBA student perceptions of CEO effectiveness
and found evidence in a pilot study that their perceptions of CEO
effectiveness might in fact be related to their internationally diverse
cultural backgrounds. This study is in some ways a follow-on study. It
examines the preferences of undergraduate students to possibly work for
individuals whose philosophies reflect various cultural backgrounds
typical of the USA and certain European, Latin American and Asian
regions.
Currently a wealth of literature exists on the impact of culture on
business operations particularly in the area of employer/employee
relations. Much of this literature is useful in examining the role that
cultural differences plays in the understanding of how business works.
Culture though extends farther than just an individual's
background; one must also consider the culture of business to which a
new graduate will be entering upon completion of a degree program or an
experienced worker might enter upon changing jobs. "Corporate
culture is comprised of philosophy and mission, manner of internal
communications and hiring practices. From the outside such things as
dress code, flexibility of work schedules and level of volunteerism
often form a company's public profile" (Pfister 1999).
This perception of corporate culture is particularly true in
America. As one U.S. CEO stated: "I think the most important thing
you can have is pride in how you dress, in how you act ... when you have
facilities that are clean and painted, people take pride in that, and
you end up with a better safety record, a better environmental
record" (Pfister 1999). The philosophy of the CEO was reflected in
how the employees reacted to it. One employee indicated that as a result
of the CEO's attitude the company had become "extremely image
conscious" and failure to adhere to norms would result in
co-workers quietly pointing out violations (Pfister 1999).
The American value system as pointed out in a number of studies is
not universally accepted. In an application of the Hofstede values
(collectivism vs. individualism, high vs. low uncertainty avoidance,
high vs. low power distance, masculine vs. feminine and dynamic vs.
Confucian) Gouttefarde (1996) observed the interaction between Americans
and French working in French companies. The comparison was justified
given the cultural differences between the two nations. The Gouttefarde
study showed the French are high on the power distance scale whereas
Americans rate low. The study also showed French and Americans are at
odds on uncertainty avoidance. Not surprisingly many of the respondents
in the Gouttefarde study expressed frustration at their cultural
counterparts. The Americans were seen as brash and too individualistic.
The French were seen as micromanagers who stifled the creativity of
American employees and were mired in bureaucracy. French bosses were
seen as aloof from subordinates whereas the feeling was among French
subordinates that the American supervisors got too close.
In other international studies (e.g. MacMahon 1996 and Channon
& Dakin 1995) the importance of cultural understanding and identity
was seen as a key to business success. In terms of hiring practices by
small business owners in Ireland one entrepreneur stated: "I think
that in a small company you have to be careful in selecting your
workforce anyway--you try to make sure that people will fit in with your
way of thinking, your way of doing things and the other employees"
(MacMahon 1996). One study conducted in the relatively calm years of the
mid-1990s showed that in the emerging markets of Central and Eastern
Europe after the fall of communism, over 70% of companies surveyed for a
study in hiring practices were aiming for "99-100 per cent local
staffing in the long term, as most felt local staff would have a better
understanding of the local market" (Channon and Dakin 1995).
Arguably Ireland and the individual countries of the former Warsaw Pact are for the most part internally homogenous. But the expressed
preferences for hiring people who shared a common viewpoint may in fact
reflect a widely held desire that new employees fit the mold of the
corporation's culture.
The role of organizational culture within a corporation has been
the focus of a number of studies during the last 30 years. Van Dick,
Christ et al (2004) noted that social identification factors played a
vital role in employee retention. Pearson and Porath (2005) examined the
relationship and consequences between incivility and organizational
loyalty. Additionally Macklem (2005) found that an unhealthy desire for
profits created a cultural atmosphere that was detrimental to corporate
long-term health. But how does the idea of corporate culture play into
job acceptance decisions by prospective candidates?
In reviewing the literature relating to job acceptance factors, we
discovered that researchers choose to look at a number of items that
impact decisions to accept a job offer. In a study by Turban, Campion and Eyring (1995) it was noted that factors including "site visit,
perceptions of the location, and host likableness were related to job
acceptance decisions" but no mention was made of perceived cultural
attributes. Other studies by Swinth (1976), Sheridan, Richards and
Slocum (1975), Schmitt and Coyle (1976), Saks (1989), Saks and Cronshaw
(1990), Saks, Wiesner and Summers (1994) have sought to predict the
likelihood of job acceptance by potential employees using realistic job
previews and instruments which attempted to measure the candidates'
perceptions of employers based on factors such as likeability and
adequate communications. These studies appear to discount, ignore or
minimize the role corporate culture plays in job acceptance decisions.
Saks and Cronshaw (1990) point out that the channels of
communication used to provide potential applicants with "realistic
job information" comes in two forms: either through written
materials such as brochures or in oral communication during a job
interview. Such verbal communications fail to provide potential
employees with vital non-verbal communications which might provide clues
and insights into the nature of specific cultures within companies.
Additionally we wanted to know what companies are doing to foster
the culture that will best suit their strategic objectives (particularly
those objectives related to hiring qualified personnel). Herb Kelleher,
former CEO of Southwest Airlines is credited with establishing a culture
that carefully "protects and nurtures its employees" (Fryer
2004). Other firms offer employees access to top executives and perks
such as African safaris and other exotic trips. Such actions are
significant in that "employee morale and performance improvement
are top management concerns" (McMaster 2003). Problems begin to
arise when bosses begin to label subordinates as weak performers.
According to Jean-Francois Manzoni, a management scholar at a leading
French business school: "employee performance tends to adjust up or
down according to such expectations" (Mount 2002). As a highlight
to the importance of these issues Manzoni and Barsoux (2005) recently
conducted a study to determine the relationships between management and
employees and how employees disconnect from both management and their
jobs. Such studies are important in determining and understanding the
role culture plays within corporations.
Corporate culture also is impacted by popular media image. Given
that most people rely on the popular media to formulate and reinforce
viewpoints, the observations of this group cannot be discounted or
ignored. As an example the emphasis on cultural understanding in
corporations was succinctly framed in a recent editorial in the Dallas
Morning News: "A company is only as good as its employees. That
employer-employee relationship is being tested as never before, making
it more difficult for companies to get workers to agree with their
leaders on the same business goals. Successful businesses will
understand the trends that are undermining workplace morale and will
respond accordingly" (March 15, 2004).
Being aware of employer responsibilities in creating and managing
corporate culture as seen above, what can be investigated about the
expectations or desires of subordinates coming into a new work
situation? Do workers desire to be employed by someone who has a similar
cultural background and values to them? In the United States, is
cultural similarity a factor in determining whether or not a junior
candidate would base a decision on job acceptance based upon the
perceived opinions and judgments of similarity with the boss?
Research Questions
In order to investigate the whether or not cultural similarities
are desirable between employees and employers, we posed the following
research questions based on the above:
R1: Do US undergraduate students prefer a boss whose values are
consistent with the US "archetype" that was identified by
Hofstede?
R2: Do US business students prefer an entrepreneurial-type boss or
one who is more conservative?
R3: Is the type of business, its size and environment, a factor in
determining whether an entrepreneurial or conservative boss would be
preferred?
METHOD
The sample consisted of 100 undergraduate students enrolled in
several sections of the authors' required business courses.
Participation was voluntary and extra credit was provided.
We devised an in basket-like laboratory experiment for this study.
Written instructions asked respondents to assume the role of a
relatively new employee who has been appointed as a member of a 360/
style selection committee seeking a replacement for a "very senior
management position" for either a large multinational corporation
or a small local business in the service sector. After reading brief
statements of philosophy attributed to each candidate in turn, subjects
were asked to rate them as good for the organization, whether they would
enjoy working for the person, whether the person would be a good role
model for them, whether this person should receive priority in being
given a personal interview, and the excellence of each candidate's
philosophy. Each item was evaluated on a seven point Likert-type scale
anchored by "Disagree" (1) and "Agree" (7).
Additionally, we collected data on how the candidates were
perceived with respect to the Hofstede dimensions
masculinity/femininity, low/high uncertainty avoidance,
individualism/collectivism, and high/low power distance on seven point
Likert-type scales. The scales asked subjects to rate candidate in terms
of: hard driving or easygoing (to represent Hofstede's
masculinity/achieving orientation); concern for subordinates, to
represent collectivism, or out for himself, to represent individualism;
entrepreneurial, representing low uncertainty avoidance, or cautious,
representing high uncertainty avoidance; and elitist, representing high
power distance, or democratic, representing low power distance.
Four candidates were presented for evaluation. Alex's
statement of philosophy was designed to have him perceived as masculine,
individualistic, with low power distance (LPD) and low uncertainty
avoiding. According to Hofstede (1980) this is a typical value structure
in the USA. Bobby was portrayed as masculine, LPD, collectivistic, and
high uncertainty avoiding, typical of value systems in Japan and other
parts of Asia and Latin America (Hofstede, 1980).
Charlie's philosophy was intended to present him as feminine,
high power distance (HPD, individualistic, and high uncertainty
avoiding. This value system is consistent with Western European
countries such as France and some Asian states (Hofstede, 1980). Finally
Del's philosophy was intended to portray his/her value system as
masculine, individualistic, HPD, and high uncertainty avoiding, typical
of countries such as Belgium and Italy (Hofstede, 1980).
Independent variables were subject gender, business type (MNC v.
Small Business) and type of value system. We used analysis of variance
and stepwise multiple regression to analyze our data.
RESULTS
As stated above, Masculinity/Femininity was measured with the
anchors "hard driving" (M) and "easy going" (F);
Individualism/Collectivism was measured with the anchors "out for
himself" and "concerned for subordinates." Low/High UA
was measured with the anchors "entrepreneurial" and
"cautious" and High/Low Power distance used the anchors
"elitist" and "democratic" (Please see Table 1
below.)
As is shown in Table 1 below, Alex (A) is perceived as Masculine
(mean <4.0) and significantly more masculine than Bobby (B). Del (D)
is most masculine. Alex is perceived as more individualistic (>4.0),
and more so than the collectivist Bobby. He is perceived as a low
uncertainty avoider (entrepreneurial) (<4.0) and low PD (democratic)
(>4.00). With two exceptions, the "applicants" were
perceived as expected. The exceptions were that Bobby was perceived as
feminine and Charlie (C) was perceived as masculine. Overall, subjects
perceived the candidates as would be expected based on their
"statements of philosophies."
There were no gender differences in how men and women perceived the
candidates. There were no business-type differences in how subjects
perceive the candidates suitability for the position. Means for the
dependent variables based on type of business tended to be close to the
neutral midpoint value of 4 on the seven point Likert--type scales.
The candidates' value systems do appear to have influenced
subjects' perceptions and recommendations. We expected that Alex
would be rated more favorably because his/her value profile was intended
to be similar to that for the USA, and the vast majority of the subjects
were also from the US. In performing tests of differences in
perceptions, we took the mean value of the rating for the dependent
variable for Alex (representing the US value structure) as the test
value against whether the others were compared. Results for these
comparisons are discussed below.
Would this person be good for the organization? Using t-tests,
Charlie (Feminine, High Power Distance, Individualistic, High
Uncertainty Avoidance had a rating of 4.52 (s.d. 1.22) versus 5.09 (s.d.
1.09) for Alex, T=-4.65, p<.000. Thus Charlie was seen as somewhat
neutral for the organization compared to Alex. (Please see Table 2
below).
Would the subject enjoy working for this person? Bobby received the
highest rating, 5.12 (s.d. 1.23). This was significantly higher than the
ratings for Alex. (Please see Table 3). Del had a significantly lower
rating and Charlie's low rating approached significance.
Would this person be a good role model? None of the candidates
stood out in this regard with mean ratings close to the midpoint of
4.00, but C was significantly lower than A (3.99, s.d. 1.31, vs. 4.28,
s.d.. 1.48) and t=-2.22, p< .03.
Should preference be given to this person to be offered an
interview? No candidate stood out here either. There were no significant
differences between A and the other candidates. (Please see Tables 4 and
5 below).
Does this applicant have a good overall philosophy? B was rated
marginally higher than A, T>1.81, p<.074. C was not significantly
different and D was significantly lower (t=-2.29, p<.024). (Please
see Table 6 below).
Finally, we wondered whether perceptions of an applicant's
strengths (would he/she be good for the organization, would the
respondent enjoy working with him/her, would be a good role model, has a
good overall philosophy would be predictors of an offer for interviews.
We used stepwise multiple regression to explore this issue. For
candidates A, B, and C only "excellent philosophy" entered the
equation, but for D "a good role model" added explanatory
power. Adjusted R-squares ranged from about .26 to about .49. (Please
see Table 7 below.)
DISCUSSION
We thought that US students would prefer a candidate, one for whom
they might actually work according to our scenarios, who matched the
value profile of U.S. culture as determined by Hofstede. This was not
the case. In fact, using the mean for Alex as the reference point our
subjects did not demonstrate a statistically significant preference that
any of the candidates be offered an interview before the others.
The regression equations in which the candidates' philosophy
was shown to be associated with making an offering all provide food for
thought. These are no doubt many factors which bear on an
individual's preferences in choosing to join an organization on the
one hand, or whom to recruit to it on the other. Our results indicate
that consideration of a candidate's philosophy (values) may be a
major influence on those decisions. Often the emphasis in making such
decisions is placed on using "hard" data such as income or
profits or some other measure of achievement. In this experiment,
however, we depicted the candidates as equally able and so there was not
tangible data on which to base a decision. Another possibility would be
to determine preference based on self-interest, but here self-interest
concerns such as "would enjoy working with the person," or
"would be a good role model," did not enter into the
regression equations for three of the four candidates. In fact, for
three of the four candidates nothing entered into the regression
equation but philosophy.
Only is the case of D was "would be a good role model" a
predictor with a beta of .37. Perhaps this association is related to the
fact that on average our subjects did not think D would be as good a
role model as the other candidates.
Recently Gabarro and Kotter (2005) showed that "effective
managers take time and effort to manage not only their relationships
with their subordinates but also with their bosses." Perhaps given
the relative young age of the participants in the study reflects the
observation by Gabarro and Kotter (2005) that "some people's
instinctive reaction...is to resent the boss's authority and to
rebel against the boss's decision." As Hood (2004) noted
"businesses are now using mentorship to hone top talent, to broaden
staff skills and especially integrate new people into the
organization." These mentorship programs would give new employees
insights into cultural norms and mores within the corporation they might
not otherwise receive from managers or other top executives. One
question to consider is whether or not students who participated in
internships or co-operative education programs would have greater
insight into the dynamics of corporate culture than undergraduates who
did not participate in such programs.
An interesting point to note is that Meglino and Ravlin (1997)
found that when subjects had perceptions regarding jobs based upon
previously obtained information, they tended to respond more negatively
in accepting job offers, yet the opposite was true when subjects had no
prior exposure. Bretz and Judge (1994) point out that
"organizations tend to differentiate themselves on the basis of
what is rewarded and how rewards are distributed" and that
"individuals prefer to work in environments in which their
individual efforts and contributions are recognized." If the
subjects in this study tend to follow individualistic traits as most
Americans do, then one must ask if a perception bias arose among the
participants regarding potential recognition of individual efforts?
In their study of recruitment effectiveness Vandenberg and Seo
(1992) point out that individual needs and rankings of items of
importance vary and are time and circumstance sensitive. Vandenberg and
Seo (1992) also point out that the communication efforts needed to
provide all information individuals require for making decisions
regarding job acceptance will invariably fall short due to "the
infinite variety and combination of needs characterizing a group of
applicants." Clearly a challenge exists in business communication
efforts to recruit and persuade undergraduates to join organizations.
DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
Edwards and Kuruvilla (2005) identified culture as an area needing
greater study and clarification in understanding how international human
resources management works. We agree with their assessment and contend
that more work needs to be done, with a cross-cultural study and with
other types of samples, to determine the impact of senior management
characteristics on the preferences of subordinates. In-situ survey
research may be most appropriate for such a follow-up study. More
research into the impact of organizational philosophies, degree of
entrepreneurial culture and the like is warranted.
IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE
In the real world, it is the subordinate who is most often the one
under consideration to be hired and so is the one to be offered an
interview, contrary to the scenario in this study. But the hiring
process is a two way street since the subordinate has an opportunity to
accept or reject an offer. Organizations that hope to attract the best
candidates cannot ignore this part of the process and should be aware of
the things that make them attractive to potential employees. That is
where this study makes a small contribution. "Soft" issues
such as leader philosophy, organizational culture and so on should not
be ignored if organizations wish to attract the best candidates.
LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY
As this was a laboratory study with a student population from a
limited geographic area it would be unwise to generalize at this time.
Nevertheless, as this sample is drawn from a segment of the population
to which the results would be generalized the nature of the sample is
not per se problematic (Arnold, 2004). More research is needed to
clarify relationships and to validate the generalizability of the
results.
CONCLUSION
From this study it appears that a "similarity to me"
syndrome does not apply to the sample population making these
evaluations (assuming that the students sample does itself match the US
value archetype). It is striking that a candidate's overall
philosophy is important as a determinant of offers (as indicated by the
regression equations). This is a positive sign inasmuch as business
schools of late seek to emphasize the importance of values in the
workplace. But business schools also have emphasized the importance of
an entrepreneurial culture, networking and mentoring and these do not
seem to have been influential since an entrepreneurial orientation, role
modeling potential and enjoying working with an individual were, for the
most part, inconsequential considerations.
REFERENCES
Arnold, J. (2004) Editorial. Journal of Occupational &
Organizational Psychology 77(1), 1-11.
Bretz Jr., R. D. & Judge, T. A. (1994). The Role of Human
Resource System in Job Applicant Decision Processes. Journal of
Management, 20(3), 531-551.
Buntzman, G. & Parker, R. D. (2004). MBA Perceptions of
Entrepreneurial CEOs: A Pilot Study. Proceedings of the 2004 United
States Association for Small Business and Entrepreneurship Conference.
Business morality? Gap between employers, workers is too wide.
(March 15, 2004). Dallas Morning News, (p. 16A).
Channon, J. & Dakin, A. (1995). Coming to terms with local
people. People Management, 1(12), 24-28.
Chatman, J. A., Polzer, J. T., Barsade, S. G., & Neale, M. A.
(1998). Being different yet feeling similar: the influence of
demographic composition and organizational culture on work processes and
outcomes. Administrative Science Quarterly, 43 (4), 749-781.
Edwards, T. & Kuruvilla, S. (2005). International HRM: national
business systems, organizational politics and the international division
of labour in MNCs, International Journal of Human Resource Management,
16(1), 1-20.
Fryer, B. (2004). Accentuate the positive. Harvard Business Review,
82(2), 22-23.
Gabarro, J. J. & Kotter, J. P. (2005), Managing Your Boss.
Harvard Business Review, 83(1), 92-98.
Gouttefarde, C. (1996). American values in the French workplace.
Business Horizons, 39(2), 60-69.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's Consequences: International
Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Hood, S. B. (2004), Learn from the best. Canadian Business, 77(24),
107-109.
Macklem, K. (2005), The Toxic Workplace. Maclean's, 118(5),
34-35.
MacMahon, J. (1996). Employee relations in small firms in Ireland:
an exploratory study of small manufacturing firms. Employee Relations,
18(5), 66-77.
Manzoni, J. & Barsoux, J (2005), How bosses reveal their
attitudes towards employees. European Business Forum, 20(1), 86-88.
McMaster, M. (2003). Incentive insider tips. Meeting News, 27(6),
67-68.
Meglino, B. M. & Ravlin, E. C. (1997). When does it hurt to
tell the truth? The effect of realistic job reviews on employee
recruiting. Public Personnel Management, 26(3), 413-422.
Mount, I. (2002). How your boss is hexing you. Business 2.0, 3(11),
28.
Pearson, C. M. & Porath C. L. (2005). On the nature,
consequences and remedies of workplace incivility: No time for
"nice"? Think again. Academy of Management Executive, 19(1),
7-18.
Pfister, B. (1999). Philosophy, attitudes in workplace define
personality of a business. San Antonio Express-News. 1K
Saks, A. M. (1989). An examination of the combined effects of
realistic job previews, job attractiveness and recruiter affect on job
acceptance decisions. Applied Psychology: An International Review,
38(2), 145-163.
Saks, A. M. & Cronshaw, S. F. (1990). A process investigation
of realistic job previews: Mediating variables and channels of
communication. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 11(3), 221-236.
Saks, A. M., Wiesner, W. H. & Summers, R. J. (1994). Effects of
job previews on self-selection and job choice. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 44(3), 297-316.
Schmitt, N. & Cole, B. (1976). Applicant decisions in the
employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(2), 184-192.
Sheridan, J. E., Richards, M. D., & Slocum, J. W. (1975).
Comparative analysis of expectancy and heuristic models of decision
behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60(3), 361-368.
Swinth, R. (1976). A decision process model for predicting job
preferences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61(2), 242- 5.
Turban, D. B., Campion, J. E. & Eyring, A. R. (1995),
"Factors related to job acceptance decisions of college recruits.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 47(2), 193-213.
Van Dick, R., Christ, O., Stellmacher, J.; Wagner, U.; Ahlswede,
O.; Grubba, C.; Hauptmeier, M.; Hohfeld, C.; Moltzen, K.; Tissington,
P.A, (2004). Should I Stay or Should I Go? Explaining Turnover
Intentions with Organizational Identification and Job Satisfaction.
British Journal of Management, 15(4), 351-360.
Vandenberg, R. J. & Seo, J. H. (1992). Placing Recruiting
Effectiveness in Perspective: A Cognitive Explication of the Job-Choice
and Organizational Entry Period. Human Resources Management Review,
2(4), 239-273.
Gabe Buntzman, Degma Investing LLC
Richard D. Parker, High Point University
Table 1: Mean Perceptions of Candidates
Lower Values Alex Bobby Charlie Del
Masculine 3.67 4.80 * 3.66 3.27 *
Collectivism 4.27 3.03 * 4.70 * 5.08 *
Low UA 3.36 4.99 * 3.88 * 3.91 *
High PD 4.35 4.84 * 3.77 * 3.78 *
Note: Lower values imply more agreement.
* p<.05
Table 2: Mean values for "Would the candidate be good for the
organization?"
Dependent Variable Mean SD
a-good 5.09 1.09
b-good 5.07 1.24
c-good 4.52 *** 1.23
d-good 4.90 3.93
Note: *** p < .001.
Table 3: Mean values for "Would you enjoy working for this
candidate?"
Dependent Variable Mean SD
b-enjoy 5.12 *** 1.23
c-enjoy 4.29 * 1.45
a-enjoy 4.56 1.51
d-enjoy 4.08 ** 1.52
Note: * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
Table 4: Mean values for "Would this person be a good role model
for you?"
Dependent Variable Mean SD
a-model 4.28 1.48
b-model 4.49 1.38
c-model 3.99 1.31
d-model 4.06 1.41
Note: n = 100
Table 5: Means for "Should this person receive an offer of an
interview before the others?"
Dependent Variable Mean SD
a-offer 4.05 1.58
b-offer 4.02 1.51
c-offer 3.91 1.49
d-offer 4.11 1.42
Note: n = 99
Table 6: Means for "This person has an excellent philosophy."
Dependent Variable Mean SD
a-philos 4.43 1.29
b-philos 4.66 1.25
c-philos 4.20 1.49
d-philos 4.12 1.34
Note: n = 99 Note: n = 99 Note: n = 99
Table 7: Results of Regression Analysis for Offers
Predictors DV
R R Square
Step 1 Offer to A .27 ***
Constant 1.28
a-philos .60
Step 1 Offer to B .30 ***
Constant .29
b-philos .66
Step 1 Offer to C .49 ***
Constant 1.20
c-philos .65
Step 1 Offer to D .43 ***
Constant 1.26
d-philos .26
Step 2 Constant .93 .43 *** [DELTA]
d-philos .40 [R.sup.2] 0.06
d-model .38
Note: *** p < .001