White vs. blue: does the collar color affect job attitudes and behaviors?
Rozell, Elizabeth J. ; Pettijohn, Charles E. ; Parker, R. Stephen 等
INTRODUCTION
Researchers continue to be curious about the role of job attitudes
and behaviors as they relate to a variety of workplace variables.
Specifically, past empirical studies have investigated a variety of work
attitudes related to dispositional affectivity, with most studies
examining a few workplace attitudinal variables within a study. In the
current study, we investigate the impact of dispositional affectivity on
a wide spectrum of workplace attitudes and behaviors using a sample of
white and blue collar workers. Much of the research in the area of
dispositional affectivity has largely focused on negative affectivity
(Fredrickson & Losada, 2005; Hochwarter et al, 2003). As such,
researchers have criticized the exclusive focus on the negative
affectivity construct (Fortunato & Stone-Romero, 1999; Stone-Romero,
2005). The positive psychology movement, first advocated by Seligman
(2000), has shifted the focus to positive affectivity and its advantage
for promoting a healthy organizational environment. Hence, there has
been a shift in the literature on dispositional affectivity to a greater
emphasis on evaluating positive affect and its statistical relationships
with a variety of variables. This paper looks at both positive and
negative affect and their impact on a range of job attitudes and
behaviors.
In most research regarding workplace attitudes and behaviors,
research is conducted with employee samples without regard to the
'type of job'. The assumption seems to be one that holds that
workers are workers. Thus, factors that may affect one group will
logically affect another group. However, what if this assumption fails
to hold true? What if there are differences in attitudes and behaviors
that exist independent of one's workplace environment? It seems
logical to assume that individuals enter the workplace with certain
predispositions that have been formed as a result of their experiences
and perhaps genetics. Further, it seems logical to assume that an
element of self-selection exists in the workplace, with certain
individuals possessing specific predispositions selecting careers that
match these predispositions. Thus, this research fills a void in the
literature pertaining to differences in white and blue collar workers.
Indeed, little, if any recent research has examined the attitudinal and
behavioral differences between white and blue-collar workers. Therefore,
another purpose of the current research study was to investigate these
differences.
We begin by reviewing the pertinent literature for dispositional
affectivity and each of the individual difference variables. Next, we
consider the conceptual linkages between these variables, as well as
their effects on work-related attitudes and behaviors such as job
satisfaction, organizational commitment, turnover intentions,
absenteeism, and tardiness. Drawing on this discussion, we use
regression analysis comparing the white and blue-collar samples to test
a set of hypotheses regarding the relationships between dispositional
affectivity and certain work attitudes and behaviors. We examine these
models using a powerful sample of 595 employees (an 85% response rate)
of a Midwestern manufacturing company. We conclude with a discussion of
the results and their implications for management research and practice.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Dispositional Affectivity and Work-Related Attitudes and Behaviors
Social scientists have long been intrigued by individual
differences in people's interpretations of their own emotional
experiences (Berry & Hansen, 1996). In particular, research shows
that some individuals report experiencing increased amounts of positive
emotions relative to others. The phenomenon is referred to as positive
affect, and these persons are usually self-described as joyful,
exhilarated, excited, and enthusiastic. Those low in PA have been
described as listless, lethargic, drowsy, apathetic, and dull
(Cropanzano et al, 1993; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). In contrast,
other individuals describe themselves as experiencing greater amounts of
negative feelings than others, and are often referred to as
high-negative-affect individuals (Berry & Hansen, 1996; Cropanzano
et al., 1993). Such individuals report being afraid, anxious, angry, and
tend to be nervous and tense. Those low in NA tend to view conditions as
less upsetting and stressful than high NA individuals (Chiu &
Francesco, 2003). Interestingly, the research on dispositional
affectivity has shown that there are two general dimensions of affective
responding: trait-positive affect (PA) and trait-negative affect (NA).
These dimensions do not appear to represent opposite ends of a
continuum; but rather they are independent of one another (Berry &
Hansen, 1996; Diener & Emmons, 1985). That is, it is possible for an
individual to be high on both, low on both, or high on one but not the
other (George, 1992; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). An individual who
rates high on both dimensions would be characterized as quite emotional,
and would experience fluctuating moods in response to environmental
stimuli (Diener & Emmons, 1985). In sharp contrast is the individual
that rates low on both who would likely display little affect; i.e. the
person would likely be unemotional and unresponsive (Cropanzano et al.,
1993).
Several researchers have documented the significant relationship
between dispositional affectivity and work attitudes. For example, an
inverse relationship has been found to exist between NA and job
satisfaction ENRfu(Levin & Stokes, 1989; Staw, Bell, & Clausen,
1986). A minority of researchers has criticized negative affectivity as
a construct (Stone-Romero, 2005) citing construct validity problems,
however, several others have shown success in using an established and
validated scale (Watson et al., 1988; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988;
Watson 1988a, 1988b). Researchers have documented that NA may be
negatively correlated with not only job satisfaction, but also
organizational commitment, and positively correlated with turnover
intentions; the exact opposite pattern of correlations has been obtained
for PA ENRfu(Cropanzano et al., 1993). One explanation for these
relationships is that work attitudes are primarily a function of how an
individual affectively responds to his or her work environment, and are
therefore influenced by one's underlying affective disposition.
Consequently, high PA individuals are likely to exhibit extremely
positive responses to their work environment which are reflected in
their work attitudes, while extreme negative responses are usually seen
in high NA persons ENRfu(George, 1992).
Research notes the tendency of individuals to be dispositionaly
inclined to form positive or negative attitudes about their work
(Cropanzano et al., 1993). Interestingly, Arvey, Bouchard, Segal, and
Abramson (1989) demonstrated that approximately 30% of the observed
variance in general job satisfaction was attributable to genetic
factors. Longitudinal studies indicate that scores on job satisfaction
measures remain correlated over time, and that this relationship holds
even when individuals change employers or occupations (Staw et al.,
1986; Staw & Ross, 1985). These findings do not mean that work
attitudes are entirely stable, or that the job context is unimportant;
in actuality, work attitudes do indeed fluctuate over time. Instead,
these longitudinal studies are consistent with the view that while work
attitudes vary as a function of changes in the work setting (Cropanzano
& James, 1990; Newton & Keenan, 1991), the rank ordering of
individuals' attitudes remains relatively stable, and that such
stability can be attributed to certain underlying personality
dispositions (George, 1992) such as positive or negative affectivity
(Cropanzano et al., 1993).
Research by Fredrickson (1998, 2001) has proposed a
"broaden-and-build" theory of positive affect which contends
that individuals who experience positive emotions and generally
experience "chronic" positive affectivity are able to adapt
and be flexible to workplace changes. Further, it has been proposed that
positive affect individuals possess a wider range of thoughts than
individuals who experience negative affectivity on a regular basis.
Recent empirical support has shown how positive affect influences
behavioral responses (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), and
psychological growth (Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh & Larkin, 2003).
Indeed, Fredrickson and Losada (2005) contend that PA individuals
experience a broader range of thoughts that are proactive in nature as
opposed to thoughts that are single-mindedly stagnant, which in essence
broadens their behavioral repertoire. Based on this reasoning,
Fredrickson (2001) hypothesized that positive affectivity may lead to an
increase in psychological resources over time.
In a recent study by Fisher (2002), it was found that positive
affectivity was predictive of affective commitment and helping
behaviors. Interestingly, in the same study, intention to leave was
predicted by work attitudes rather than affective reactions. Further,
research has indicated that positive affectivity is characteristic of
employees that are successful at dealing with organizational stressors
(Isen et al, 1987; Fredrickson et al 2003; Fredrickson & Branigan,
2005; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Moreover, in a study by Chiu
and Francesco (2003) it was found that dispositional affectivity
predicted turnover intentions. Based on the research outlined above, we
hypothesized the following:
H1a: Higher positive affect levels will be significantly and
positively related to organizational commitment levels for both white
and blue collar workers.
H1b: Higher negative affect levels will be significantly and
negatively related to organizational commitment levels for both white
and blue collar workers.
H2a: Higher positive affect levels will be significantly and
negatively related to turnover intention levels for both white and blue
collar workers.
H2b: Higher negative affect levels will be significantly and
positively related to turnover intention levels for both white and blue
collar workers.
Most measures of job satisfaction include questions containing both
positively and negatively worded items, for example, "my job makes
me content", and "my job is disagreeable" from the Job in
General scale by Ironson, Smith, Brannick, Gibson, and Paul (1989).
Fisher (2002) contends that items such as these most likely trigger
recall of both positive and negative emotions experienced in the
workplace. Indeed, Price (2001) notes that PA and NA may impact job
satisfaction through selective perception. That is, PA individuals may
selectively perceive positive aspects of the job rather than the
negative, resulting in greater job satisfaction. Other researchers have
confirmed a similar relationship between dispositional affectivity and
job satisfaction (Judge, 1993; Agho et al, 1992; Levin & Stokes,
1989; Cropanzano et al, 1993). Hence, we hypothesized the following:
H3a: Higher positive affect levels will be significantly and
positively related to job satisfaction levels for both white and blue
collar workers.
H3b: Higher negative affect levels will be significantly and
negatively related to job satisfaction levels for both white and blue
collar workers.
Other workplace behaviors have also been linked to dispositional
affectivity. Interestingly, Iverson and Deery (2001) found that high PA
individuals were associated with increased tardiness and early departure
but decreased absenteeism. These same authors note the lack of empirical
research exploring the causes of tardiness and absenteeism. Indeed, most
research on these two workplace variables has focused on the Big Five
personality traits (Iverson & Deery, 2001). For example, Cooper and
Payne (1967) found that extraversion was significantly associated with
both tardiness and absenteeism. In a more recent example, Furnham and
Miller (1997) found that PA had a positive relationship to absenteeism.
With regard to NA, Ferris, Youngblood, and Yates (1985) and Cooper and
Payne (1967) both found that anxiety was associated with absenteeism.
Based on the research noted above, we hypothesized the following:
H4a: Higher positive affect levels will be significantly and
negatively related to levels of absenteeism for both white and blue
collar workers.
H4b: Higher negative affect levels will be significantly and
positively related to levels of absenteeism for both white and blue
collar workers.
H5a: Higher positive affect levels will be significantly and
negatively related to levels of tardiness for both white and blue collar
workers.
H5b: Higher negative affect levels will be significantly and
positively related to levels of tardiness for both white and blue collar
workers.
METHODS
It was determined that the sample for this study should be drawn
from a firm engaged in manufacturing operations employing both white and
blue-collar workers. This firm had approximately 400 employees engaged
in blue collar shift-work and 300 white-collar workers. Therefore, the
population consisted of 700 hourly employees of a manufacturing firm
located in the Midwestern United States. The final sample size resulted
in 594 workers.
In the construction of the survey, a variety of standardized
instruments were used to measure the variables included in the research
model. Descriptions of these measures and the evidence of reliability
and validity are provided below.
Positive and negative affect were measured using the Positive and
Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) developed by Watson, Clark, and
Tellegen (1988). The PANAS includes a list of 20 mood-relevant
adjectives, of which 10 indicate positive (e.g., active, enthusiastic)
and 10 indicate negative (e.g., angry, afraid) mood states. Respondents
are instructed to "indicate to what extent you generally feel this
way, that is, how you feel on the average." Extensive validity
evidence is provided by Watson et al. (1988), Watson, Clark, and Carey
(1988), and Watson (1988a; 1988b). Alpha coefficients of .86 and .80 for
the PA and NA scales, respectively, were obtained in the current study.
A measure of intent to leave developed by O'Reilly, Chatman,
and Caldwell (1991) was employed in this study. This scale is composed
of four 7-point Likert-type questions: (1) "To what extent would
you prefer another more ideal job than the one you now work in?"
(2) "To what extent have you thought seriously about changing
organizations since beginning to work here?" (3) "How long do
you intend to remain with this organization?" (4) "If you have
your own way, will you be working for this organization three years from
now?" Each employee was asked to respond to these questions. A
coefficient alpha of .80 for this scale was obtained in this research.
Tardiness was measured by a single item which read "How
frequently do you arrive at least 10 minutes late to work?" A 7
point Likert scale was used ranging from "never" (1) to
"very often (7)." Absenteeism was also measured with a single
item which read "Not counting holidays, vacation days,
hospitalizations and surgeries, how many days of scheduled work did you
miss over the past year?"
In a review of the organizational commitment literature, Meyer and
Allen (1991) identified affective, continuance, and normative commitment
as three distinctive components of commitment. Affective commitment
refers to an affective attachment to the organization. Continuance
commitment involves a perceived cost of leaving the organization.
Normative commitment stems from a perceived obligation to remain with
the organization. Based on the Organizational Commitment Questionnaire
developed by Mowday et al. (1982), Allen and Meyer (1990) developed and
validated separate measures for each component. Given the focus of the
current study, we included Allen and Meyer's 8-item Affective
Commitment Scale (ACS) as our measure of organizational commitment.
Coefficient alphas for the ACS of .87 and .90 were obtained by Allen and
Meyer, and in the present study, respectively.
Overall job satisfaction was measured using the 18-item "Job
in General" (JIG) scale (Ironson, Smith, Brannick, Gibson, &
Paul, 1989) from the revised version of the Job Descriptive Index (JDI)
(Smith, Kendall, & Hulin, 1969). Validation evidence for the JIG is
provided by Ironson et al. (1989); coefficient alphas for the JIG scale
range from .91 to .95. In the present study, an alpha coefficient of .89
was obtained. Additionally, a single item was used to assess job
satisfaction. Subjects were asked to respond to the following question
using a 7-point Likert scale: "All in all, how satisfied are you
with your current job?"
The administration of the instrument packets was conducted in
cooperation with contact members of the targeted organization.
Specifically, data collection was designed to reach all employees at the
participating manufacturing firm. The method used was a "drop-off
method whereby contact persons in the firm distributed the survey
packets to all employees in their work units. Respondents completed the
instruments during normal work hours, and returned them directly to the
researchers using a pre-addressed and pre-paid postage packet.
Of the survey packets distributed, 594 were completed and returned
for a response rate of 85 percent. Table 1 provides a summary of the
demographic attributes of the subjects.
ANALYSIS
The research plan was designed to first determine whether there
were significant relationships between the variables of interest and
positive and negative affect levels exhibited by both white and blue
collar workers. Second, the research was then focused on whether white
and blue collar workers exhibited similar levels of organizational
commitment, turnover intentions, job satisfaction, tardiness,
absenteeism, positive affect, and negative affect. The third research
question was whether the regression equations relating positive and
negative affect levels to the dependent variables of interest were
fundamentally equal with regard to the statistical relationships.
Since the research was designed to compare the mean levels of
organizational commitment, job satisfaction, absenteeism, tardiness, and
turnover intentions of workers, regression analysis were used to
investigate the relationships between worker type and the outcome
variables. A separate regression analysis was performed for each of the
outcome variables. Further, we used the Chow test to compare the
equality of a series of regression equations which had evaluated the
statistical relationships between the variables. The results of all
analyses are presented in the results section.
RESULTS
As the results in Table 2 indicate, H1 is supported by the results.
Both groups of employees, white and blue collar, show significant
univariate relationships between their levels of positive affect,
negative affect, and organizational commitment. Positive affect scores
are significantly and positively related to levels of organizational
commitment. Thus, as positive affect levels rise, levels of
organizational commitment also rise. Conversely, as levels of negative
affect increase, both groups of workers' organizational commitment
levels decline significantly. An examination of the results indicates
that positive affect contributed over 20 percent of the explanation of
the variation in organizational commitment levels for both blue and
white collar workers (R2 > .20).
Table 2 also indicates that H2 was supported by the results, as
turnover intentions are significantly related to the workers'
levels of positive/negative affect. As levels of positive affect
increase turnover intentions decline and as levels of negative affect
increase turnover intentions increase. While these findings are
significant for both blue and white collar workers, an examination of
the results indicates that 22 percent of the variance (R2 = .22) in
turnover intention levels was explained by positive affect scores for
blue collar workers, but less than 10 percent of the variance (R2 = .09)
in turnover intention levels was explained by positive affect scores for
white collar workers.
The findings also lend support to H3. As indicated in Table 2,
positive affect scores are significantly related to the levels of job
satisfaction for both white and blue collar employees. Also, the
findings show that higher levels of negative affect lead to
significantly lower levels of job satisfaction (lower levels of negative
affect lead to higher levels of job satisfaction). Positive affect
scores explain over 20 percent of the variation (R2 > .20) in job
satisfaction levels.
The fourth hypothesis is not supported by the findings as neither
positive nor negative affect are significantly related to worker
absenteeism. As shown in the table, for both white and blue collar
workers, the results indicate the affect levels are not significantly
related to levels of absenteeism.
H5 is largely supported by the findings as negative affect levels
are positively related to worker tardiness for both white and blue
collar employees. However, with regard to the levels of positive affect,
the relationship is significant only for white collar employees. As
indicated in Table 2, as white collar employee levels of positive affect
increase, worker tardiness levels decline. However, the relationship
between tardiness and positive affect levels is not significant for blue
collar workers.
While the tests of the hypotheses provide some insight into the
issues regarding whether blue and white collar workers are substantially
equal with regard to the relationships existing between affect levels
(positive/negative) and the dependent variables (organizational
commitment, turnover intentions, job satisfaction, absenteeism, and
tardiness), questions may still remain regarding the equality of the two
groups and their relationships. To determine whether differences exist
between the two groups, the Chow (1960) test was used. Chow (1960)
developed an equation designed to determine the degree to which two sets
of observations might be "regarded as belonging to the same
regression model." The equation for assessing these differences is
provided below:
[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]
Where:
RSS = residual sum of squares - pooled
RSS1 = residual sum of squares - group 1
RSS2 = residual sum of squares - group 2
n1 = number of observations - group 1
n2 = number of observations - group 2
k = number of parameters
Using this test, models were tested which evaluated the
relationships existing between positive/negative affect and the relevant
dependent variables (organizational commitment, turnover intentions, job
satisfaction, absenteeism, and tardiness). The results of these
regressions and the Chow Test are provided in Table 3 and are discussed
below.
With regard to organizational commitment, the relationship existing
between worker affect levels and organizational commitment is not
significantly different between the two worker categories. The
regression equations indicate that only positive affect is significantly
related to organizational commitment in the regression model, while
negative affect is not significantly related. The findings in this case
indicated that there are no differences based on worker type (white vs.
blue).
Similar findings exist pertaining to the relationships between
positive/negative affect and turnover intentions. As shown, the
differences between the two regression equations are not significant,
and one can thus assume that the two models are equal. However, for
these two equations, both positive and negative affect levels are
significantly related to turnover intentions.
The Chow Test indicates significant differences between the two
regression equations computed for job satisfaction. As shown, the Chow
Test reveals that the two equations are not equal (p = .0021). A review
of the findings indicates that the differences may lie in the increased
size of the standardized betas for the blue collar grouping. As may be
noted, the blue collar betas are .56 and .51 for positive and negative
affect levels while the white collar betas are .45 and .43 respectively.
The results also indicate that differences between the two
regression equations computed for absenteeism exist. However, in this
case, interpretation is limited because the regression models themselves
are not significant for either the white or blue collar workers.
However, a review of the results indicates that the differences may lie
in the increased standardized beta coefficient pertaining to the
relationship between negative affect and absenteeism for the blue collar
sample.
Finally, the Chow Test indicates that the two regression equations
are not significantly different as they relate to the relationship
between the employees' affect (positive/negative) levels and their
tardiness. Nevertheless, the findings show that tardiness is
significantly affected by positive affect for the white collar grouping
and significantly affected by negative affect for the blue collar
grouping. Yet, these differences are not significant and thus one cannot
interpret the two equations as being significantly different.
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
The findings clearly indicate that the workers' relative PA/NA
levels are significantly related to their job satisfaction,
organizational commitment, turnover intentions, and tardiness. These
findings suggest that firms could logically use PA/NA as a tool in their
employee selection and training processes. By selecting employees with
higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect,
firms might discover that their employees are more satisfied, more
organizationally committed, and have lower levels of turnover
intentions.
Indeed, these findings suggest that managers might use positive and
negative affect levels as a selection tool. It has generally been
assumed that "positive people" make better employees. However,
these findings indicate that being "positive" alone is not the
"ideal" circumstance. Similarly, the results indicate that
one's being negative alone is not the "worst"
circumstance. Instead the findings show that one who has the following
traits: positive, happy, perceiving the "best" in situations;
combined with traits of being low in anger, negativity, etc. will obtain
the optimal work attitudes. On the separate end of the continuum, the
individual who has traits that don't allow him/her to experience
joy, to see the good in situations, or to be positive; combined with the
worker who possesses traits that make him/her angry, negative, etc. will
possess the least desirable work attitudes. However, combinations of
these traits, may allow a worker to experience less than optimal work
attitudes.
Thus, it may be concluded that managers might use positive and
negative affect levels of their employees discriminately. For example,
the fact that a worker has a high positive affect score (or a high
negative affect score) alone should not necessarily qualify (or
disqualify) him/her for a job. Instead, the manager needs to assess the
combinations of affect levels to use this as a tool in selection.
A manager interested in selecting and developing high performing
workers may discover that the measurement of the individual's
dispositional affect is an indicator of his/her likely work attitudes.
However, the findings in this study indicate that the relationship is
not a clear-cut as one might speculate. Instead, the findings indicate
that combinations of positive and negative affect levels are related to
work attitudes. Based on this finding, managers should evaluate the
applicants' levels of both positive and negative affect to ensure
that those with the lowest (i.e., worst) combination of scores are not
selected and then encourage the development of higher levels of positive
affect and lower levels of negative affect through selection decisions.
This study examines a topic which has not been studied in depth in
nearly 30 years. Indeed, an important purpose of the current study was
to assess differences in work attitudes in blue and white collar
samples. Further, a strength of the current study was the high response
rate (85%) which reduces non-response bias in the data. Within the
sample, significant differences were found with regard to dispositional
affectivity and job satisfaction and absenteeism. In both instances, it
was found that the relationships were stronger for the blue collar
sample. It is interesting to note that there were no significant
differences found with regard to dispositional affectivity and
organizational commitment, turnover, and tardiness. Hence, companies
should be aware of the strong relationship that exists for blue collar
workers in terms of dispositional affectivity and job satisfaction and
absenteeism. That is, companies desiring high levels of both of these
job attitudes should certainly pay attention to their blue collar
workers. Selecting for certain levels of both positive and negative
affectivity might be advantageous for companies employing large numbers
of blue collar workers. Given the mounting evidence of the impact of
dispositional affectivity as it relates to many work attitudes, firms
should seriously consider selection issues with regard to both positive
and negative affectivity.
LIMITATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
While the findings reported in this research provide strong
indications that there exist significant differences in specific work
attitudes and behaviors between blue and white-collar workers,
limitations do exist. The first limitation is related to the fact that
these results are based on a single company, a single group of workers,
at a single point in time. Thus, the sampling frame limits the
generalizability of these findings. Although a strength of the current
study was the examination of many attitudinal and behavioral variables
in a single sample, it also warrants replication. Second, the research
is limited by the degree to which both the criterion variables and the
independent variables are accurately measured.
These limitations provide potential avenues for future research.
The first suggestion for subsequent research involves expanding the
sample to include workers from other firms, industries and in other
geographic regions. A related extension of the present research could
entail a longitudinal study. This research would assess the stability of
these relationships over time and could lead to a more concrete
evaluation of the empirical relationships between these variables. A
third area for future research might entail an evaluation of the
measures used in the research. This research would then lead to an
establishment of norms for the scales which could then be used in
identifying employees with the most desirable work attitudes and
behaviors.
REFERENCES
Agho, O.A., Mueller, C. W., & Price, J.L. (1992). Discriminant
validity of measures of job satisfaction, positive affectivity, and
negative affectivity. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 65, 185-196.
Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and
antecedents of affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the
organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1-18.
Arvey, R. D., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Segal, N. L., & Abramson,
L. M. (1989). Job satisfaction: Environmental and genetic components.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 187-192.
Berry, D. & Hansen, J. (1996). Positive affect, negative
affect, and social interaction. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 71(4), 796-809.
Chiu, R.K. & Francesco, A.M. (2003). Dispositional traits and
turnover intention. International Journal of Manpower, 24(4), 284-298.
Chow, G. (1960). Tests of equality between sets of coefficients in
two linear regressions. Econometrica, 28(3), 591- 605.
Cooper, R. & Payne, R. (1967). Extraversion and some aspects of
work behavior. Personnel Psychology, 20, 45-57.
Cropanzano, R., & James, K. (1990). Some methodological
considerations for the behavioral genetic analysis of work attitudes.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 433-439.
Cropanzano, R., James, K., & Konovsky, M. A. (1993).
Dispositional affectivity as a predictor of work attitudes and job
performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 595-606.
Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). The independence of
positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 47, 1105-1117.
Ferris, G. R., Youngblood, S. A. & Yates, V. L. (1985).
Personality, training performance, and withdrawal: A test of the
person-group fit hypothesis for organizational newcomers. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 27, 377-388.
Fisher, C.D. (2002). Antecedents and consequences of real-time
affective reactions at work. Motivation and Emotion, 26(1), 3-30.
Fortunato, V. J., & Stone-Romero, E. F. (1999). Taking the
strain out of negative affectivity: Development and initial validation
of scores on a strain-free measure of negative affectivity. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 59, 77-97.
Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review
of General Psychology, 2, 300-319.
Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive
psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American
Psychologist, 56, 218-226.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Branigan, C.A. (2005). Positive emotions
broaden the scope of attention and thought action repertoires. Cognition
and Emotions, 19, 323-332.
Fredrickson, B.L. & Losada, M.F. (2005). Positive affect and
the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60,
678-686.
Fredrickson, B., Tugade, M., Waugh, C. & Larkin, G. (2003).
What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of
resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United
States on the September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 365-376.
Furnham, A. & Miller, T. (1997). Personality, absenteeism and
productivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 705-707.
George, J. M. (1992). The role of personality in organizational
life: Issues and evidence. Journal of Management, 18, 185-213.
Hochwarter, W.A., Kiewitz, C., Castro, S.L., Perrewe, P.L. &
Ferris, G.R. (2003). Positive affectivity and collective efficacy as
moderators of the relationship between perceived politics and job
satisfaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(5), 1009-1035.
Hulin, C. & Blood, M. (1968). Job enlargement, individual
differences, and worker responses. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 41-55.
Ironson, G. H., Smith, P., Brannick, M. T., Gibson, W. M., &
Paul, K. B. (1989). Construction of a job in general scale: A comparison
of global, composite, and specific measures. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 74, 193-200.
Isen, A.M., Daubman, K.A., & Nowicki, G.P. (1987). Positive
affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 52, 1122-1131.
Iverson, R.D., & Deery, S.J. (2001). Understanding the
"personalogical" basis of employee withdrawal: The influence
of affective disposition on employee tardiness, early departure, and
absenteeism. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86 (5), 856-866.
Judge, T.A. (1993). Does affective disposition moderate the
relationship between job satisfaction and voluntary turnover? Journal of
Applied Psychology, 78, 395-401.
Levin, K., & Stokes, J. P. (1989). Dispositional approach to
job satisfaction: Role of negative affectivity. Journal of Applied
Psychology.
Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component
conceptualization of organizational commitment. Human Resource
Management Review, 1, 61-98.
Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1982).
Employee-organization linkages: The psychology of commitment,
absenteeism, and turnover. New York: Academic Press.
Newton, T., & Keenan, T. (1991). Further analyses of the
dispositional argument in organizational behavior. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 76, 781-787.
O'Reilly, C. A., III, Chatman, J., & Caldwell, D. F.
(1991). People and organizational culture: A profile comparison approach
to assessing person-organization fit. Academy of Management Journal^ 34,
487-516.
Price, J. (2001). Reflections on the determinants of voluntary
turnover. International Journal of Manpower, 22(7), 600624.
Smith, P., Kendall, L. & Hulin, C. (1969). The measurement of
satisfaction in work and retirement. Chicago: Rand.
Seligman, M. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (Eds.) (2000). Positive
psychology (Special Iss.). American Psychologist, 55, 1.
Staw, B. M., Bell, N. E., & Clausen, J. A. (1986). The
dispositional approach to job attitudes: A lifetime longitudinal test.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 31, 56-77.
Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of
change: A dispositional approach to job attitudes. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 70, 469-480.
Stone-Romero, E. F. (2005). Personality-based stigmas and unfair
discrimination in work organizations. In R. L. Dipboye, & A. Colella
(Eds.), Discrimination at work: The psychological and organizational
bases (pp. 255280). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Watson, D. (1988a) Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of
positive and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints,
perceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 1020-1030.
Watson, D. (1988b) The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of
varying descriptors, time frame, and response formats on measures of
Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 55, 128141.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and
negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive
disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 346-353.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development
and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
1063-1070.
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual
structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235.
Elizabeth J. Rozell, Missouri State University
Charles E. Pettijohn, Nova Southeastern University
R. Stephen Parker, Missouri State University
Table 1: Demographic Attributes
Gender Frequency-Percentage
Male 272-92.5%
Female 22-7.5%
Skill Level
High 94-32.9%
Med-High 69-24.1%
Low 65-22.7%
Shipping 27-9.4%
Maintenance 31-10.8%
Education
Less than High School 46-16.0%
High School 132-46.0%
Some College 79-27.5%
Associates 11-3.8%
Bachelor's 5-1.7%
Graduate 2-.7%
Other 12-4.2%
Marital
Single 50-17.2%
Married 197-67.9%
Widowed 2-.7%
Divorced 41-14.1%
Average Years Worked in Company 11.3
Average Years Worked in Job 7.2
Table 2: Relationship Between Positive/Negative Affect and the
Dependent Variables
Dependent Independent
Variable (R2) Variable (B) F-Value Significance
White Collar Commit (.21) Positive (.46) 65.6 < .0001
Blue Collar Commit (.25) Positive (.51) 107.8 < .0001
White Collar Turnover (.09) Positive (-.31) 18.8 < .0001
Blue Collar Turnover (.22) Positive (-.47) 60.3 < .0001
White Collar Job Satis. (.25) Positive (.50) 80.0 < .0001
Blue Collar Job Satis. (.23) Positive (.48) 94.8 < .0001
White Collar Absent (-.003) Positive (.02) .11 .74
Blue Collar Absent (-.003) Positive (.03) .20 .65
White Collar Tardy (.03) Positive (-.17) 7.6 .007
Blue Collar Tardy (-.003) Positive (.02) .1 .71
White Collar Commit (.03) Negative (-.17) 7.4 .007
Blue Collar Commit (.01) Negative (-.10) 3.7 .05
White Collar Turnover (.09) Negative (.31) 18.7 < .0001
Blue Collar Turnover (.07) Negative (.27) 15.9 < .0001
White Collar Job Satis. (.17) Negative (-.41) 49.7 < .0001
Blue Collar Job Satis. (.15) Negative (-.38) 55.3 < .0001
White Collar Absent (-.004) Negative (.008) .01 .90
Blue Collar Absent ( .010) Negative (.10) 2.93 .09
White Collar Tardy (.03) Negative (.17) 6.9 .007
Blue Collar Tardy (.02) Negative (.15) 7.6 .006
Table 3: Comparisons of Models Using the Chow Test
Dependent Positive Negative
Variable (R2) Affect (p) Affect (p)
Full Model Commit (.26) .63 (<.0001) -.10 (.0843)
White Collar Commit (.21) .59 (<.0001) -.07 (.4661)
Blue Collar Commit (.26) .60 (<.0001) -.07 (.3492)
Chow Test
Full Model Turnover (.23) -.36 (<.0001) .24 (<.0001)
White Collar Turnover (.14) -.22 (.0015) .30 (.0010)
Blue Collar Turnover (.26) -.42 (<.0001) .18 (.0120)
Chow Test
Full Model Job Satis. (.37) .55 (<.0001) -.51 (<.0001)
White Collar Job Satis. (.33) .45 (<.0001) -.43 (<.0001)
Blue Collar Job Satis. (.34) .56 (<.0001) -.51 (<.0001)
Chow Test
Full Model Absent (.01) .01 (.7437) .08 (.0222)
White Collar Absent (-.01) .01 (.7502) .01 (.8206)
Blue Collar Absent (.01) .03 (.4568) .10 (. 0728)
Chow Test
Full Model Tardy (.02) .00 (.8130) .04 (.0012)
White Collar Tardy (.04) -.03 (.0343) .03 (.0669)
Blue Collar Tardy (.03) .01 (.2873) .04 (.0047)
Chow Test
F-Value Significance
Full Model 98.7 < .0001
White Collar 32.2 < .0001
Blue Collar 53.7 < .0001
Chow Test 1.8 .1461
Full Model 56.4 < .0001
White Collar 15.7 < .0001
Blue Collar 36.6 < .0001
Chow Test 2.4 .07
Full Model 162.2 < .0001
White Collar 59.2 < .0001
Blue Collar 80.5 < .0001
Chow Test 4.9 .0021
Full Model 2.7 .0711
White Collar .06 .9419.
Blue Collar 1.7 .1791
Chow Test 3.1 .0282
Full Model 5.5 .0045
White Collar 5.7 .0040
Blue Collar 4.3 .0150
Chow Test 1.8 .1545
* regression coefficients are standardized