Perceived forgiveness from god and self-forgiveness.
McConnell, John M. ; Dixon, David N.
Perceived Forgiveness from God and Self-Forgiveness
Self-forgiveness is an adaptive process by which people replace
guilt, shame, and self-punishment with self-benevolent beliefs,
feelings, and actions (Enright, 1996; Exline, Root, Yadavalli, Martin,
& Fisher, 2011; Fisher & Exline, 2006, 2010; Hall & Fincham,
2005, 2008; Wohl, DeShea, & Wahkinney, 2008). People move through
the self-forgiveness process by spiritual, intrapsychic, and
interpersonal means that require time and effort (Fisher & Exline,
2006, 2010; Hall & Fincham, 2005, 2008). Hall and Fincham (2005)
proposed that guilt, shame, empathy, conciliatory behavior,
attributions, offense severity, and perceived forgiveness all play
important roles in the process of self-forgiveness. Indeed, the extant
literature suggests feeling forgiven from God facilitates
self-forgiveness (Hall & Fincham, 2005, 2008). Although Christians
believe God is forgiving, some may paradoxically believe God does not
forgive them personally. Consequently, people may simultaneously hold
general and personalized views of God. A general God concept is akin to
a theological definition of God, whereas a personalized God image is
experiential (Lawrence, 1997; Rizzuto, 1979). Along this line of
reasoning, we hypothesized that viewing God as forgiving in personal
matters would relate to self-forgiveness, but general views on
God's forgiving nature would not. The current study tested Hall and
Fincham's (2005) hypothesis that perceived forgiveness from God
increases the likelihood of self-forgiveness. In order to test this
hypothesis, we assessed participants' general and personal views of
a forgiving or punitive God as a correlate with participants'
degree of self-forgiveness.
Proposed Antecedent Variables of Self-Forgiveness
Guilt and Shame. People who commit transgressions may experience
guilt and shame through tension, remorse, or regret (Tangney, 1995).
Transgressors are most likely to experience guilt as a state (e.g.,
situational) and shame as a trait (e.g., character flaw). Compared to
shame, Hall and Fincham (2005) theorized that guilt might not have as
much of a negative correlation with self-forgiveness because it is
"other-oriented." Shame seems to be a stronger negative
correlate with self-forgiveness because it is naturally more
"self-centered" (Hall & Fin cham, 2005; Lewis, 1971;
Tangney, 1995). Hall and Fincham (2005) theorized that the
"otheroriented" focus of guilt is more likely than shame to
lead to conciliatory behavior. Conciliatory behavior includes apologies,
reparations, gifts, or acts of service aimed to reduce guilt and/or
shame. Alternatively, the self-focus of shame is more likely to lead to
destructive criticism, especially when transgressors label their
transgressions as character flaws. Furthermore, shame may evoke an
avoidance response in relation to the transgression (Tangney, 1995),
perhaps inhibiting self-forgiveness. This destructive avoidant response
inherent to shame may affect the transgressors' relational
interactions with God. For instance, transgressors may avoid God for
fear of being punished. Thus, transgressors who experience guilt, as
opposed to shame, are more likely to initiate healthy forms of
reparative interactions with God. Conversely, transgressors who
experience shame are more likely to instigate maladaptive interactions,
such as avoidance of self-forgiveness and reconciliation with God. The
disparate behavior correlates of guilt and shame are consistent with
results from Hall and Fincham (2008), who found guilt, but not shame,
related to self-forgiveness above and beyond the passage of time.
Empathy and Conciliatory Behavior. Hall and Fincham (2005) stated
that conciliatory behavior serves as an attempt to dissolve the guilt
and shame transgressors have experienced. When transgressors thought in
remorseful ways, pro-social behavior such as humility, repentance, and
conciliatory behavior positively correlated with self-forgiveness
(Fisher & Exline, 2006). Hall and Fincham (2008) found conciliatory
behaviors positively related to self-forgiveness above and beyond time.
In addition, empathy for others has positively correlated with the
inability to self-forgive (Zechmeister & Romero, 2002). However,
researchers also have found that empathy is unrelated or weakly related
to self-forgiveness (cf., Barbetta, 2002; Hall & Fincham, 2008;
Macaskill, Maltby, & Day, 2002; McConnell, 2009). A problem can
occur when transgressors' empathy and consequential guilt are the
causes for conciliatory behaviors (Hall & Fincham, 2005). In this
case, transgressors may feel obligated to make continuous compensatory
offers to their victims or God. Due to residual guilt or shame,
transgressors may not feel or believe others have forgiven them.
Additionally, many transgressors may believe self-forgiveness is a sign
of disrespect and consequently not acceptable.
Attributions. Hall and Fincham (2005) theorized that external,
unstable, and/or global attributions are likely to increase
self-forgiveness, although it is likely that these attributions may be a
case of pseudo-self-forgiveness. In other words, transgressors may be
quick to "forgive" themselves by not taking full
responsibility for their actions. For instance, an abusive father may
place blame for his abusive actions on his son's behavioral
problems. Such internal dialogue might be, "I didn't do
anything wrong, he deserved what he got." Placing blame on
external, unstable, and/or global attributions may be a self-serving
mechanism to avoid the high amounts of energy that true self-forgiveness
involves (Hall & Fincham, 2005). In fact, narcissistic traits and
blaming victims positively related with self-forgiveness in two studies
(Strelan, 2007; Zechmeister & Romero, 2002). On the other hand,
internal, stable, and/or specific attributions are likely to correlate
negatively with forgiveness of self (Hall & Fincham, 2005), due to
self-assigned responsibility and consequential guilt and/or shame. Hall
and Fincham (2008), however, found changes in attributions were
unrelated to self-forgiveness when they controlled for the passage of
time.
Severity of Transgression. Hall and Fincham (2005) theorized that
more severe transgressions might correlate with the lack of
self-forgiveness. Several studies have pointed towards increased guilt
and shame associated with transgressions with greater perceived severity
(Fisher & Exline, 2006; Ingersoll-Dayton & Krause, 2005;
McConnell, 2009). Hall and Fincham (2008) found changes in perception of
transgression severity impacted levels of self-forgiveness beyond the
variance accounted for by time. In this sense, transgressors may feel
that more severe transgressions are too grave to permit
self-forgiveness.
Perceived Forgiveness. Perceived forgiveness from victims or God
also is a factor that Hall and Fincham (2005) thought correlated with
self-forgiveness. Transgressors accomplish perceived forgiveness when
they believe that their victims have forgiven them; that is, remitted
resentments, condemnations, and desires for revenge, alongside
experiencing compassion, generosity, and/or love (Enright & The
Human Development Study Group, 1991). Perceived forgiveness may allow
transgressors to remit their guilty and/or shameful feelings associated
with their transgressions because their victims have freely chosen to
abandon seeing them in negative ways. Consequently, transgressors may
move past associated feelings by having senses of "being filled ...
and ... forgiven" (Bauer et al., 1992, p. 157). one surgeon noted,
"In all that process I had a sense that he forgave me. That was
very powerful for me" (Gerber, 1990, p. 79). For transgressors, it
may be a small jump from believing their victims have forgiven them to
believing it is now okay to self-forgive. Transgressors can experience
guilt and/or shame "internally" in relation to their selves
and "horizontally" in relation to other persons, but also
"vertically" in relation to God.
Several studies have given support that transgressors'
perceived forgiveness from victims or their higher power is positively
associated with self-forgiveness beyond the variance accounted for by
time (Hall & Fincham, 2008; Martin, 2008; Witvliet, Ludwig, &
Bauer, 2002). Witvliet and colleagues (2002) found imagining mercy from
a victim resulted in physiological responses that were consistent with
the augmentation of positive emotions and the alleviation of negative
emotions. Martin (2008) asked participants to recall a specific event in
which they did something wrong to another person. Martin found that
experiences of forgiveness from God uniquely predicted
transgressors' self-forgiveness in two samples. Using Hierarchical
Linear Modeling, Hall and Fincham (2008) found that transgressors'
perceived forgiveness from their victims uniquely predicted their
self-forgiveness above and beyond time. Nevertheless, in a qualitative
study, Layer, Roberts, Wild, and Walters (2004) found many transgressors
had feelings of forgiveness from their higher power, yet still struggled
with the process of self-forgiveness. The current study tested the
hypothesis that self-forgiveness is a possible antecedent variable of
perceived forgiveness from God in personal instances.
Stage Models of Self-Forgiveness
Enright (1996) theorized that in order for transgressors to
experience self-forgiveness, they must progress through a stage model:
uncovering, decision, work, and outcome stages. The exact progression
through this model is likely to differ across people and cultures.
First, transgressors work through possible denial before they accept
responsibility in the uncovering stage (Enright, 1996). If transgressors
maintain denial, they will not progress through subsequent stages.
Consequently, maintaining denial fosters pseudo-self-forgiveness; that
is, self-forgiveness that avoids taking responsibility for harmful
actions. Upon accepting responsibility, transgressors may experience
guilt and/or shame ruminations, event ruminations, and understanding of
the relative deprivation and possible permanence the transgression may
have on the victim. They may believe that they have disappointed God and
that they are not worthy of God's love. Accordingly, transgressors
have changed senses of selves, perhaps resulting in self-condemnations,
self-criticisms, and lowered self-esteems. In the next stage, the
decision stage, transgressors have changed hearts and realize that they
must change the course of their behavior. In this stage, transgressors
may seek forgiveness from their victims and/or God through apologies or
other actions. Persons may pray to God and apologize for their
wrongdoings. If the victim accepts the apology or they feel like God has
forgiven them, this may accelerate the transgressors' progress
through the work and outcome stages. once transgressors consider
self-forgiveness as an option, they must make a commitment to follow
through with the remaining course of action. Through the third stage,
the work stage, transgressors form new senses of selfawareness and
self-compassion (Enright, 1996). Varying degrees of guilt and/or shame,
empathy, conciliatory behavior, severity of the transgression, and
beliefs about God's forgiveness impact the difficulty of passing
through the work stage. Lastly, in the outcome stage, transgressors find
new meaning in their transgressions and they experience growth and
release (i.e., feelings of self-forgiveness; Enright, 1996).
Worthington (2006) offered an alternative stage model of
self-forgiveness that he coined the "Five Steps to REACH
Forgiveness"; that is, (R)ecall the hurt, (E)mpathy towards the
self, (A)lturistic gift of forgiveness, (C)ommitment to forgive, and
(H)old on to forgiveness. Through this model transgressors learn to take
responsibility, exercise compassion, extend forgiveness to themselves,
and maintain senses of self-forgiveness (Worthington, 2006). During the
REACH model of self-forgiveness, transgressors learn how to take a
rational look at whether they have met religious and personal conditions
of forgiveness. Flanigan's (1996) self-help book offers another
approach to self-forgiveness.
Purpose of Study
As previously discussed, transgressors seeking self-forgiveness may
feel self-forgiveness is a sign of disrespect to their victims.
Therefore, when transgressors perceive their harmful behavior as a
transgression also against God, they may also feel self-forgiveness is a
sign of disrespect to God. Consequently, they may feel obligated to
offer continuous compensatory offers (e.g., prayers, praises,
confessions, and repentances). Some people may include a spiritual
dimension in their self-forgiveness processes. In this case,
self-forgiveness is especially problematic if transgressors view God as
unforgiving. This is supported by research (Cafaro-Martin & Exline,
2006), in which a confessional prayer style correlated with viewing God
as unforgiving. In this case, the guilt and/or shame felt
"vertically" by transgressors could only hinder the ability to
self-forgive. Based on Cafaro-Martin and Exline's (2003) findings
that views of God as forgiving positively correlated with
self-forgiveness, Hall and Fincham (2005) hypothesized:
There is preliminary evidence to suggest
that perceived forgiveness from
God is positively associated with
self-forgiveness. Cafaro and Exline
(2003) asked individuals to focus on
an incident in which they had offended
God, and found that self-forgiveness
was positively correlated with
believing that God had forgiven the
self for the transgression. (p. 633)
The current study extends this research from self-forgiveness of a
specific incident (e.g., Cafaro-Martin & Exline, 2003; Hall &
Fincham, 2008; Martin, 2008) to a person's widespread belief about
God and self-forgiveness in many instances. The study investigated the
correlation between views of God as forgiving or punitive and degrees of
self-forgiveness.
Method
Participants
Participants (male [27], female [63]) were 90 undergraduate
students (freshman [15.6%], sophomore [16.7%], junior [37.8%], and
senior [30%]) at a mid-western university who completed the scales for
partial fulfillment of a psychology course requirement. Ages ranged from
18-25 (M= 20.73, SD= 1.36). The sample consisted of 82.2% Caucasian, 10%
African-American, 4.4% Hispanic/Mexican-American, and 3.3% mixed race.
Religious affiliation was determined by checking one of the following
categories: Protestant (31.1%), Catholic (23.3%), Buddhist (2.2.%),
Hindu (1.1%), Agnostic (7.8%), and other (34.5%). Those who checked the
other category indicated nondenominational Christian (14.4%), various
denominational affiliations (17.7%, i.e., Apostolic, Baptist, Lutheran,
Methodist, Mormon, Pentecostal, or Presbyterian), and undecided (2.2%).
Implications of these demographics are discussed in the limitations
section.
Procedures
Complications in validity could arise if the priming of
self-forgiveness prompted a certain response on the God forgiveness
measures, or visa-versa. Thus, we counterbalanced the administration of
the scales to control for such demand characteristics. In other words,
participants--evenly distributed among three groups-received the
materials in three separate orders. The counterbalance procedure allowed
us to assume that order of presentation was not an extraneous factor.
Upon administration, we instructed the participants to indicate their
responses to the questions, as they typically felt, on the three scales
and a demographic questionnaire. The principal investigator
administrated the measures in small group settings (i.e., 5-15
students).
Materials
Adjective Ratings of God (ARG): Wrathful ness Subscale (Modified).
To quantify the participants' broad conceptualization of God (i.e.,
forgiving or punitive), we modified the ARG subscale (Gorsuch, 1968).
The original Wrathfulness subscale by Gorsuch (1968) reported a
reliability coefficient of .83; however, reliability of the modified
version still needed to be confirmed. Factor analysis research with the
original scale supported the Wrathfulness subscale as an independent
factor (Hill & Hood, 1999).
We modified the subscale (13-items) to address the degree to which
the participant conceptualized God as the original adjective (wrathful
oriented) or its antonym (forgiving oriented). The italicized adjectives
indicate the item from the original subscale, whereas we generated the
antonyms. The items included avenging vs. tolerant, blunt vs. sensitive,
critical vs. accepting, cruel vs. kind, damning vs. patient, hard vs.
soft, jealous vs. trusting, punishing vs. lenient, severe vs. mild,
sharp vs. even, stern vs. agreeable, tough vs. tender, and wrathful vs.
pardoning. The modified scale used a 7-point scale between the
adjectives (i.e., original items and antonyms) to quantify
participants' conceptualization of God's forgiveness.
Instructions were for participants to select the number that most
applied to their views of God.
We conducted a Cronbach's alpha on the 13item modified scale,
which obtained a high internal consistency reliability (M= 70.23, SD=
13.57, [alpha]= .91). Thus, we concluded that we could sum the scale
items together. Additionally, we calculated separate reliabilities for
men and women. We found the female subset to have a similar alpha level
(M= 70.52, SD= 13.66, a= .92) to the male subset (M= 69.56, SD= 13.59,
a= .89).
God Image Inventory (GII), Acceptance Subscale (Modified). To
quantify personalized views of how the participants conceptualize God
(i.e., forgiving or punitive), we modified the GII Acceptance subscale
(Lawrence, 1997). Lawrence (1997) found the original GII subscale to
have an internal consistency reliability of .90. A self-esteem scale
correlated positively (.54) with the Acceptance subscale (Lawrence,
1997). The theoretical basis for modifying Lawrence's (1997)
subscale was to address more directly the construct of forgiveness from
God. We viewed the two constructs of acceptance (i.e., love and
forgiveness) from God as closely related.
There were 14-items selected from the 59item Acceptance subscale;
omission of items was due to their irrelevance to forgiveness. In
addition, we modified some items (i.e., 2, 3, 10, 11, 13, and 14) by
replacing love with forgive, to address more directly the construct of
forgiveness from God. The following are the original items with the
italicized modified words from the Acceptance subscale. 1. When I do
wrong, God's back is turned to me (reverse score). 2. I am
sometimes anxious about whether God forgives me (reverse score). 3. I am
confident of God forgiving me. 4. I know I am not perfect, but God loves
me anyway. 5. I have sometimes felt that I have committed the
unforgivable sin (reverse score). 6. God's love for me has no
strings attached. 7. Even when I do bad things, I know God still
forgives me. 8. God loves me only when I am perfect (reverse score). 9.
God loves me regardless. 10. Sometimes I feel that God doesn't
forgive me anymore (reverse score). 11. I worry about whether God can
forgive me (reverse score). 12. God's love for me is unconditional.
13. God forgives a lot of other people more than me (reverse score). 14.
I am not good enough for God's forgiveness (reverse score). All 14
items were quantified with a 7-point scale with 1 (Almost Always False
of Me), 4 (Undecided), and 7 (Almost Always True of Me).
We calculated a Cronbach's alpha on the 14item modified scale,
which demonstrated a high internal consistency reliability (M= 82.97,
SD= 12.84, [alpha] = .84). Thus, we concluded that we could sum the
scale items together. Additionally, we calculated separate reliabilities
for men and women. We found the female subset to have a similar alpha
level (M= 83.6, SD= 12.65, [alpha]= .85) to the male subset (M= 81.48,
SD= 13.37, [alpha]= .82).
Heartland Self-Forgiveness Subscale. To quantify different items
relating to forgiveness of self, we used the Heartland Self-Forgiveness
subscale (HFS) (Thompson et al., 2005). The HFS has demonstrated
desirable levels of convergent validity and internal consistency
reliability. The HFS subscale has also shown Cronbach's alpha
ranging from .72 to .76 across three samples, a high level of
inter-rater reliability (.95), and test-retest reliabilities for a
3-week interval (.72) and a 9-month interval (.69; Thompson et al.,
2005).
For the focus of this study, we used all six items of the
Self-Forgiveness subscale, including, 1. Although I feel bad at first
when I mess up, over time I can give myself some slack. 2. I hold
grudges against myself for negative things I've done (reverse
score). 3. Learning from bad things that I've done helps me get
over them. 4. It is really hard for me to accept myself once I've
messed up (reverse score). 5. With time I am understanding of myself for
mistakes I've made. 6. I don't stop criticizing myself for
negative things I've felt, thought, said, or done (reverse score).
All items were quantified with a 7-point scale with 1 (Almost Always
False of Me), 4 (Undecided), and 7 (Almost Always True of Me).
We calculated a Cronbach's alpha on the 6item subscale, which
elicited a moderate internal consistency reliability (M= 30.1, SD= 5.41,
[alpha]= .64). Thus, we concluded that we could sum the scale items
together. Additionally, we calculated separate reliabilities for men and
women. We found the male subset to have a higher alpha (M= 29.07, SD=
6.71, [alpha]= .70) than the female subset (M= 30.54, SD= 4.75, [alpha]=
.58).
Results
Correlations
Predictor Correlations. We expected that the modified ARG subscale
would significantly correlate with the modified GII subscale. We
conducted a Pearson-r correlation on the two scale sums. The correlation
between the ARG and GII modified subscales although significant, was
only moderate, r(90)= .44, p < .01. The results suggest that the two
scales were measuring related, but not identical constructs.
ARG: Wrathfulness Subscale (Modified).
We expected that the modified Adjective Ratings of God subscale
would not significantly correlate with the HFS Self-Forgiveness
subscale. We conducted a Pearson-r correlation on the two scale sums.
The two scales failed to be significantly correlated, r(90)= .14. The
results suggest that the participants' general views of God's
forgiveness do not correlate with their self-forgiveness.
GII: Acceptance Subscale (Modified). We expected that the modified
GII subscale would significantly correlate with the HFS Self-Forgiveness
subscale. We conducted a Pearson-r correlation on the two scale sums.
The two scales were found to be significantly correlated, r(90)= .38, p
< .01. The male subset was found to have a larger correlation, r(27)=
.57, p < .01, than the female subset, r(63)= .26, p< .01. The
results suggest that the participants' personal views of God's
forgiveness do in fact correlate with their degree of self-forgiveness,
more so for men than women.
Discussion
We found a personal, but not a general, perceived forgiveness from
God significantly correlated with self-forgiveness. The respective
findings seem to make sense because self-forgiveness largely depends on
personal, not global, matters. Thus, viewing God's forgiveness in a
more personal, as opposed to a general, form relates more to
self-forgiveness. Scores on the modified GII accounted for 14.6% of the
variance, which supports Hall and Fincham's (2005) hypothesis that
the two constructs positively correlate.
We found the male subset had a higher correlation than the female
subset. A speculative explanation for these findings could be attributed
to pseudo-self-forgiveness in males. McMahan (1982) found men more often
attributed their successes to internal causes and their failures to
external situations (i.e., partial blame of victim, self-serving bias),
whereas women more often attributed their successes to external
situations and their failures to internal factors. Even so, this
tendency found in McMahan (1982) may have an association to males'
traditional instrumental personality and/or females' customary
communal personality (Welch, Gerard, & Huston, 1986). As Snodgrass
(1985) discussed, higher degrees of empathy are more a function of
position (e.g., nurturing roles), not genetics. Consequently, as a
speculative interpretation, women's conventionally nurturing role
in American society may have augmented levels of empathy, thereby
inhibiting self-forgiveness. Future research should further explore this
unexpected gender difference.
Limitations
A non-representative distribution of religious affiliations, class
standings, and races limits generalizability. Furthermore, the use of
undergraduate students as participants for partial fulfillment of a
psychology course requirement also limits generalizability. Future
research should address cross-cultural replications because the relation
between perceived forgiveness from God and self-forgiveness may differ
across religions and cultures. Nevertheless, university students may be
a particularly relevant group because of their level of spiritual
development. Related to this development, Marcia (1966) describes four
ego identity statuses: 1. moratorium (i.e., identity
crisis/questioning), 2. identity achievement (i.e., successful identity
resolution and commitment after moratorium), 3. foreclosure (i.e.,
identity commitment without moratorium), and 4. diffusion (i.e., lack of
commitment and lack of moratorium). The identities of college-age
students undergo significant changes from their freshman to senior years
characterized by increased identity achievement (Sanders, 1998;
Waterman, Geary, & Waterman, 1974). The current study consisted of
mostly juniors and seniors (67.8%) as compared to freshmen and
sophomores (32.2%), representing students at various ego identity
statuses, and, for many, in the process of changes in their identities.
Modification of scales and extracting specific items (i.e., ARG
& GII) may have compromised validity. Thus, there needs to be
replications of the current findings after further construct validation
of the modified scales. Further, although the HFS Self-Forgiveness
subscale reliability was acceptable (Cronbach's alpha= .64), it was
less than optimal. In addition, the HFS subscale did not assess the
participants' levels of pseudo-self-forgiveness. Future research
could increase reliability with an increased number of items and by
addressing pseudo-self-forgiveness. of course, there are many other
variables involved in self-forgiveness, as perceived forgiveness from
God accounted for only 14.6% of the variance. Additionally, because of
the correlational method of research, assuming perceived forgiveness
from God causes higher levels of self-forgiveness is problematic.
Therapeutic Implications
Cognitive. Transgressors will tend to act in accordance with the
attitudes they have of themselves (Bem, 1967). This can be especially
problematic, because transgressors who cannot forgive themselves due to
faulty attributions (e.g., I am a horrible person and I will always be
this way, therefore God will never forgive me, why should I forgive
myself?) are likely to act on these attributions. In other words,
transgressors may continue to transgress due to self-fulfilling
prophecies and a lack of perceived efficacy in the forgiveness process.
In addition, when people have residual guilt and/or shame, excessive
rumination is possible (Enright, 1996). Inasmuch as ruminations of
transgressions continue, thoughts such as desires, methods, and outlets
of recommitting transgressions are possibly salient to transgressors.
These thoughts may facilitate temptation to fall back into old habits,
thus inhibiting self-forgiveness and reconciliation with God. In this
sense, thought stopping may assist in breaking the chain that leads to
transgressions. Hence, thought stopping may allow transgressors to avoid
recurring temptations, thereby enabling them to move forward past
chronic transgressions.
Emotional. Distress may come from clients' emotional binds
(Wubbolding, 2002). These emotional binds derive from personal standards
(e.g., "I should be good enough to never require forgiveness";
Glasser, 1965). Applying reality therapy, practitioners can help resolve
guilt and shame (Turnage, Jacinto, & Kirven, 2003) to help relieve
the clients' distress. Therapists also must focus on the present
and future when considering self-forgiveness situations. Turnage and
colleagues (2003) stated, "Although focusing on past faulty
thinking could be helpful as a measure to resolve anger, solely focusing
on the past would not allow the person to accept personal responsibility
and move forward" (p. 24).
Spiritual. To better foster self-forgiveness and reconciliation
with God, therapists may want to assess the clients' views of God
as forgiving or punitive. Possible complications could arise in the
self-forgiveness process if clients view God as punitive. As Lawrence
(1997) pointed out, people's image of God is likely consistent with
their own self-concept. Therapists therefore may highlight or develop
their clients' forgiving nature in order to make God's greater
forgiveness more salient. Additionally, the God image is, in part, a
mental reification of parental figures (Lawrence, 1997) so therapists
may help their clients reconceptualize their parental schemas.
Ultimately, the therapist may want to help the client work through
destructive guilt and/or shame experienced vertically. This may help
clients accelerate the process of feeling forgiven from God and moving
towards self-forgiveness.
Moral and Relationship Strengthening. It is important to note that
self-forgiveness counseling is not only limited to attempts to attenuate
clients' guilt and/or shame. Forgiveness counseling should
correspond with moral strengthening such as increasing the desire not to
commit further transgressions. As clients continue to practice the
forgiveness triad (i.e., forgiving others, receiving forgiveness, and
self-forgiveness), they may be able to gain moral strength (Enright,
1996). Counseling from a forgiveness perspective can assist clients in
understanding the relationship between receiving forgiveness and
self-forgiveness (Enright, 1996). This understanding might take the
focus off the transgressions and place the focus on clients'
relationship with God, as well as intrinsic self-worth and/or respect
(Enright, 1996). Thus, forgiveness insight may aid in spiritual healing
and heightened moral strength. Therapists may be able to facilitate this
process while considering Enright's (1996), Worthington's
(2006), or Flanigan's (1996) stage models of self-forgiveness.
Conclusion
There are many proposed antecedent variables of self-forgiveness
(e.g., guilt, shame, empathy, conciliatory behavior, attributions,
severity of offense, and perceived forgiveness). one proposed
antecedent, perceived forgiveness from God, might play an important role
in the self-forgiveness process. The current study was concerned with
perceived forgiveness from God as a possible antecedent variable of
self-forgiveness. The findings extended the research from
self-forgiveness of a specific incident to a person's widespread
belief about God and self-forgiveness. We found the modified GII
subscale (i.e., personal forgiving God dimension) positively correlated
with the HFS subscale (i.e., self-forgiveness dimension), but the
modified subscale (i.e., general forgiving God dimension) was not. These
findings suggest viewing God's forgiveness in a more personal form,
as opposed to a general view, relates more to self-forgiveness.
Therapists are directly involved in assisting people with the process of
seeking and extending forgiveness. Forgiveness of self and perceived
forgiveness from God are perhaps intimately connected to the ability to
accept and extend forgiveness.
References
Barbetta, F. (2002). An exploration of the relationship between
empathy, forgiving others and self--forgiveness. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 64(1), 407B. (UMI No. 3079194).
Bauer, L., Duffy, J., Fountain, E., Halling, S., Holzer, M., &
Jones, E., ... Rowe, J. O. (1992). Exploring self--forgiveness. Journal
of Religion and Health, 31, 149-160.
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation
of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200.
Cafaro-Martin, A., & Exline, J. J. (2003, March). Correlates of
self--forgiveness after offending God. Poster presented at the first
annual Mid--Winter Research Conference on Religion and Spirituality,
Timonium, MD.
Cafaro-Martin, A., & Exline, J. J. (2006, March). A closer look
at divine forgiveness: Correlates of perceiving God as forgiving and
unforgiving. Poster presented at the fourth annual Mid--Winter Research
Conference on Religion and Spirituality, Timonium, MD.
Enright, R. D. (1996). Counseling within the forgiveness triad: on
forgiving, receiving forgiveness, and self--forgiveness. Counseling
& Values, 40, 107-127.
Enright, R. D., & the Human Development Study Group (1991). The
moral development of forgiveness. In W. Kurtines & J. Gewirtz
(Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development (vol. 1, pp.
123-152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Exline, J. J., Root, B. L., Yadavalli, S., Martin, A. M., &
Fisher, M. L. (2011). Reparative behaviors and self-forgiveness: Effects
of a laboratory-based exercise. Self and Identity, 10 , 101-126. doi:
10.1080/15298861003669565
Fisher, M. L., & Exline, J. J. (2006). Self-forgiveness versus
excusing: The roles of remorse, effort, and acceptance of
responsibility. Journal of Self & Identity, 5, 127-146.
doi:10.1080/15298860600586123
Fisher, M. L., & Exline, J. J. (2010). Moving toward self
forgiveness: Removing barriers related to shame, guilt, and regret.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4/8, 548-558. doi:
.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00276.x
Flanigan, B. (1996). Forgiving yourself: A step-by-step guide to
making peace with your mistakes and getting on with your life. New York:
Macmillan.
Gerber, L. A. (1990). Transformation in self-understanding in
surgeons whose treatment efforts were not successful. American Journal
of Psychotherapy, 44, 75-84.
Glasser, W. (1965). Reality therapy: A new approach to psychiatry.
New York: Harper & Row.
Gorsuch, R. L. (1968). The conceptualization of God as seen in
adjective ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 7,
56-64. doi:10.2307/1385110
Hall, J. H., & Fincham F. D. (2005). Self-forgiveness: The
stepchild of forgiveness research. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 24, 621-637. doi:10.1521/jscp.2005.24.5.621
Hall, J. H., & Fincham F. D. (2008). The temporal course of
self-forgiveness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27,
174-202. doi:10.1521/jscp.2008.27.2.174
Hill, P. C., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (1999). Measures of
religiosity. Birmingham, Alabama: Religious Education Press.
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., & Krause, N. (2005). Self-forgiveness: A
component of mental health in later life. Research on Aging, 27,
267-289. doi:.1177/0164027504274122
Lawrence, R. T. (1997). Measuring the image of God: The God image
inventory and the God image scales. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
25, 214-226.
Layer, S. D., Roberts, C., Wild, K., & Walters, J. (2004).
Postabortion grief: Evaluating the possible efficacy of a spiritual
group intervention. Research on Social Work Practice, 14, 344-350.
doi:10.1177/1049731504265829
Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York:
International Press.
Macaskill, A., Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Forgiveness of
self and others and emotional empathy. The Journal of Social Psychology,
142, 633-665.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity
status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558.
doi:10.1037/h0023281
Martin, A. M. (2008). Exploring forgiveness: The relationship
between feeling forgiven by God and self-forgiveness for an
interpersonal offense. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
University Microforms International (3321224).
McConnell, J. M. (2009). Confirming a model of self-forgiveness.
(Unpublished master's thesis). Ball State University, Muncie, IN.
McMahan, I. D. (1982). Expectancy of success on sex linked tasks.
Sex Roles, 8, 949-958. doi:10.1007/BF00290019
Rizzuto, A. M. (1979). The birth of the living God: A
psychoanalytic study. : University of Chicago Press.
Sanders, J. L. (1998). Religious ego identity and its relationship
to faith maturity. The Journal of Psychology, 132, 653-658.
Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women's intuition: The effect of
subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 49(1), 146-155. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.146
Strelan, P. (2007). Who forgives others, themselves, and
situations? The roles of narcissism, guilt, self-esteem, and
agreeableness. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 259-269. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.017
Tangney, J. P. (1995). Recent advances in the empirical study of
shame and guilt. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 1132-1145.
doi:10.1177/0002764295038008008
Thompson, L. Y., Snyder, C. R., Hoffman, L., Michael, S. T.,
Rasmussen, H. N., Billings, L. S., ... Roberts, D. E. (2005).
Dispositional forgiveness of self, others, and situations. Journal of
Personality, 73, 313-360. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00311.x
Turnage, B., Jacinto, G., & Kirven, J. (2003). Reality therapy,
domestic violence survivors, and self-forgiveness. International Journal
of Reality Therapy, 22(2), 24-27.
Waterman, A. S., Geary, P. S., Waterman, C. K. (1974). Longitudinal
study of changes in ego identity status from the freshman to the senior
year at college. Developmental Psychology, 10(3), 387-392.
doi:10.1037/h0036438
Welch, R., Gerard, M., & Huston, A. (1986). Gender related
personality attributes and reaction to success/failure: An examination
of mediating variables. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 10, 221-233.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1986.tb00748.x
Witvliet, C. v., Ludwig, T. E., & Bauer, D. J. (2002). Please
forgive me: Transgressors' emotions and physiology during imagery
of seeking forgiveness and victim responses. Journal of Psychology &
Christianity, 21, 219-233.
Wohl, M. J. A., DeShea, L., & Wahkinney, R. L. (2008). Looking
within: Measuring state self-forgiveness and its relationship to
psychological wellbeing. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 40,
1-10. doi: 10.1037/0008-400x.40.1.1.1
Worthington, E. L. (2006). Forgiveness and reconciliation: Theory
and application. New York: Routledge.
Wubbolding, R. E. (2002). Reality therapy for the 21st century.
Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge.
Zechmeister, J. S., & Romero, C. (2002). Victim and offender
accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives of
forgiveness and unforgiveness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 675686. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.675
Authors
John M. McConnell, M.A., Department of Counseling Psychology and
Guidance Services, Ball State University. McConnell earned a B.S. in
Psychological Science and a M.A. in Counseling from Ball State
University. He is currently a third-year Doctoral student of Counseling
Psychology at Ball State University.
David N. Dixon, Ph.D., Professor Emeritus, Department of Counseling
Psychology and Guidance Services, Ball State University. Dixon earned a
B.S. from The Ohio State University, a M.P.S from the University of
Colorado-Boulder, and a Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology from the
University of Minnesota.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John
M. McConnell, Department of Counseling Psychology and Guidance Services,
Ball State University, 2000 W. University Ave., Muncie, IN 47306.
E-mail:
[email protected]
John M. McConnell and David N. Dixon
Ball State University