Religiousness and congruence between sexual values and behavior.
Hook, Joshua N. ; Farrell, Jennifer E. ; Ramos, Marciana J. 等
Religious individuals often adopt strict morals and values about
sexual behavior; however, their actual behavior may or may not line up
to these attitudes. This low sexual congruence may be especially common
among younger individuals. For example, a study of adolescents comparing
those who made abstinence pledges and those who did not found no
differences in occurrence of premarital sex and sexually transmitted
diseases between these two groups five years after their pledges
(Rosenbaum, 2009). Furthermore, 82% of pledgers denied having made the
pledge five years later (Rosenbaum, 2009). Similarly, a study of
"hooking up" in college students found that although
religiousness was a moderate predictor of negative attitudes toward
"hooking up" in both males and females, religiousness was a
weak predictor of actual "hooking up" behavior for women, and
it was not a significant predictor for men (Owen, Rhoades, Stanley,
& Fincham, 2010).
This research is consistent with previous research on cognitive
dissonance (Festinger, 1957), in which incongruence between attitudes
and behavior creates a negative, motivational state, which impels
individuals to reduce the inconsistency and ease the tension. Although
research on cognitive dissonance is ample, and sexual incongruence may
be common, especially for young people, there has been little empirical
research that has explored (a) what types of individuals may be more
likely to struggle with sexual congruence, as well as (b) possible
consequences that may arise from sexual incongruence. The current
investigation explores the intersection between religion and sexual
congruence.
Religious individuals may be particularly susceptible to the
cognitive dissonance evoked by sexual incongruence because many
conservative religious groups teach that the only acceptable sexual
expression occurs within the context of a heterosexual marriage, with
the most conservative groups even proscribing sexual expression without
a partner (e.g., masturbation; Beck, 2011). However, social pressures
around sexuality have changed over the past 100 years, including (a) an
increasingly sexualized popular culture with easy access to sexualized
media and (b) an increased length of singleness due to increasing spans
of education and time needed to establish a professional identity (Kwee
& Hoover, 2008). Therefore, such religious teachings have become
increasingly difficult to practice, causing increased tension between
sexual attitudes and behavior among religious youth.
There may be consequences for individuals who struggle to maintain
sexual congruence. For example, not living up to one's standards
can lead to guilt, shame, difficulty forgiving the self from past
mistakes, and struggles with one's relationship with God
(Worthington, 2013). Sexual incongruence may also lead individuals to
pathologize developmental^ normative behavior (e.g., sexual
exploration). For example, Kwee, Dominguez, and Ferrell (2007) noted
that religious male clients would often view unwanted sexual thoughts,
feelings, and behaviors as a sexual addiction, irrespective of whether
they met criteria for an addiction. Similarly, Grubbs, Exline,
Pargament, Hook, and Carlisle (2015) found that religiousness was a
positive predictor of perceived addiction to pornography, even when
actual pornography use was held constant, and the relationship between
religiousness and perceived addiction to pornography was mediated by
moral disapproval of pornography use. Their findings indicate that
religious individuals who disapprove of a certain sexual behavior (e.g.,
viewing pornography) but still engage in that behavior often experience
profound distress and negative interpretations of that behavior (Grubbs
et al., 2015).
Thus, although there has been theory and some indirect research
showing that sexual incongruence may be more common in religious
individuals and may be related to negative consequences, no research has
measured the construct of sexual congruence and tested these hypotheses
directly. In the present studies, we investigated the role of religion
and sexual congruence. We had two main hypotheses. First, we
hypothesized that more highly religious individuals would report lower
levels of sexual congruence, even when controlling for objective levels
of sexual behavior (similar to Grubbs et al., 2015). Second, we
hypothesized that sexual incongruence would be related to negative
intrapersonal consequences, such as difficulties forgiving the self and
higher levels of spiritual struggle following engaging in a sexual
behavior that was against one's values. Also, we hypothesized that
individuals with lower levels of sexual congruence would be more likely
to view themselves as having high levels of problematic sexual behavior,
even when controlling for objective levels of sexual behavior.
Study 1
In Study 1, we began to explore the relationship between
religiousness and sexual congruence. We hypothesized that more religious
participants would show lower levels of sexual congruence, even when
controlling for objective sexual behavior.
Method
Participants and procedure. Participants were 491 college students
who reported being involved in a romantic relationship. The mean age was
21.1 years (SD = 4.4). Participants were 25.3% male, 73.7% female, and
1.0% other. Participants were 56.2% White, 14.7% Black, 7.5% Asian,
16.5% Latino, 1.2% Native American, and 3.9% Multiracial. Participants
were 89.4% Heterosexual, 3.9% Gay/Lesbian, 5.3% Bisexual and 1.4% Other
(e.g., pansexual). Participants were 69.0% Christian, 2.2% Muslim, 1.6%
Buddhist, 1.0% Hindu, 1.4% Jewish, 0.2% Mormon, 4.5% Atheist, 9.2%
Agnostic, and 10.8% None.
Participants were recruited from undergraduate courses at a large
university in the Southwestern United States. Participants completed the
study online. First, participants read an informed consent sheet that
described the study and their rights as participants. Specifically,
participants read that they would be asked to answer questions regarding
their romantic relationship and sexual experiences. Participants also
read about procedures to ensure confidentiality. After giving consent,
participants completed a series of questionnaires. After completing the
questionnaires, participants were debriefed and given the contact
information of the researcher should they have any questions or concerns
about the study. Participants received a small amount of course credit
or extra credit in exchange for completing the study.
Measures
Sexual Values Scale (SVS). The SVS was created for the present
study, and consisted of seven items that assessed the extent to which
participants experienced sexual congruence. First, participants were
directed to describe their morals and values regarding the sexual part
of their life. Second, participants completed the seven items of the
SVS. For each item, participants were asked to indicate the extent to
which each aspect of their sexual life aligned with their morals and
values. Participants rated each item on a 5-point rating scale from 1 =
not at all aligned to 5 = completely aligned. A principal components
analysis revealed that a single factor best accounted for the variance
in items. Factor loadings ranged from .60 to .94. Means, standard
deviations, and factor loadings for items are in Table 1. For the
current sample, the Cronbach's alpha coefficient was .92
Religious Commitment Inventory--10 (RCI-10). Religious commitment
was measured with the RCI-10 (Worthington et al., 2003). The RCI-10
consists of 10 items that assess one's commitment to one's
religion (e.g., "Religion is especially important to me because it
answers many questions about the meaning of life"). Participants
rate each item on a 5-point rating scale from 1 = not at all true of me
to 5 = totally true of me. Scores on the RCI-10 have shown evidence for
internal consistency and construct validity (Worthington et al., 2003).
For the current sample, the Cronbach's alpha coefficient was .97.
Objective sexual behavior. We asked several questions to assess
actual levels of sexual behavior. First, to assess sexual behavior
without a partner, participants answered the following question "On
average, how many times a month do you masturbate?" on a 5-point
rating scale from 1 = never to 5 = more than 12. Second, to assess
sexual behavior with a partner, participants answered how many times per
month they engaged in the following behaviors: oral sex, anal sex,
penile-vaginal sex, and manual stimulation of one another's
genitals on a 5-point rating scale from 1 = never to 5 = more than 12.
We then calculated the mean score for sexual behavior with a partner
(Cronbach's alpha for sexual behavior with a partner was .76).
Finally, to assess total sexual behavior, we calculated the mean score
of (a) sexual behavior without a partner and (b) sexual behavior with a
partner. There was a small, positive correlation between these two items
(r = .16, p < .001).
Results and Discussion
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations between study
variables are in Table 2. Our main hypothesis was that highly religious
individuals would report lower levels of sexual congruence, even when
controlling for objective sexual behavior. Although individuals who were
more religious reported lower levels of objective sexual behavior (r =
-.28, p < .001), they also reported lower levels of sexual congruence
(r = .19, p < .001). We calculated the partial correlation between
religious commitment and sexual congruence, controlling for total
objective sexual behavior. Controlling for objective sexual behavior,
there was a small, negative correlation between religious commitment and
sexual congruence, r = -.12, p = .007, indicating that participants who
were more religious reported less sexual congruence, even when actual
sexual behavior was held constant.
As an exploratory analysis, we examined the extent to which our
findings were similar for men and women. In regard to mean differences
on our study variables, an independent samples t-test revealed that men
reported higher levels of total objective sexual behavior and sexual
behavior without a partner (both p's < .001). There were no
significant gender differences on the other study variables. In regard
to the relationships between variables, in general the pattern of
relationships was similar for men and women. Two significant differences
emerged, using a t-test for the difference between two independent
correlations. First, the positive correlation between sexual congruence
and total objective sexual behavior was larger for women (r = .34) than
men (r = .14, p = .042). Second, the negative correlation between
religious commitment and sexual behavior with a partner was larger for
women (r = -.32) than men (r = -.11, p = .036). (See Table 3 for the
intercorrelations separated by gender.)
In this first study, we assessed the extent to which sexual
congruence was related to religious commitment, and found that more
religious individuals reported less sexual congruence, even when actual
sexual behavior was held constant. This relationship was small, however,
so readers should be cautious when interpreting this finding. In the
next study, we shifted our focus to explore possible consequences of
sexual incongruence.
Study 2
In Study 1, we investigated the relationship between religiousness
and sexual congruence. In Study 2, we shifted our focus to explore
possible consequences of sexual incongruence. We focused on three
primary areas of interest: self-forgiveness, spiritual struggle, and
perceptions of problematic sexual behavior. As in Study 1, we controlled
for objective level of sexual behavior. Thus, our primary hypothesis was
that sexual incongruence would predict (a) less self-forgiveness and
more spiritual struggle following behavior that went against one's
sexual values, and (b) higher levels of perceived problematic sexual
behavior, even when the actual level of sexual behavior was held
constant.
Method
Participants and procedure. Participants were 191 college students.
The mean age was 24.4 years (SD = 5.3). Participants were 34.4% male and
65.6% female. Participants were 34.2% White, 41.1% Black, 12.6% Asian,
7.4% Latino, 3.7% Multiracial, and 1.1% Other. Participants were 74.6%
Christian, 7.4% Muslim, 1.6% Buddhist, 2.1% Hindu, 2.6% Atheist, 8.5%
Agnostic, 2.6% None, and 0.5% Other.
Participants were recruited from undergraduate courses at a large
university in the Southeastern United States. Participants completed the
study online. First, as in the prior study, participants read an
informed consent sheet that described the study and their rights as
participants. Participants were told they would be asked to answer
questions regarding a time in which they had violated their sexual
values. Participants were told about procedures to ensure
confidentiality. After giving consent, participants described an
instance in the past month in which they had done something counter to
their sexual values, and completed a series of questionnaires. After
completing the questionnaires, participants were debriefed and given the
contact information of the researcher should they have any questions or
concerns about the study. Participants received a small amount of course
credit or extra credit in exchange for completing the study.
Measures.
Sexual Values Scale (SVS). Participants completed the SVS, as
described in Study 1. For the current sample, the Cronbach's alpha
coefficient was .83.
Self-Forgiveness Scale (SFS). Self-forgiveness was measured with
the feelings subscale of the Self-Forgiveness Scale (SFS; Wohl, DeShea,
& Wahkinney, 2008). This subscale consists of 8 items that assess
emotions associated with self-forgiveness regarding a specific offense
(e.g., "As I consider what I did that was wrong, I feel
compassionate toward myself"). Participants completed each item on
a 7-point rating scale from 1 = not at all to 7 = completely. Scores on
the SFS show evidence for internal consistency and construct validity
(Wohl et al., 2008). For the current sample, the Cronbach's alpha
coefficient was .81.
Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI-19). Problematic sexual
behavior was measured with the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI-19;
Reid, Garos, & Carpenter, 2011). The HBI-19 consists of 19 items
that assess symptoms associated with hypersexual behavior (e.g., "I
engage in sexual activities I know I will later regret").
Participants completed each item on a 5-point rating scale from 1 =
never to 5 = always. Scores on the HBI show evidence for internal
consistency and construct validity (Reid et al., 2011; Reid et al.,
2012). For the current sample, the Cronbach's alpha coefficient was
.92.
Spiritual Struggle Scale (SSS). Spiritual struggle was measured
with nine items created for the present study. Items reflected damage or
hurt in one's relationship with God due to the behavior the
participant did that was counter to his or her values (e.g., "I
feel distant from God", "What I did hurt my sense of closeness
with God"). Participants completed each item on a 5-point rating
scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. For the current
sample, the Cronbach's alpha coefficient was .91.
Objective sexual behavior. We asked two questions to assess actual
levels of sexual behavior. First, to assess sexual behavior without a
partner, participants answered the following question "In the past
month, how often have you engaged in sexual behavior without a partner
(e.g., viewing pornography or masturbation)?" on a 5-point rating
scale from 1 = never to 5 = more than 12. Second, to assess sexual
behavior with a partner, participants answered the following question:
"In the past month, how often have you engaged in sexual behavior
with a partner?" on a 5-point rating scale from 1 = never to 5 =
more than 12. To assess total sexual behavior, we calculated the mean
score of (a) sexual behavior without a partner and (b) sexual behavior
with a partner. There was a small, positive (but non-significant)
correlation between these two items (r = .10, p = .254).
Results and Discussion
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations between study
variables are in Table 4. Our primary hypothesis was that sexual
congruence would be (a) positively related to self-forgiveness, (b)
negatively related to spiritual struggle, and (c) negatively related to
perceptions of problematic sexual behavior, even when actual sexual
behavior was controlled for. Using a similar strategy as Study 1, we
calculated partial correlations between the SVS and self-forgiveness,
spiritual struggle, and perceived problematic sexual behavior,
controlling for total objective sexual behavior. Consistent with our
hypotheses, controlling for objective sexual behavior, sexual congruence
was positively related to self-forgiveness (r = .18, p = .025),
negatively related to spiritual struggle (r = -.34, p < .001), and
negatively related to perceptions of problematic sexual behavior (r =
-.22, p = .005). These findings indicate that participants who
experienced sexual incongruence (a) were less likely to forgive
themselves for doing something that was discrepant from their sexual
values, (b) were more likely to experience struggles with God about
doing something that was discrepant from their sexual values, and (c)
were more likely to perceive themselves as struggling with problematic
sexual behavior, even when actual sexual behavior was held constant.
As an exploratory analysis, we examined the extent to which our
findings were similar for men and women. In regard to mean differences
on our study variables, an independent samples t-test revealed that men
reported higher levels of sexual behavior without a partner (p = .010).
There were no significant gender differences on the other study
variables. In regard to the relationships between variables, the
relationships between congruence and the other study variables were
similar for men and women. Two significant differences emerged, using a
t-test for the difference between two independent correlations. First,
the relationship between perceptions of problematic sexual behavior and
total objective sexual behavior was greater for women (r = .28), than
men (r = -.06, p = .026). Second, the relationship between perceptions
of problematic sexual behavior and objective sexual behavior with a
partner was positive for women (r = .19) but negative for men (r = -.18,
p = .016). (See Table 5 for the intercorrelations separated by gender.)
In this second study, we focused on possible consequences of sexual
incongruence. Specifically we found that participants who experienced
sexual incongruence also tended to struggle with self-forgiveness,
spiritual struggle, and view their sexual behavior as problematic, even
when statistically controlling for objective sexual behavior.
General Discussion
The present set of studies investigated the role that religion
plays in experiencing sexual congruence (or not). Individuals who
reported being high in religious commitment also reported less sexual
congruence, although this relationship was modest in size. Religious
individuals also reported having less sex--both with and without a
partner--so it is likely that sexual incongruence is due to religious
individuals having a set of morals and values regarding their sexuality
that is more difficult to maintain. This finding is consistent with
prior theorizing by Kwee and Hoover (2008), which posited that religious
young adults face increasingly high levels of social pressures in
maintaining moral standards regarding sexuality and purity.
In addition to investigating the link between religiousness and
sexual congruence, we also explored the possible consequences of
struggling to maintain sexual congruence. Following a time in which
participants engaged in a sexual behavior that was against their values,
participants who had low levels of sexual congruence reported more
difficulties forgiving themselves and higher levels of spiritual
struggle. This finding is consistent with prior theory by Worthington
(2013), who noted that not living up to one's standards can lead to
difficulties with guilt, shame, unforgiveness, and one's
relationship with God.
We also found that individuals who had low levels of sexual
congruence were more likely to describe themselves as struggling with
problematic sexual behavior, even when levels of objective sexual
behavior (i.e., number of sexual engagements per month) were held
constant. This finding is consistent with prior theory and research that
has found that religious folks may be more likely to label unwanted
sexual behavior as addiction, even when the actual pattern of behavior
may not be characterized by addiction or even objectively problematic
(Grubbs et al., 2015; Kwee et al., 2007). For example, Grubbs and
colleagues (2015) noted that, although religious individuals were less
likely to report engaging in certain sexual behaviors (i.e., viewing
pornography), they were more likely to view their sexual behaviors as
pathological and problematic. In this past work, these pathological
interpretations were mediated by personal moral disapproval of
pornography use. In essence then, past research has shown that religious
individuals who (a) viewed pornography use as a moral failure but (b)
still used pornography were more likely to report negative
interpretations of their use (Grubbs et al., 2015). Such findings are
consistent with our present work.
In regard to gender differences, for the most part, findings were
consistent between men and women. The gender differences were as
follows: First, men reported higher levels of sexual behavior without a
partner than did women. This is consistent with previous research
showing higher rates of masturbation in men compared with women
(Peterson & Hyde, 2010). Second, the correlations between sexual
congruence, religious commitment, and objective sexual behavior were
stronger for women than men. This may indicate that sexual congruence
and religious commitment have a stronger effect on women's sexual
behavior than men's sexual behavior. Or it may suggest that
women's sense of sexual congruence is more aligned with their
actual behavior. Third, the correlation between perceptions of
problematic sexual behavior and objective sexual behavior were different
for men and women. For men, perceptions of problematic sexual behavior
were positively correlated with sexual behavior without a partner, but
negatively correlated with sexual behavior with a partner. For women,
perceptions of problematic sexual behavior were positively correlated
with both types of objective sexual behavior. This may indicate that for
male college students, problematic sexual behavior may be more likely to
show up in solitary activities such as masturbation and viewing
pornography, whereas for female college students, problematic sexual
behavior may include both solitary and partnered sexual activities.
Limitations and Areas for Future Research
There were several limitations of the present study. First, both
studies were correlational in nature; thus, causal conclusions should
not be made. Although the data are consistent with our theory and
hypotheses (i.e., religious commitment leading to lower sexual
congruence, lower sexual congruence leading to difficulties forgiving
the self), there are other theoretical models that could also be
applied. For example, perhaps individuals who consistently find it
difficult to achieve sexual congruence eventually choose to become less
religious over time. Future research could use longitudinal or
experimental designs to further explicate the relationships among these
variables and more closely approximate causal directionality. Second,
all the data were self-report. Future research should supplement
self-report measures with other-report and behavioral measures (Dorn,
Hook, Davis, Van Tongeren, & Worthington, 2014). Third, the primary
measure used in this study (the SVS) was created for the present study;
thus, there is limited evidence for the reliability and validity of this
measure. Finally, the sample from both these studies utilized
undergraduate students, and the majority of participants in both studies
were female. On one hand, this may be an especially important time to
assess sexual congruence, given that young people may be a group that
particularly struggles with sexual congruence. However, due to the
somewhat homogenous sample, caution should be used before generalizing
these findings to more broad samples of adults. Future research in this
area should explore the extent to which the relationships found in the
present study extend to other demographic groups.
Implications for Counseling
The findings from the present set of studies have several
implications for counseling. First, young adults may experience a
certain level of dissonance between their sexual values and sexual
thoughts/feelings/behaviors, and this lack of sexual congruence may be
exacerbated in religious college students. It should be noted that the
size of the correlation between religiousness and sexual incongruence
was small, so this struggle with sexual congruence may not apply to all
religious clients. When counseling religious young adults, it may be
important to assess a person's morals or values regarding the
sexual part of their life, as well as how these morals or values line up
with the individual's actual behavior. For individuals with a large
amount of sexual incongruence, it may be helpful to work with the client
to bring these two constructs into closer alignment. This may involve
helping a client to change his or her behavior, but it may also involve
working collaboratively with a client to assess and discuss the extent
to which the morals and values regarding the sexual part of their life
are realistic. Furthermore, although the present work only involved
collegiate populations, past work (e.g., Grubbs et al., 2015) has found
that discrepancies between sexual values and behaviors predicts
pathological interpretations of behaviors, even among adults. As such,
the present findings may also apply to more general counseling settings
that include adults.
If clients do struggle to maintain sexual congruence, they may find
it difficult to recover when they act in a way that is counter to their
values. Clients in this position may need help to forgive themselves in
a healthy way and/or repair their relationship with God. It should be
noted that these relationships were found even when actual objective
sexual behavior was held constant. Thus, the client's perception of
their sexual behavior (and its discrepancy from their values) may be
more important to the client than the counselor's perception of the
magnitude of the particular sexual behavior.
Finally, a client who is experiencing sexual incongruence may be
more likely to perceive his or her sexual behavior as problematic (or
even addictive), even if the actual level of sexual behavior may not
meet this threshold. One implication of this finding is that when
assessing for problems regarding one's sexual behavior, it is
important to assess both (a) the client's subjective feelings of
distress regarding his or her sexual thoughts/feelings/behavior and (b)
objective levels of sexual behavior (Hook, Hook, Davis, Worthington,
& Penberthy, 2010). Assessing both will allow the counselor to work
collaboratively with the client to determine whether the main work lies
in changing problematic sexual behavior or perhaps working toward
acceptance or moderating one's standards.
Conclusion
Strongly religious individuals may find it difficult to achieve
sexual congruence. This struggle for sexual congruence may have negative
consequences for one's health and well-being, especially when
people act in ways counter to their values. We hope this article will
facilitate more research in the area of sexual congruence, while
additionally offering clinical utility for counselors working with
clients who are struggling with their values and sexual behavior.
Joshua N. Hook
Jennifer E. Farrell
Marciana J. Ramos
University of North Texas
Don E. Davis
Sara Karaga
Georgia State University
Daryl R Van Tongeren
Hope College
Joshua Grubbs
Case Western Reserve University
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Joshua
N. Hook, Ph.D., Assistant Professor of Psychology, University of North
Texas, 1155 Union Circle, Denton, TX 76203;
[email protected]
References
Beck, R. (2011). Unclean: Meditations on purity, hospitality, and
mortality. Eugene, OR: Wipf and Stock Publishers.
Dorn, K., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Van Tongeren, D. R., &
Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2014). Behavioral measures of assessing
forgiveness. Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 75-80.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press.
Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., &
Carlisle, R. D. (2015). Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and
moral disapproval as predictors of perceived addiction to pornography.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 125-136.
Hook, J. N., Hook, J. P., Davis, D. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr.,
& Penberthy, J. K. (2010). Measuring sexual addiction and
compulsivity: A critical review of instruments. Journal of Sex and
Marital Therapy, 36, 227-260.
Kwee, A. W., Dominguez, A. W., & Ferrell, D. (2007). Sexual
addiction and Christian college men: Conceptual, assessment, and
treatment challenges. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 26, 3-13.
Kwee, A. W., & Hoover, D. C. (2008). Theologically-informed
education about masturbation: A male sexual health perspective. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 36, 258-269.
Owen, J., Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., & Fincham, F. D.
(2010). "Hooking up" among college students: Demographic and
psychosocial correlates. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 653-663.
Peterson, J. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). A meta-analytic review
of research on gender differences in sexuality, 1993-2007. Psychological
Bulletin, 136, 21-38.
Reid, R. C., Carpenter, B. N., Hook, J. N., Garos, S., Manning, J.
C., Gilliland, R., Cooper, E. B., McKittrick, H., Davtian, M., &
Fong, T. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 field trial for
hypersexual disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 2868-2877.
Reid, R. C., Garos, S., & Carpenter, B. N. (2011). Reliability,
validity, and psychometric development of the Hypersexual Behavior
Inventory in an outpatient sample of men. Sexual Addiction and
Compulsivity, 18, 30-51.
Rosenbaum, J. E. (2009). Patient teenagers? A comparison of sexual
behavior of virginity pledgers and matched nonpledgers. Pediatrics, 123,
e110-e120.
Wohl, M. J. A., DeShea, L., & Wahkinney, R. L. (2008). Looking
within: Measuring state self-forgiveness and its relationship to
psychological well-being. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 40,
1-10.
Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2013). Moving forward: Six steps to
forgiving yourself and breaking free from the past. Colorado Springs,
CO: Waterbrook Press.
Worthington, E. L., Jr., Wade, N. G., Hight, T. L., Ripley, J. S.,
McCullough, M. E., Berry, J. W., Schmitt, M. M., Berry, J. T., Bursley,
K. H., & O'Connor, L. (2003). The Religious Commitment
Inventory--10: Development, refinement, and validation of a brief scale
for research and counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50,
84-96.
Authors
Joshua N. Hook (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology, Virginia
Commonwealth University) is an Assistant Professor of Counseling
Psychology at the University of North Texas. His research interests
include humility, religion/spirituality, forgiveness, and multicultural
counseling.
Jennifer E. Farrell is a student in the Counseling Psychology Ph.D.
program at the University of North Texas. Her research interests
include: positive psychology, religion and spirituality, adult
attachment style, and romantic relationships.
Marciana J. Ramos (doctoral candidate in Counseling Psychology,
University of North Texas) is completing her Doctoral Internship in
Professional Psychology at the Vanderbilt University-Department of
Veteran's Affairs Internship Consortium. Her research interests
include romantic relationship satisfaction, sexuality, couples therapy,
and perception of romantic partners.
Don E. Davis (Ph.D. in Counseling Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth
University) is an Assistant Professor in Counseling Psychology at
Georgia State University. His interests include positive psychology,
especially virtues related to the strengthening and repair of
relationships such as humility, forgiveness, and gratitude. He is also
interested in spirituality and its intersection with other identities in
counseling.
Sara Karaga is a doctoral student in Counseling Psychology at
Georgia State University. Her research interests are in positive
psychology, especially resilience and post-traumatic growth.
Daryl R. Van Tongeren (Ph.D. in Social Psychology, Virginia
Commonwealth University) is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at Hope
College. His research interests include meaning in life, religion, and
virtues, such as humility and forgiveness, as well as positive
psychology.
Joshua B. Grubbs (M.A. in Clinical Psychology, Case Western Reserve
University) is a doctoral candidate at Case Western Reserve University
and intern at the Louis Stokes Department of Veteran Affairs Medical
Hospital in Cleveland, Ohio. His interests include human sexuality,
self-regulation, entitlement and narcissism, and the psychology of
religion and spirituality.
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Factor Loadings
for all Items of the SVS (Study 1)
Item M (SD) Factor Loading
1. Intrapersonal sexual behavior 3.34 (1.33) .60
(i.e., sexual activities without a
partner)
2. Interpersonal sexual behavior 3.94 (1.15) .81
(i.e., sexual activities with a
partner)
3. Sexual thoughts 3.77 (1.17) .94
4. Sexual feelings 3.90 (1.14) .93
5. Sexual fantasies 3.67 (1.20) .88
6. Sexual attractions 3.95 (1.10) .89
7. Sexual orientation 4.17 (1.09) .76
Table 2
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations
for study variables (Study 1).
Variable M (SD) 1 2
1. SVS 3.82 (.96) - -
2. RCI-10 2.39 (1.16) -.19 ** -
3. Total Sex 2.34 (.88) .27 ** -.28 **
4. Sex w/o partner 2.24 (1.35) .13 ** -.18 **
5. Sex w/ partner 2.44 (.94) .32 ** -.27 **
Variable 3 4 5
1. SVS - - -
2. RCI-10 - - -
3. Total Sex - - -
4. Sex w/o partner .85 ** - -
5. Sex w/ partner .65 ** .16 ** -
Note. * p < .05 **p < .01. SVS = Sexual Values Scale;
RCI-10 = Religious Commitment Inventory-10.
Table 3
Intercorrelations for study variables separated
by gender (Study 1).
Variable M (SD) 1 2
1. SVS 3.82 (.96) - -.22 *
2. RCI-10 2.39 (1.16) -.10 -
3. Total Sex 2.34 (.88) .14 -.17
4. Sex w/o partner 2.24 (1.35) .03 -.14
5. Sex w/ partner 2.44 (.94) .23 * -.11
Variable 3 4 5
1. SVS .34 ** .20 ** .34 **
2. RCI-10 -.32 ** -.20 ** -.32 **
3. Total Sex - .83 ** .72 **
4. Sex w/o partner .87 ** - .21 **
5. Sex w/ partner .63 ** .16 -
Note. * p < .05 ** p < .01. Correlations for men
are below the diagonal; correlations for women are
above the diagonal. SVS = Sexual Values Scale;
RCI-10 = Religious Commitment Inventory-10.
Table 4
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations
for study variables (Study 2).
Variable M (SD) 1 2 3
1. SVS 3.66 (.94) - - -
2. SFS 4.43 (1.28) .20 ** - -
.5. SSS 2.93 (1.11) -.32 ** -.19 * -
4. HBI-19 2.08 (.70) -.16 * -.25 ** .37 **
5. Total Sex 3.45 (1.07) .13 .10 -.07
6. Sex w/o partner 3.24 (1.49) .14 -.02 .11
7. Sex w/ partner 3.41 (1.40) .08 15 -.11
Variable 4 5 6 7
1. SVS - - - -
2. SFS - - - -
.5. SSS - - - -
4. HBI-19 - - - -
5. Total Sex .19 * - -
6. Sex w/o partner .26 ** .77 ** - -
7. Sex w/ partner .07 .78 ** .10 -
Note. * p< .05 ** p < .01. SVS = Sexual Values Scale;
SFS = Self-Forgiveness Scale; SSS = Spiritual Struggle
Scale; HBI-19 = Flypersexual Behavior Inventory-19.
Table 5
Intercorrelations for study variables separated by gender (Study 1).
Variable M (SD) 1 2 4
1. SVS 3.66 (.94) - .16 -.33 **
2. SFS 4.43 (1.28) .28 * - -.18
4. SSS 2.93 (111) -.30 * -.20 -
4. HBI-19 2.08 (.70) -15 -.31 * .46 **
5. Total Sex 3.35 (1.07) 19 .27* .06
6. Sex w/o partner 3.24 (1.49) .12 .15 .25
7. Sex w/ partner 3.41 (1.40) .14 .19 -.10
Variable 4 5 6 7
1. SVS -.16 .11 .18 .04
2. SFS -.21 * .02 -.14 .14
4. SSS .31 ** -.16 -.04 -.14
4. HBI-19 - .28 ** .29 ** .19 *
5. Total Sex -.06 - .79 ** .80 *
6. Sex w/o partner .16 .71 ** - .12
7. Sex w/ partner -.18 .74 ** .02 -
Note. * p < .05 ** p < .01. Conelations for men are below
the diagonal; conelations for women are above the diagonal.
SVS = Sexual Values Scale; SFS = Self-Forgiveness Scale;
SSS = Spiritual Struggle Scale; F1BI-19 = Flypersexual
Behavior Inventory-19.