首页    期刊浏览 2024年12月14日 星期六
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:The relationship of religious and existential variables to scores on the animal-human continuity scale.
  • 作者:King, Frank L. ; Thomas, John C. ; Habermas, Gary R.
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Psychology and Christianity
  • 印刷版ISSN:0733-4273
  • 出版年度:2014
  • 期号:September
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:CAPS International (Christian Association for Psychological Studies)
  • 关键词:Existentialism;Human-animal relationships

The relationship of religious and existential variables to scores on the animal-human continuity scale.


King, Frank L. ; Thomas, John C. ; Habermas, Gary R. 等


Today, in America, there is a split in the philosophical, religious, and scientific worldviews of modern man (Newman, 1987). One aspect of the split is between those whose world view is that animals and humans are dichotomous in nature and those who hold that human beings and animals are on a continuum. At one extreme are creationists who hold the dichotomous view that God created animals first and then created humans in God's own image in a literal week (Genesis 1:20-27). At the other end is a strict evolutionary perspective that suggest that humans only evolved from the lowest cell, a continuum that took hundreds of millions of years to develop, and that humans have no spiritual aspects. There are, of course, world views that could be regarded as a blend of the two. There are many people who believe that the human body can be viewed as a product of evolution but that humanness did not begin until God asserted a soul. Difference in these world views may determine how we treat animals and each other. Mahatma Ghandi said that "the greatness of a nation and its moral progress can be measured by how it treats its animals" (Ridgeway, 2008). The purpose of the present study was to correlate church attendance, strength of belief, and existential life attitudes with the Animal Human Continuity Scale (Templer, Connelly, Bassman, & Hart, 2006), which assesses the extent to which animals and humans are viewed on a continuum versus a dichotomy.

Literature Review

This review of literature has two parts. The first part contains the traditional views of Christian theologians and philosophers that there is a qualitative difference between humans and animals with the former having a spiritual aspect and the latter not possessing such. These scholars often traced the roots of their ideas to Plato and Aristotle. The second part consists of the cognitive abilities in animal research. These two areas should not be viewed as contradictory. Since humans have body structure and functioning similarity with some animals, it is to be expected that brains and associated cognitive processes would have some similarity. An additional caveat is that the authors never conceptualized a one-to-one relationship between one's theological position and score on the Animal-Human Continuity Scale.

Religious and Philosophical Literature

There is a wide variation of attitudes toward animals among naturalists, with some viewing them as genetic cousins, while others see them as inferior species (Darwin, 1859). The book of Genesis records that God created the world in seven days. God's creation of all species of animals on day 6 and then humans on day 7 implies distinct differences and importance. Providing humans with dominion over all living things also emphasizes a unique position of humans. Furthermore, the making of humans in the image of God implies a spiritual aspect to humans.

The Greek philosophers, especially Plato and Aristotle, dealt with the concept of the soul. Plato maintained that the soul continues to live after death and that the quality of life after death is a function of degree of virtue during life. Plato maintains that one goes to either paradise or purgatory for a thousand years following going to paradise or a hell-like existence. Aristotle maintained that plants and animals have a soul but that the human soul is superior and qualitatively different. Aristotle maintained that rational souls should seek intellectual and moral virtue.

Descartes was unquestionably a strict mind-body dualist. He (1993) maintained that animals do not have minds but are like complex machines such as a clock or church organ. He maintained that animals lack immortal souls, reason, and any kind of inner experience, including pain. He maintained that humans have no moral obligation toward animals and he was sometimes viewed as having hostility toward animals. He maintained that the cries of animals that he did experiments on were mechanical and that they felt no pain (Preece & Fraser, 2000). McGoldrick (2012) stressed the roles of free will and consciousness in the Catholic conceptualization of the human soul. He maintained that, although "rational human soul is united to the body at the cellular level the person is able to transcend the body and make free choices" (p. 498), that the souls of only humans, which are not bound by time or place, apprehend truth and beauty and goodness. The Catholic position is anchored in the philosophy of Thomas Aquinas, who maintained that there are different kinds of animal souls that vary as a function of mental ability of the animal. Thomas, however, said that the human soul is completely different because it is rational and immortal. Pope John Paul II in 1990 appeared to view animals in a less disparate fashion than traditional Catholic teaching. He said that animals have souls and that in this respect are identical to all living creatures. This pronouncement is interpreted by some Catholics to mean that animals have immortal souls and by other Catholics to mean that animal souls cease to exist on physical death.

Martin Luther said that humans but not animals have the image of God. He said that animals would not participate in the same eternity as humans but did not rule out the possibility of some sort of existence after death. Luther said that there was a greater discrepancy between humans and animals before the fall and that humans then had greater strength and vision and hearing than animals. They also had greater dominion over animals. Luther at times held up animals as moral examples and contended that animals have greater empathy in regard to the suffering of the same species. He maintained that humans should not be cruel to animals and that animals should be regarded as good gifts that God gave us as stewards (Clough, 2009) .

Bakhos (2009) found that Jewish and Christian and Muslim scholars have viewed humans as superior to animals and to be justified in their subjugation. He cited the Muslim philosopher Al-Hazen in saying that the ranks from highest to lowest are God, angels, humans, the animal kingdom, the vegetable kingdom, and the mineral kingdom. Al-Hazen stated that the human body belongs to the material world but the soul belongs in the spiritual realm. It is apparent that Christian scholars have traditionally maintained that humans have a spiritual and immortal soul but animals do not. There is not complete agreement on what sort of souls, if any, animals possess. There is no complete agreement about the role of evolution and about the cognitive differences between humans and animals. Nevertheless, there tends to be more agreement about a qualitative, spiritually rooted difference.

Scientific Literature on Cognitive Ability in Animals

Older research on cognitive psychology and animals showed that they are able to think abstractly, that relationships between black and grey objects can be abstractly thought through, and psychologists can train animals to distinguish lightness, from grey to white. This ability, therefore, is an indication that animals have some abstract ability (Thomas, 1996). A more reasonable inference is that animals do have cognitive abilities resembling those of humans, although this does not constitute strong evidence that they have a soul (Harrison, 1992; Hatfield, 2008), as spirituality and cognitive ability are not synonymous.

There have been many empirical studies regarding the cognitive abilities of animals, particularly regarding the capacity of gorillas and other primates to learn American Sign Language (Fouts, 1974; Howell, 2003; Patterson, 1978; Patterson & Lindin, 1981; Terrace, 1985) and then to spontaneously sign to one another (Gardner & Gardner, 1978). There is an ability to understand hundreds of signs of American Sign Language by these primates, as well as to put together coherent phrases with independent coherent meaning. Gorillas have also demonstrated the ability to transfer this to their offspring without specific human training (Fouts, Hirsch, & Fouts, 1982). Primates were not only able to carry out simple commands but they could recall the commands, travel to another room, locate an object, and carry out the command given to them (Rumbaugh-Savage & Boysen, 1978; Rumbaugh-Savage, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, & Rupert, 1986; Rumbaugh-Savage, Pate, Lawson, Smith, & Rosenbaum, 1983). Seidenberg and Petitto's (1987) apes, Kanzi and Mulika, were taught symbol usage and found that they shared many characteristics of word usage with those seen in young children. Rather than concentrating on food, which previous research had indicated that primates favored, Kanzi and Mulika's favorite topic was social play, the same as young children (Rumbaugh & Rumbaugh-Savage, 1996; Rumbaugh-Savage, 1987).

The research of the Gestalt psychologist Wolfgang Kohler (1967, 2001) highlighted that in problem-solving, chimpanzees could grasp whole situations and were able to understand the relationships among the various stimuli. Schultz and Schultz (2007) were able to describe how the cognitive revolution in psychology restored consciousness not only to humans but to animals as well. Beginning in the 1970's, animal psychologists attempted to demonstrate how animals encode, transfer, compute, and manipulate symbolic representations of the real world's spatial, temporal, and visual features for the purpose of adaptively organizing their behaviors (Cook, 1993). in other words, the computer-like system of information processing that occurs in humans is now being studied in animals.

Method

Sample

College students were administered the scale from both a conservative evangelical university and a secular university. The 99 evangelical university students in the South, 51 males and 48 females, ranged in age from 17 to 26 years with a mean age of 24.41 and a standard deviation of 6.06. The 96 Southern secular university students, 47 males and 49 females, ranged in age from 19 to 42 years with a mean of 21.26 years and a standard deviation of 4.48. The 195 combined group of students, 98 males and 97 females, ranged in age from 17 to 42 years, with a mean of 23.16 and a standard deviation of 5.58.

Instruments

Animal-Human Continuity Scale (AHCS).

The Animal-Human Continuity Scale (AHCS) is a 12-item scale that was constructed to measure the extent to which the respondent views humans and animals in a dichotomous fashion versus as a continuum (Templer et al., 2006). items were generated on a rational basis. item selection was based on ratings of content validity followed by item total score correlations with graduate students, faculty, and university staff participants. The scales contain such items as "humans can think but animals cannot," "people evolved from lower animals," and "people have a spiritual nature but animals do not." The Animal-Human Continuity Scale has reasonably good psychometric properties (Templer et al., 2006). it has good internal consistency as gauged by Chronbach's Alpha (.69). its construct validity is demonstrated by significant correlations with religion, gender, and theoretical orientation variables. it has good criterion-oriented validity insofar as members of a fundamental religion scored in the dichotomous direction in comparison to a unitarian group (Templer, et al., 2006). More traditional religious participants tended to respond in the dichotomous direction. The instrument yielded a meaningful factor analysis. The scale yielded three factors, Rational Capacity, Superiority versus Equality, and Evolutionary Continuum. The Animal-Human Continuity Scale takes about ten minutes to complete. The Animal-Human Continuity Scale has a Likert-like format with strongly disagree = 1, moderately disagree = 2, slightly disagree = 3, unsure = 4, slightly agree = 5, moderately agree = 6, and strongly agree = 7. The dichotomous items (1, 2, 3, 7, 9, 10, 11, and 12) are reverse scored.

Life Attitude Profile--Revised (LAP-R). The Life Attitude Profile--Revised (LAP-R) is a 48-item questionnaire developed from the original 156-item Life Attitude Profile (Reker & Peacock, 1981). The manual indicates that it can be used with participants ranging from adolescence to older adulthood and requires only a fifth grade reading level. Each item is scored on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from seven representing strongly agree to one representing strongly disagree. The profile produces six scores titled Purpose (PU), Coherence (CO), Choice/Responsibility (CR), Death Acceptance (DA), Existential Vacuum (EV), and Goal Seeking (GS) and two composite scores entitled Personal Meaning Index (PMI) and Existential Transcendence (ET). Each of the six scales has eight items, producing the total 48 items. A high score on the scale indicates a high degree of that attitude. The LAP-R was constructed to empirically measure Frankl's concept of "will to meaning," as developed in his logotherapy. The first scale of the LAP-R, Purpose, refers to having life goals, a sense of direction, and a notion of worthiness (Reker, 1992). The second scale, Coherence, refers to having an ordered, logical, and consistent understanding of self, others, and life in general.

The third scale, Choice/Responsibility, refers to one's sense of freedom to make choices and decisions and have a sense of control over the direction that an individual's life takes. The fourth scale, Death Acceptance, refers to an absence of fear and anxiety about death and the acceptance of death as a natural part of life. The fifth scale, Existential Vacuum, refers to a lack of meaning, goals, and directions, and results in feelings of boredom, apathy, and indifference. The sixth scale, Goal Seeking, refers to the desire to avoid the routine of life by seeking out new experiences and challenges. The first composite score, the Personal Meaning index, is derived by adding the Purpose and Coherence scales. The second composite score, Existential Transcendence, is derived by summing the Purpose, Coherence, Choice/Responsibility, and Death Acceptance Scales and then subtracting the Existential Vacuum and Goal Seeking scales.

Measures of Religiosity

Table 1 consists of the religious/spiritual inventory administered to all participants. Both question 1 and question 2 yield two scores, one pertaining to strength of belief and the other to degree of certainty.

For questions 1, (strength of belief), 5 is scored for "God definitely exists," 4 for "God probably exists," 3 for "I do not know whether God exists or not," 2 for "God probably does not exist," and 1 for "God definitely does not exist."

For question 1 (degree of certainty), 3 is scored for either "God definitely exists or God definitely does not exist," 2 for either "God probably exists or God probably does not exist," and 1 for "I don't know whether God exists or not."

For question 2 (strength of belief), 5 is scored for "Life after death definitely exists," 4 for "Life after death probably exists," 3 for "I don't know whether life after death exists or not," 2 for "Life after death probably does not exist," and 1 for "Life after death definitely does not exist."

For question 2 (degree of certainty), 3 is scored for either "Life after death definitely exists" or "Life after death definitely does not exist," 2 is scored for either "Life after death probably exists" or "Life after death probably does not exist," and 1 for "I don't know whether life after death exists or not."

Procedure

Participants were given a stapled packet that included a consent form, a short demographic questionnaire, the brief religious inventory, the Life Attitude Profile--Revised, and the Animal-Human Continuity Scale. Participants were instructed to read and sign the consent form and then continue on with the questionnaires if they chose to do so. When they had completed the questionnaires, the participants were instructed to return the entire packet with the signed consent form to the senior author. The entire procedure took less than 75 minutes, with most of the participants finishing within an hour.

Results

Table 2 contains the means and standard deviations for all continuous variables for the evangelical university students, the secular university students, and the combined groups. The secular university score (M = 33.5) is significantly higher (i.e., in the continuity direction) than the evangelical university score (M =14.41), t = 11.21, p < .001.

Although previous AHCS factor analyses have been reported, it was decided to do a factor analysis with the present data because factor structure differs in different populations. The present factor analysis combined the two groups of participants with the evangelical university students appearing not to be a representative group of American university students. For the combined group, two factors were extracted. Table 3 contains the factor loadings for the combined group. Factor 1, labeled "animal spirituality," had an eigenvalue of 4.66 and accounted for 38.9% of the variance. Factor 2, labeled "animal-human similarity," had an eigenvalue of 1.30 and accounted for 16.84% of the variance.

Table 4 presents the correlations of the Animal-Human Continuity Scale with the religious and existential variables with the combined group of college participants. it should be borne in mind that a higher score on this instrument indicates more of a continuous orientation. it should also be noted that in regard to the religious variables, a positive correlation indicates greater belief or greater certainty. Table 4 indicates that a dichotomous orientation toward animals and humans is associated with greater church attendance, belief and certainty regarding God, belief and certainty regarding a life after death, purpose, choice/responsibilities, and existential transcendence.

Discussion

The lower scores of the evangelical university students on the Animal-Human Continuity Scale in comparison to those of the secular university students are consistent with the traditional Christian religious teaching that humans and animals are qualitatively different. Furthermore, the evangelical university itself stands for the literal interpretation of the Bible in which humans were created by God rather than merely the product of evolution.

The negative correlation of the Animal-Human Continuity Scale with that of the religious variables for the evangelical and secular students and the combined group is congruent with the group differences. That is, more traditionally religious persons perceive less continuity between humans and animals. The fact that the correlations tend to be higher for the combined group can be attributed to greater variability yielding higher correlations.

The religious variable that yielded the highest and greatest number of significant negative correlations with the Animal-Human Continuity Scale was frequency of church attendance. it could be argued by some that greater continuity represents extrinsic rather than intrinsic (Allport, 1950) religiosity. A more parsimonious explanation, however, is that frequency of attendance is more behavioral objective and quantitative than belief. It is difficult to quantify subjective experiences such as strength of belief and certainty of belief.

Nevertheless, most of the significant correlations between the Animal-Human Continuity Scale and the Life Attitude Profile--Revised do show that persons with higher existential attitudes such as Purpose, Choice Meaning, and Transcendence tend to believe that there are qualitative differences between humans and animals. This seems reasonable. if a person views human nature as having more spiritual than biological determination, he or she views humans as having attributes distinct from animal attributes.

It is apparent that Choice/Responsibility of the Life Attitude Profile-Revised (Reker, 1992) yields the highest and most significant (negative) correlations across the evangelical and secular and combined groups. This is understandable in view of Choice/Responsibility assessing the degree that the respondent feels that he or she has responsibility and control of one's life. Low scores on the Animal-Human Continuity Scale imply a free will in contrast to a continuity of perspective in which humans are animals whose behavior is entirely determined by the same principles that govern animal behavior.

The composite of the findings converges to a perspective in which stronger traditional Christian beliefs are associated with perceiving greater qualitative differentiation between humans and animals. Evangelical university students scored more in the dichotomous direction on the Animal-Human Continuity Scale than the secular university students. With the evangelical and secular and combined groups, those students who were more religious endorsed a dichotomous distinction between humans and animals. Furthermore, more existential attitude frames of reference were associated with a dichotomous as opposed to a continuous perceived relationship between humans and animals.

It is noteworthy that existential scores were associated with greater church attendance and traditional Christian belief of qualitative difference between humans and animals. These findings were not unquestionably predictable because existential scholars and clinicians are often atheistic or agnostic and often maintain that existentialism rests on the assumption that there is nothing after death. May (1983) pointed out that both Nietzsche and Kierkegaard had a contemptuous attitude toward Christianity. Sartre (1967) said that his position is one of atheistic existentialism. The Life Attitude Profile--Revised, the measure of existential values used in the present research, was based on the thinking of Viktor Frankl, a Jewish psychiatrist incarcerated in a Nazi concentration camp. Frankl speaks frequently of spirituality but does not write about the beliefs or practices of Judaism or Christianity. Nevertheless, in the present study, greater Christian religiosity seems to be related to values of persons who apparently have more of a theistic than humanistic orientation.

This present research incorporated two southern universities. one university was a conservative evangelical university and the other a southern secular university. The locations of these universities are in the Bible Belt of the south. Future research with different regions may gleam different findings. Research with Jewish and with Muslin participants is recommended since these two monotheistic religions also believe in the Bible. It would be expected that more religious participants would have a more dichotomous orientation. Such a prediction would not necessarily be made with Eastern or Native American religions since both to a greater extent incorporate animals in the spiritual realm. in the Hindu religion one may be human in one life and an animal in another.

References

Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his religion. New York, NY: MacMillan.

Bakhos, C. (2009). Jewish, Christian, and Muslim attitudes toward animals. Comparative Islamic Studies, 155, 1203-1207. doi: 10.1558/cis.v5i2.177

Clough, D. (2009). The anxiety of the human animal: Martin Luther on non-human animals and human animality." In Deane-Drummond, C., and Clough, D., Eds.), Creaturely theology: On God, humans and other animals (pp. 41-60). London: SCM Press.

Cook, R. G. (1993). The experimental analysis of cognition in animals. Psychological Science, 4, 174-178. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00483x

Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of the species. London, England: John Murray.

Descartes, R. (1993). Animals are machines. in S. J. Armstrong & R. G. Botzler (Eds.), Environmental ethics: Divergence and convergence (pp. 281-285). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Fouts, R. S. (1974). Language: origins, definitions, and chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 475-482. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(74)90007-4

Fouts, R. S., Hirsch, A. D., & Fouts, D. H. (1982). Cultural transmission of a human language in a chimpanzee mother-infant relationship. in H. E. Fitzgerald, J. A. Mullins, & P. Gage (Eds.), Child nurturance (Vol. 3, pp. 159-193). New York, NY: Plenum.

Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1978). Comparative psychology and language acquisition. Annuals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 309, 37-76. doi: 10.1111/j.1749.66-32.1978.tb29441.x

Harrison, P. (1992). Descartes on animals. The Philosophical Quarterly, 42, 219-227. doi: 10.2307/2220217

Hatfield, G. (2008). Psychology and philosophy: Historical perspectives. in S. Heinamaa & Reuter, M. (Eds.), Psychology and philosophy: Inquiries into the soul from late scholasticism to contemporary thought (Vol. 8, pp. 1-25). New York, NY: Springer.

Hatfield, P. (1992). Psychology and philosophy: Historical perspectives. in S. Heinamaa & Reuter, M. (Eds.), Psychology and philosophy: Inquiries into the soul from late scholasticism to contemporary thought (Vol. 8, pp. 1-25). New York, NY: Springer.

Howell, N. (2003). The importance of being chimpanzee. Theology and Science, 1 , 179-191. doi:10.1080/1474670032000124.86

Kohler, W. (1967). Gestalt Psychology. Psychological Research, 31, 18-30. doi: 10.1007/BF00422382

Kohler, W. (2001). Simple structural functions in the chimpanzee and in the chicken. in W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology (pp. 217-227). New York, NY: Routledge.

May, R. (1983). The discovery of being: Writings in existential psychology. New York, NY: Norton.

McGoldrick, T. A. (2012). The spirituality of human consciousness: A Catholic evaluation of some current neuro-scientific interpretations. Science and Engineering Ethnics, 18, 483-501.

Newman, L. E. (1987). The quality of mercy: On the duty to forgive in the Judaic tradition. Journal of Religious Ethics, 15, 155-172

Patterson, F. (1978). The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another pongid, Brain and Language, 71-97. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(78)90008-1

Patterson, F., & Linden, E. (1981). The education of Koko. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Preece, R., & Fraser, D. (2000). The status of animals in biblical and Christian thought: A study in colliding values. Journal of Human-Animal Studies, 8, 245-263.

Reker, G. T. (1992). Life Attitude Profile-Revised: Procedures manual (Research Ed.). Petersborough, ontario, Canada: Student Psychologists Press.

Reker, G. T., & Peacock, E. J. (1981). The life attitude profile (LAP): A multidimentional instrument for assessing attitudes toward life. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 13, 264-273.

Ridgeway, S. H. (2008). History of veterinary medicine and marine animals. A personal perspective. Historic Perspective Series, 34, 471-513.

Rumbaugh, D. M., & Rumbaugh-Savage, E. S. (1996). In communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Rumbaugh-Savage, E. S. (1987). Communication, symbolic communication, and language: Reply to Seidenberg and Pettito. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 116, 288-292. doi: 10.1037/00963445.116.3.288

Rumbaugh-Savage, E. S., & Boysen, S. T. (1978). Cooperation in primates: Critical analysis of behavioural criteria. Behavioural Processes, 35, 101-111.

Rumbaugh-Savage, E. S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rupert, E. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use of pygmy chimpanzees (Panpaniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology, 115, 211-235. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.115.3.211

Rumbaugh-Savage, E. S., Pate, J. K., Lawson, J., Smith, S. T., & Rosenbaum, S. (1983). Can a chimpanzee make a statement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 457-491. doi: 10.1037/00963445.112.4.457

Sartre, Jean-Paul. (1967). Existentialism and human emotions. New York, NY: Philosophical Library.

Schultz, D. P., & Schultz, S. E. (2007). History of modern psychology (7th ed.). New York, NY: Harcourt.

Seidenberg, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (1987). Communication, symbolic communication and language: Comment on Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevick, Hopkins, and Rupert. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 116, 279-287. doi: 10.1037/00963445.116.3.279

Templer, D. I., Connelly, H., Bassman, L., & Hart, J. (2006). Construction and validation of an animal-human continuity scale. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 34, 769-776. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2006.34.7.769

Terrace, H. S. (1985). in the beginning was the "name." American Psychologist, 40, 1011-1028. doi: 10.1037/0003-006x.40.9.1011

Thomas, R. K. (1996). investigating cognitive abilities in animals: unrealized potential. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(3-4), 156-166. doi: 10.1016/0926 6410(96)00003-1

Frank L. King

John C. Thomas

Gary R. Habermas

Liberty University

Donald I. Templer

Alliant International University

Kimberly Tangen

Fresno, California

Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Donald Templer, Ph.D., 257 W. Los Altos, Fresno, CA 93704; [email protected]

Frank L. King has earned two doctorate degrees, one a Doctor of Ministry degree and the other a Doctorate in Counseling. He also has an earned MBA degree and a graduate degree in education. Dr. King has retired from full-time teaching as an Associate Professor of Religion at Liberty University and is currently an adjunct professor at Liberty University, where he has been associated for the past 15 years.

John C. Thomas has two earned doctorates and is an Associate Professor in the Center for Counseling and Family Studies at Liberty University.

Gary R. Haberman (Ph.D., Michigan State University) is Distinguished Research Professor and Chair of the Philosophy Department at Liberty University. He has published 36 books and more than 60 chapters or articles in other books. He has been a visiting or adjunct professor, teaching dozens of courses at 15 graduate schools and seminaries in the U.S. and abroad.

Donald I. Templer received his Ph.D. from the University of Kentucky. He has well over 200 publications and well over 2,000 citations. His Death Anxiety Scale has been translated into 19 languages. He is retired Professor of Psychology at Alliant International University, Fresno.

Kimberly Tangen received her bachelor's degree from the University of Minnesota and a Ph.D. from Alliant International University. She has published on professional issues and attitudes toward animals. Her clinical practice involves professional psychology.
Table 1
Religious/Spiritual Inventory Administered to All Participants

INSTRUCTIONS:  Circle the response that best expresses your
current religious/spiritual beliefs and practices

1   How certain are your beliefs about God?

    a   God definitely exists
    b   God probably exists
    c   I do not know whether God exists or not
    d   God probably does not exist
    e   God definitely does not exist

2   How certain are your beliefs about life after death?

    a   Life after death definitely exists
    b   Life after death probably exists
    c   I do not know whether life after death exists or not
    d   Life after death probably does not exist
    e   Life after death definitely does not exist

3   How frequently do you currently attend an organized
    religious function?

    a   Rarely or never
    b   Several times a year
    c   At least once a month
    d   At least once a week

Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for All Continuous Variables

                                     Evangelical      Secular
                                     University      University

Variable                             X      SD       X      SD

Age                                24.41   1.06    21.23   4.48
  Strength of belief in God        3.87     .44    3.42     .99
  Certainty about belief in        2.99     .10    2.80     .49
  God
  Strength of belief in life       3.87     .44    3.42     .99
  after death
  Certainty about belief in life   2.87     .44    2.56     .74
  after death
  Frequency of service atten-      3.72     .65    2.18    1.21
  dance
Animal-Human Continuity            33.50   10.03   52.58   13.54
Scale
Life Attitude Profile-Revised
  Purpose                          22.93   7.10    17.81    6.01
  Coherence                        19.44   6.66    20.17    6.47
  Choice/Responsibility            28.71   8.90    22.84    9.98
  Death Acceptance                 25.40   9.89    25.06    9.53
  Existential Vacuum               35.82   8.17    38.34    8.87
  Goal Seeking                     20.00   6.98    20.27    6.02
  Personal Meaning                 42.38   12.23   37.98   11.34
  Existential Transcendence        57.06   16.19   39.39   15.97

                                       Combined
                                     Universities

Variable                             X       SD

Age                                23.16    5.38
  Strength of belief in God        3.80      .49
  Certainty about belief in        2.89      .37
  God
  Strength of belief in life       3.65      .79
  after death
  Certainty about belief in life   2.71      .62
  after death
  Frequency of service atten-      2.96     1.24
  dance
Animal-Human Continuity            42.94   15.26
Scale
Life Attitude Profile-Revised
  Purpose                          20.41    7.06
  Coherence                        19.80    6.56
  Choice/Responsibility            16.79    6.98
  Death Acceptance                 25.23    9.69
  Existential Vacuum               37.06    8.59
  Goal Seeking                     20.13    6.51
  Personal Meaning                 40.22   11.97
  Existential Transcendence        48.36   18.32

Table 3
Factor Loadings for Animal-Human Continuity Scale With Combined
University Students

                                                      Factor

Item                                                 1      2

1.  Humans have a soul but animals do not. *        .79    .37

10. The needs of people should always come before   .78    .15
    the needs of animals. *

3.  People have a life after death but animals do   .74    .43
    not. *

7.  People are superior to animals. *               .65    .25

11. It's okay to use animals to carry out tasks     .65    -.15
    for humans. *

9.  Humans have a spiritual nature but animals do   .63    .41
    not. *

5.  Animals are afraid of death.                    -.32   .64

6.  People evolved from lower animals.              .21    .62

8.  Animals can fall in love.                       .30    .60

4.  People are animals.                             .33    .52

12. It's crazy to think of an animal as a member    .20    .50
    of your family. *

2.  Humans can think but animals cannot. *          .43    .33

* Dichotomous items that are reverse-scored.

Table 4
Correlations of Animal-Human Continuity Variables With Religious
and Life Attitude Variables With All Participants

                                      Animal-Human Continuity Scale

Independent Variable               Total Score   Factor 1    Factor 2

Religious variable
  Strength of belief in God (a)     -.22 ***       -.04      -.30 ***
  Certainty of belief in God (b)    -.22 ***        .04      -.30 ***
  Strength of belief in life         -.18 **       -.09      -.19 **
  after death (a)
  Certainty of belief in life       -.23 ***        .00      -.26 ***
  after death (b)
  Frequency of service atten-       -.61 ***       -.10      -.48 ***
  dance
Life Attitude Profile--Revised
  Purpose                           -.28 ***      -.19 **    -.22 **
  Coherence                            .07          .07        .00
  Choice/Responsibility             -.54 ***     -.36 ***    -.43 ***
  Death Acceptance                     .11         .14 *       -.00
  Existential Vacuum                   .05          .02        .03
  Goal Seeking                         .02          .03        -.04
  Personal Meaning Index              -.13         -.07        .13
  Existential Transcendence         -.35 ***     -.28 ***    -.30 ***

(a) High score = greater belief

(b) High score = greater certainty

* p < .05. ** p = < .01. *** p < .001
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有