Religious openness hypothesis: II. religious reflection and orientations, mystical experience, and psychological openness of Christians in Iran.
Watson, P.J. ; Ghorbani, Nima ; Vartanian, Meghedi 等
Whether Christianity and other traditional religions promote or
interfere with psychological openness is a contentious question within
the psychology of religion (e.g., Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis,
1993; Hood, Hill, & Williamson, 2005). The Ideological Surround
Model (ISM) of the relationship between religion and the social sciences
(Watson, 2011) essentially argues that it is not whether religions
promote openness, but rather how they do so (Kamble, Watson, Marigoudar,
& Chen, 2014b). Central to this claim is the assumption that
openness for sincerely religious individuals necessarily "operates
within a faith tradition, and for the purpose of finding religious
truth" (Dover, Miner, & Dowson, 2007, p. 204). Research
instruments like the Quest Scale valorize doubt and a willingness to
move away from foundational religious commitments as a sign of openness
(Batson & Schoenrade, 1991a, b). The original Quest Scale,
therefore, appears to operate outside a faith tradition and for the
purpose of questioning the truth about religion based upon some
unspecified standard of evaluation. Hence, the Quest Scale may reflect
(a potentially agnostic) openness about religion rather than (a
potentially faithful) religious openness. Undoubtedly, openness about
religion is an important psychological process, and the Quest Scale is
an invaluable tool for evaluating it. However, according to the
Religious Openness Hypothesis associated with the ISM, a truly
comprehensive psychology of religion should supplement measures of
openness about religion with measures of religious openness (Beck &
Jessup, 2004; Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014).
Critical evidence supporting the Religious Openness Hypothesis
rests upon use of the Religious Reflection Scale (Dover et al., 2007).
This instrument includes Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection factors
that assess efforts of the individual to pursue truth within the
framework of a specific religious tradition (Watson, Chen, & Hood,
2011). Positive correlations between these two factors in Iranian
Muslims (Ghorbani, Watson, Chen, & Dover, 2013; Ghorbani, Watson,
Geranmayepour, & Chen, 2014) and Indian Hindus (Kambie et al.,
2014b) confirm the ability of these religious traditions to wed the
intellect of believers with their faith.
In American Christians, however, Faith and Intellect Oriented
Reflection display a sometimes significant tendency to correlate
negatively (Watson et al. 2011; Watson, Chen, Ghorbani, & Vartanian,
2015; Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014). The Religious Openness
Hypothesis argues that this negative relationship reflects a
fundamentalist ghettoization of faith that walls out the intellect in
response to what some Christians perceive to be the inhospitality of an
increasingly influential Western secularism (Kamble et al., 2014b). Seen
in defensive fundamentalist terms, Western secularism divorces intellect
from faith and defines the former as "rational" and the latter
as "irrational." Western fundamentalism accepts that divorce,
but reverses the evaluation with faith as "rational" and
intellect as "irrational." Support for this interpretation
comes from American studies in which partial correlations controlling
for the Altemeyer and Hunsberger (2004) Religious Fundamentalism Scale
uncover a positive rather than a negative linkage between Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection. Partial correlations also uncover an
apparently non-defensive Biblical Foundationalism (Watson et al., 2003)
that correlates positively with both forms of religious reflection and
with religious and psychological openness more generally (Watson, Chen,
Ghorbani, & Vartanian, 2015; Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014).
Biblical Foundationalism, in other words, rejects the divorce and
maintains a Christian marriage between faith and intellect.
Present Project
A defensive ghettoization of faith should be less obvious in
societies where secularization is less influential. Iran is a theocratic
society that formally rejects secularism and operates within Muslim
cultural assumptions that faith and intellect can be married (Ghorbani,
Watson, Saeedi, Chen, & Silver, 2012). In addition to positive
linkages between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection (Ghorbani et
al., 2013), the observation that empirical markers of fundamentalism
predict greater rather than lower psychological openness in Iran
supports the notion that this Islamic society avoids a defensive
ghettoization of faith (Ghorbani, Watson, Shamohammadi, &
Cunningham, 2009). Christians living in such a context should experience
no cultural pressure to divorce faith from intellect. Hence, the
hypothesis of the present project was that Faith and Intellect Oriented
Reflection would correlate positively in Iranian Christians and that
both would predict greater religious and psychological openness.
As in a previous Iranian investigation (Ghorbani et al., 2013),
procedures included multiple measures of psychological openness.
Openness to Experience (Goldberg, 1999) and Need for Cognition
(Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, 1996) assessed cognitive
openness. The Integrative Self-Knowledge Scale (Ghorbani, Watson, &
Hargis, 2008) evaluated openness to self-experience.
Hood's (1975) Mysticism Scale served as an index of religious
openness. This instrument uses the phenomenological analysis of Stace
(1960) to operationalize three elements that define the transcendent
unity of mystical experience (Hood, Morris, & Watson, 1993).
Introvertive Mysticism involves consciousness of a timeless and
spaceless void. Extrovertive Mysticism reflects an experienced union
with all things. The Interpretation of Mysticism factor measures
tendencies to find religious meaning in mystical experience. This
three-factor structure has been confirmed in Iran (Hood et al., 2001),
and the Extrovertive and Interpretation, but not necessarily the
Introvertive, factors tend to predict Iranian Muslim religious
commitments and psychological adjustment (e.g., Hood et al., 2001;
Ghorbani, Watson, Shamohammadi, & Cunningham, 2009; Ghorbani,
Watson, Rezazadeh, & Cunningham, 2011; Ghorbani, Watson, Aghababaei,
& Chen, 2014).
Finally, Religious Orientation Scales (Gorsuch & McPherson,
1989) made it possible to evaluate the religious implications of other
variables. The Intrinsic Religious Orientation Scale records a sincere
form of commitment in which believers try to live their faith. Within
the Extrinsic Religious Orientation Scale, an Extrinsic Personal factor
records the use of religion to achieve personal well-being, whereas an
Extrinsic Social factor assesses the use of religion to obtain social
advantages. In previous studies conducted in the United States, the
Intrinsic Scale correlated positively with Faith and negatively with
Intellect Oriented Reflection, with this negative linkage theoretically
revealing an American Christian defensiveness toward the intellect
(Watson et al., 2011; Watson et al., 2015; Watson, Chen, & Morris,
2014). The Religious Openness Hypothesis suggests that the Intrinsic
Orientation should correlate positively with both forms of Christian
religious reflection in theocratic Iran where defensiveness toward the
intellect should be unnecessary.
Studies in Iranian and Pakistani Muslims, Indian Hindus, and
American Christians all suggest that Intrinsic and Extrinsic Personal
Orientations reflect more adaptive functioning, but the Extrinsic Social
Orientation often displays weak and ambiguous patterns of relationship
(Ghorbani, Watson, & Khan, 2007; Kambie, Watson, Marigoudar, &
Chen, 2014a; Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014). In these studies,
average responding on Extrinsic Social items has also been significantly
lower than on the other two religious motivation measures. The Extrinsic
Social Orientation may, therefore, reflect a generally less prominent
and ambiguous reason for being religious. Procedures made it possible to
examine whether Extrinsic Social scores would be significantly lower in
Iranian Christians as well.
Hypotheses
In summary, the Openness Hypothesis argues that Christian Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection Scales should not correlate negatively in
societies where secularization is largely nonexistent. Theocratic Iran
is just such a society. The present project evaluated this suggestion by
examining four sets of measures in Iran:
First, and most importantly, Christian Faith and Intellect Oriented
Reflection Scales supplied the centrally important data. Especially
relevant to their use in this study was a previous finding that Muslim
Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated positively in Iran
(Ghorbani et al., 2013).
Second, the Mysticism Scale served as an index of religious
openness. Extrovertive and Interpretation, but not the Introvertive
factors tend to predict religious and psychological adjustment in
Iranian Muslims (e.g., Hood et al., 2001).
Third, Need for Cognition, Openness to Experience, and Integrative
Self-Knowledge Scales assessed psychological openness. These measures
usefully clarified the religious openness of Iranian Muslims in a
previous investigation (Ghorbani et al., 2013).
Fourth and finally, Religious Orientation scales made it possible
to evaluate the religious motivational implications of all other
constructs. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Personal Orientations have had
largely adaptive implications in previous examinations of Iranian
Muslims, but the Extrinsic Social Orientation appears to be weak and
ambiguous in Muslim samples (Ghorbani et al., 2007).
Assessment of these variables made it possible to test three most
important hypotheses:
First, Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection should correlate
positively in Iranian Christians, rather than negatively as they have
done in American Christians.
Second, both religious reflection measures should predict higher
levels of psychological and religious openness as made evident in
positive correlations with Openness to Experience, Need for Cognition,
and Integrative Self-Knowledge and with the Extrovertive and
Interpretation, but not necessarily with the Introvertive mysticism
factors.
Third, the Intrinsic Religious Orientation should correlate
positively with both forms of religious reflection, in contrast to
previously observed relationships with Intellect and Faith Oriented
Reflection that were negative and positive, respectively, in American
Christians (Watson et al., 2015).
Administration of Religious Orientation Scales also made it
possible to determine if Extrinsic Social scores were lowest among these
three religious motivation measures, just as they have been in Iranian
and Pakistani Muslims, Indian Hindus, and American Christians.
Method
Participants
Research participants included 250 individuals from the Armenian
Apostolic Church in Tehran, Iran. This church operates within the
Oriental Orthodox tradition. Average age of these 82 men and 168 women
was 33.4 (SD = 11.1). One of the researchers used personal contacts to
recruit a diverse cross-section of Christians for the project.
Approximately 100 were athletes and other members of the Ararat gym and
cultural community. Another 30 individuals were students and graduates
in the sciences who belonged to the Armenian Association of University
Graduates. Musicians belonging to an Armenian chorus constituted another
approximately 30 members of the sample. Students from an Armenian high
school recruited all remaining participants from among family members
and friends.
Materials
All instruments appeared in a single questionnaire booklet. Initial
scale development procedures created a Persian version of the
Integrative Self-Knowledge measure (Ghorbani et al., 2008). Translation
of all other measures occurred in preparation for previous projects. In
these procedures, one individual translated an instrument from English
into Persian, and then another translated the Persian back into English.
Differences between original and back-translated statements were minor
and easily resolved through revisions in the Persian translation.
Reactions to all questionnaire items occurred along a 1 to 5 Likert
scale. As in previous projects in Iran, preliminary analyses eliminated
any items displaying negative item-total-correlations in order to
maximize the internal reliability of a translated scale. Scoring of all
measures involved computation of the average response per item.
Instruments appeared in the questionnaire booklet in the order in which
they are described below.
Integrative Self-Knowledge. Twelve items defined the Integrative
Self-Knowledge Scale (a = 0.76, M = 3.51, SD = 0.76), which records
openness to past, present, and desired future self-experience (Ghorbani
et al., 2008). One item said, for example, "If I need to, I can
reflect about myself and clearly understand the feelings and attitudes
behind my past behaviors."
Need for Cognition. Removal of one statement improved the internal
reliability of the Need for Cognition Scale ([alpha] = .69, M = 3.34, SD
= 0.55). Illustrative of the remaining 17 items was the self-report,
"I find satisfaction in deliberating hard and for long hours."
Openness to Experience. The Openness to Experience scale ([alpha] =
.69, M = 31.85, SD = 4.71) was the 10-item version of this instrument
associated with the International Personality Item Pool (Goldberg,
1999). A representative expression of openness said, "I have a rich
vocabulary."
Mysticism. The Mysticism Scale included 32 items (Hood, 1975)
describing three dimensions of mystical experience (Hood et al., 1993).
Removal of two items from the Introvertive and four items from the
Extrovertive factors maximized their internal reliabilities.
Introvertive Mysticism appeared in such statements as, "I have had
an experience which was both timeless and spaceless," with the
final instrument containing six items ([alpha] = .52, M = 2.85, SD =
0.89). Indicative of Extrovertive Mysticism was the self-report, "I
have had an experience in which all things seemed to be conscious."
Eight statements defined this factor ([alpha] = .63, M = 3.28, SD =
0.42). Representative of the 12-item Interpretation factor ([alpha] =
.73, M = 3.58, SD = 0.60) was the claim, "I have had an experience
which I knew to be sacred."
Christian Religious Reflection. Faith Oriented Reflection ([alpha]
= .87, M = 4.12, SD = 0.87) included seven statements that said, for
example, "Faith in Christ is what nourishes the intellect and makes
the intellectual life prosperous and productive." Removal of one
item increased the internal reliability of Intellect Oriented Reflection
([alpha] = .60, M = 4.00, SD = 0.73). Exemplifying this 4-item measure
was the assertion, "I believe as humans we should use our minds to
explore all fields of thought from science to metaphysics."
Religious Orientation. Gorsuch and McPherson (1989) Religious
Orientations Scales assessed Intrinsic (8 items, [alpha] = .68, M =
3.40, SD = 0.69), Extrinsic Personal (3 items, [alpha] = .79, M = 3.79,
SD = 0.99), and Extrinsic Social (3 items, [alpha] = .72, M = 2.34, SD =
0.97) reasons for being religious. A representative Intrinsic Scale item
said, "My whole approach to life is based on my religion."
Reflective of the Extrinsic-Personal motivation was the claim,
"What religion offers me most is comfort in times of trouble and
sorrow." The Extrinsic Social Orientation appeared in such
statements as, "I go to activities associated with my religion
because I enjoy seeing people I know there."
Procedure
Conduct of this project occurred in conformity with institutional
guidelines for ethical research. All participants were volunteers, and
their responding remained completely anonymous. Administration of
questionnaires occurred in both individual and group settings. Data
analyses focused primarily on correlations among measures, although
procedures also examined the possibility of within-subject differences
in self-reported religious orientations.
Results
Among the correlations presented in Table 1, most important was the
direct relationship between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection.
This result documented the ability of Iranian Christians to unite their
intellect with their faith. Other correlations further documented the
openness of these Iranian Christians. Faith Oriented Reflection
correlated positively with Interpretation of Mysticism and with Openness
to Experience. Intellect Oriented Reflection exhibited direct linkages
with Extrovertive Mysticism, Interpretation of Mysticism, Need for
Cognition, Openness to Experience, and Integrative Self-Knowledge. With
regard to their religious implications, both forms of religious
reflection predicted a higher Extrinsic Personal Religious Orientation,
and Faith Oriented Reflection also correlated positively with the
Intrinsic Orientation.
Other relationships generally conformed to results previously
observed with Iranian Muslims. Extrinsic Personal scores correlated
positively with the two other religious orientations and also predicted
higher levels of the Interpretation of Mysticism and Openness to
Experience measures. The Intrinsic Scale also correlated positively with
the Interpretation of Mysticism factor. Negative linkages with
Introvertive Mysticism, Interpretation of Mysticism, and Integrative
Self-Knowledge pointed toward an Extrinsic Social incompatibility with
both religious and psychological openness. All three mysticism measures
correlated positively with each other, and the Extrovertive and
Interpretation of Mysticism factors also predicted greater Need for
Cognition, Openness to Experience, and Integrative Self-Knowledge, with
these results once again suggesting their more adaptive psychological
implications in Iran. As would be expected of presumed indices of
psychological openness, Need for Cognition, Openness to Experience, and
Integrative Self-Knowledge all correlated positively with each other.
Significant differences did appear among the three religious
orientations, Greenhouse-Geisser F (1.75, 436.44) = 245.58, p < .001.
Post hoc analyses revealed that the average response per item for each
motivation was significantly different from the other two. The Extrinsic
Personal Orientation (M = 3.79, SD = 0.99) was highest, follow by the
Intrinsic (M = 3.40, SD = 0.69) and then by the Extrinsic Social (M =
2.34, SD = 0.97) measures.
Comparisons With American Data
Of interest were relationships for Iranian Christians in comparison
to those observed for American Christians in a companion project to this
study (Watson et al., 2015). Table 2 presents the six common measures
examined in both investigations. Given the large combined sample size (N
= 600), use of a p < .01 level of significance and an examination of
only those comparisons in which a relationship proved to be significant
in at least one society made it possible to ignore weak and chance
contrasts. Centrally important was the demonstration that the positive
correlation between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection in Iran was
significantly different from the negative relationship in the United
States, z = 6.04, p < .001. The positive Intrinsic Orientation
linkage with Faith Oriented Reflection was more robust in the United
Stated as was its negative association with Intellect Oriented
Reflection. The direct connection between Extrinsic Personal and
Intrinsic scores was stronger in Iran. Negative Need for Cognition
relationships with Faith Oriented Reflection and with the Intrinsic
Orientation in the United States differed significantly from the
nonsignificant outcomes in Iran.
Discussion
Results from this investigation supported the Religious Openness
Hypothesis. Confirmation of the first, critical prediction occurred when
Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection correlated positively in Iranian
Christians with this result being significantly different from the
negative relationship observed previously in American Christians (Watson
et al., 2015). In line with the second hypothesis, Faith and Intellect
Oriented Religious Reflection also displayed at least some linkages with
religious and psychological openness. The third hypothesis argued that
the Intrinsic Orientation would correlate positively with both forms of
religious reflection in Iran, in contrast to previous American data in
which Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection correlated negatively and
positively, respectively. In Iran, the Intrinsic Orientation did exhibit
a positive tie with Faith Oriented Reflection, but the hypothesized
direct linkage with Intellect Oriented Reflection failed to materialize.
However, the negative Intrinsic Orientation relationship with Intellect
Oriented Reflection in Iran was small, statistically nonsignificant, and
weaker than the negative correlation observed in American Christians.
Intellect Oriented Reflection, therefore, was relatively less
incompatible with sincere religious commitments in Iranian Christians,
and this result was consistent with the broader implications of the
Religious Openness Hypothesis.
In American Christians, Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection
tend to correlate negatively (Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014), and
sometimes significantly (Watson et al., 2011, 2015), in contrast to
positive linkages observed in Iranian Muslims (Ghobani et al., 2013) and
Indian Hindus (Kamble et al., 2014b). The Religious Openness Hypothesis
argues that this negative relationship reflects a defensive
ghettoization of thought in response to what some American Christians
perceive to be an inhospitality toward faith within the secularization
that increasingly dominates the West. Someone skeptical about this
explanation might counterclaim that this negative relationship merely
demonstrates the closed-mindedness of Christians in contrast to the
open-mindedness of Muslims and Hindus. In conformity with the Religious
Openness Hypothesis, Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection correlated
positively in Iranian Christians. Hence, this outcome documented an
open-minded Christian ability to wed intellect with faith when a
cultural context seemed to preclude any need for a defensive reaction to
secularism.
Additional support for the Religious Openness Hypothesis came in
relationships of Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection with indices of
religious and psychological openness. Openness to religious experience
appeared in positive correlations of both Christian Religious Reflection
measures with the Interpretation of Mysticism factor and in a direct
association of Intellect Oriented Reflection with Extrovertive
Mysticism. Connections with psychological openness were obvious as well
in positive ties of Faith Oriented Reflection with Openness to
Experience and of Intellect Oriented Reflection with Openness to
Experience, Need for Cognition, and Integrative Self-Knowledge. A
nonsignificant Faith Oriented Reflection relationship with Need for
Cognition differed significantly from the negative correlation observed
previously in the United States, and this result also suggested a
relatively greater cognitive openness in Iranian Christians.
Religious Orientation and Openness Measures
Data failed to confirm the Religious Openness Hypothesis in one
noteworthy instance. Intellect Oriented Reflection did not display the
predicted positive correlation with the Intrinsic Orientation. The
negative relationship between these two variables was not statistically
significant in Iran, as it has been in American Christians (e.g.,
Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014); yet, this linkage also was not
significantly positive as it has been in Iranian Muslims (Ghorbani et
al., 2013, Ghorbani, Watson, Aghababaei, & Chen, 2014) and Indian
Hindus (Kamble et al., 2014b). Findings in Iran and America, therefore,
suggest that intrinsic religious commitments may not have a potential to
facilitate the Intellect Oriented Reflection of Christians as they
appear to have in Iranian Muslims and Indian Hindus. At the same time,
however, the positive Intrinsic Orientation relationship with Faith
Oriented Reflection was more robust in the United States, as was its
negative association with Intellect Oriented Reflection. This pattern
further suggested that Christian religious reflection was more polarized
in the United States.
The Extrinsic Personal Orientation correlated positively with both
Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection. Iranian Christians, therefore,
did appear to have the religious motivational foundations for uniting
their intellect with their faith. Iranian Muslims (Ghorbani et al.,
2013) and Indian Hindus (Kamble et al, 2014b) display similar
relationships, but in American Christians, Extrinsic Personal scores
correlate negatively (Watson, Chen, & Morris. 2014) or
nonsignificantly (Watson et al., 2011, 2015) with Intellect Oriented
Reflection. The implications of the Extrinsic Personal Orientation for
Christians may, therefore, vary with cultural context.
Significant linkages with mystical experience and psychological
openness conformed to previous demonstrations that the Extrinsic
Personal and Social Orientations tend to be more adaptive and
maladaptive, respectively. The only Intrinsic Orientation relationship
with these variables was a positive association with Interpretation of
Mysticism. The Intrinsic Orientation did, nevertheless, appear to be
relatively more open in Iran because a nonsignificant Intrinsic
Orientation correlation with Need for Cognition differed from the
negative association observed in the United States.
Studies of Muslims in Iran and Pakistan reveal that the Extrinsic
Personal Orientation is strongest, the Extrinsic Social Orientation is
weakest, and the Intrinsic Orientation falls in between (Ghorbani et
al., 2007). In the United States, the Extrinsic Social Orientation is
weakest as well, but the Intrinsic and Extrinsic Personal motivations
either do not differ (Watson, Chen, & Ghorbani, 2014) or the
Intrinsic Orientation is strongest (Watson et al., 2015). Such data
might mean that the Extrinsic Personal Orientation is more important
only within the motivational dynamics of Muslims, but Indian Hindus
display the same pattern (Kamble et al., 2014a). The present data now
make it clear the Christians living in Iran are like both Muslims and
Hindus in their relative embrace of these three religious motivations.
Similarities across traditions and societies suggest that the Extrinsic
Personal Orientation may assume a special significance outside the West.
The stronger relationship between the Extrinsic Personal and Intrinsic
Orientations in Iran than in the United States may also suggest the same
thing. In contrast, the Extrinsic Social Orientation appears be less
prominent, regardless of cultural context.
Correlations among openness measures were in line with
expectations. Extrovertive Mysticism and Interpretation of Mysticism
factors displayed direct relationships with all three indices of
psychological openness, which in turn correlated positively with each
other. As in previous Iranian projects (e.g., Hood et al., 2001;
Ghorbani, Watson, Shamohammadi, & Cunningham, 2009), Introvertive
Mysticism seemed relatively less important in defining religious and
psychological functioning.
Limitations
Limitations associated with this exploration of the Religious
Openness Hypothesis in American (Watson et al., 2015) and then in
Iranian Christians necessitate interpretative caution. Five issues may
be especially noteworthy.
First, internal reliabilities for a number of measures in both
projects were below the .70 value recommended by Kaplan and Saccuzzo
(2012). More robust relationship might appear with the administration of
more internally reliable instruments.
Second, direct comparisons between Iranian and American
correlations require careful interpretation. In cross-cultural research,
the typical assumption is that definitive comparison of psychological
constructs requires measurement invariance statistical procedures which
confirm that instruments are operating with some degree of psychometric
similarity across societies (e.g., van de Vijer & Leung, 1997). The
need to achieve this criterion can result in the removal of items from
scales in order to eliminate cross-cultural dissimilarities (e.g.,
Ghorbani, Watson, & Weathington, 2009). Such procedures are clearly
useful in calibrating constructs to a nomothetic perspective that seeks
to understand populations abstracted from culture-specific ambiguities.
On the other hand, the ISM assumes that the maintenance of
dissimilarities in "ideolographic" as opposed to nomothetic
comparisons may have their place as well (Watson et al., 2015). Such
comparisons may usefully clarify the nuances and particularities that
help describe the psychology of Christian and other communities (e.g.,
Ghorbani, Watson, Krauss, Bing, & Davison, 2004).
Third, this Iranian and the companion American project did not
examine the exact same set of measures. Important insights might have
been available if they had done so, and this possibility may deserve
future research attention. At the same time, however, the ISM
exploration of religious openness has intentionally adopted the strategy
of analyzing a broad range of relevant constructs across diverse
religious and cultural contexts. Such an approach helps avoid the
development of conclusions that could be construct, culture, or religion
dependent. The most important insights, therefore, may follow from
research programs that are even more ambitious in their empirical
expansiveness.
Fourth, any attempt to explain cross-cultural differences in terms
of the influences of secularization in the West but not in Iran is
admittedly speculative. The American sample included only university
undergraduates who were primarily Protestants living in religiously
conservative area of the United States (Watson et al., 2015). The
Iranian sample was older and included individuals from the wider
Armenian Orthodox Christian community in Tehran. Age, denominational,
and numerous other differences could have produced the contrasts that
appeared between these two samples. Additional research seems necessary
to clarify these possibilities.
Fifth and finally, the two present studies operated within a
Christian Ideological Surround. The ISM, nevertheless, rests upon
Christian beliefs in the importance of dialog and peace across
traditions (Watson, 2006; Ghorbani et al., 2013). Hence, ISM assumptions
and methods will presumably have their place within other non-Christian
religious and even non-religious ideological surrounds (cf., Ghorbani et
al., 2012). Interpretations of the findings of the present project will
of course vary across ideological surrounds.
Future Studies
Given such limitations, ultimate confirmation of the Religious
Openness Hypothesis will obviously require considerable additional
research. Of interest, for example, will be efforts to discover whether
Intellect and Faith Oriented Reflection correlate positively in
Christians living in other non-Western societies or whether a negative
correlation always appears in different Christian denominations in the
West.
In addition, a central assumption of the hypothesis is that
defensive attitudes toward secularism explain the negative correlation
between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection in the West. This
suggestion should be tested more directly by operationalizing such
attitudes. Among other things, the prediction would be that these
attitudes should mediate the negative linkage of Faith Oriented
Reflection and also of Religious Fundamentalism with Intellect Oriented
Reflection in American samples (Watson, Chen, Morris, & Stephenson,
2015).
With a measure of defensive attitudes toward secularism available,
additional issues could be explored as well. An interesting question
might be whether Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection would correlate
negatively among Muslims, Hindus, and Jews living in the West. The
argument of the Religious Openness Hypothesis is that a negative
relationship between Faith and Intellect Oriented Reflection should be
obvious only in those religious communities that display defensiveness
toward secularism. In the United States, minority religious communities
might express more favorable attitudes toward a secularization that
creates a greater public space not dominated by the majority Christian
perspective. In France, however, government efforts to limit public
expressions of Islamic faith, as, for example, in the wearing of the
hijab or headscarf by Muslim women, might promote defensiveness and
encourage a divorce between faith and intellect. Such predictions may
not hold true, but evident in these possibilities is the ISM assumption
that context effects are potentially important in the psychology of
religion and deserve careful empirical consideration (e.g., Andrews,
Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014).
Conclusion
Empirical efforts to differentiate between Religious Fundamentalist
and Biblical Foundationalist Ideological Surrounds in American
Christians (Watson et al., 2015) and the examination of Christians
living in Iran both supported the Religious Openness Hypothesis.
Additional support has come from investigations analyzing the religious
reflection of Muslims in Iran (Ghorbani et al., 2013, Ghorbani, Watson,
Aghababaei, & Chen, 2014) and of Hindus in India (Kamble et al.,
2014b). At the broadest level, the Religious Openness Hypothesis argues
that a truly comprehensive understanding of openness as it relates
religion must remain sensitive to two very different perspectives. One
perspective exists outside religious traditions and expresses a
potentially agnostic skepticism that is recorded by, for example, the
original Quest Scale (Watson, Chen, & Morris, 2014). The other
operates within religion and potentially reflects a faith that
thoughtfully attempts to answer questions about the present and the
future using foundations from the past. Research examining Faith and
Intellect Oriented Reflection may be critical in clarifying this second
form of openness. In short, a truly comprehensive psychology of religion
should explore intra-traditional as well extra-traditional forms of
openness.
P. J. Watson
University of Tennessee at Chattanooga
Nima Ghorbani Meghedi Vartanian
University of Tehran
Zhuo Chen
University of Oregon
Address all correspondence to P. J. Watson, Psychology/Department
#2803, 350 Holt Hall--615 McCallie Avenue, University of Tennessee at
Chattanooga 37403, U.S.A. E-mail address is
[email protected].
References
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (2004). A revised Religious
Fundamentalism Scale: The short and sweet of it. The International
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14, 47-54.
Andrews, B., Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Morris, R. J. (2014).
Postmodernism, positive psychology, and posttraumatic growth within a
Christian ideological surround. Manuscript under review.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion
and the individual. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Batson, C. D., & Schoenrade, P. (1991a). Measuring religion as
quest: 1) Validity concerns. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 30, 416-429.
Batson, C. D., & Schoenrade, P. (1991b). Measuring religion as
quest: 2) Reliability concerns. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 30, 430-447.
Beck, R., & Jessup, R. K. (2004). The multidimensional nature
of quest motivation. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 32, 283-294.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W.
B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The
life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition.
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197-253.
Dover, H., Miner, M., & Dowson, M. (2007). The nature and
structure of Muslim religious reflection. Journal of Muslim Mental
Health, 2, 189-210.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Aghababaei, N., & Chen, Z. (2014).
Transliminality and mystical experience: Common thread hypothesis,
religious commitment, and psychological adjustment in Iran. Psychology
of Religion and Spirituality, 6, 268-275.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Dover, H. (2013).
Varieties of openness in Tehran and Qom: Psychological and religious
parallels of faith and intellect oriented Islamic religious reflection.
Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 16, 123-137.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J, Geranmayepour, S., & Chen, Z.
(2014). Muslim experiential religiousness: Relationships with attitude
toward Islam, religious reflection, and basic needs satisfaction in
Iranians. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 25,
53-72.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., & Hargis, M. B. (2008).
Integrative self-knowledge: Correlations and incremental validity of a
cross-cultural measure developed in Iran and the United States. The
Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied. 142, 395-412.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., & Khan, Z. (2007). Theoretical,
empirical, and potential ideological dimensions of using Western
conceptualizations to measure Muslim religious commitments. Journal of
Muslim Mental Health, 2, 113-131.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Krauss, S. W., Bing, M. N., &
Davison, H. (2004). Social Science as dialogue: Narcissism,
individualist and collectivist values, and religious interest in Iran
and the United States. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse
Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 23(2), 111-123.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J, Rezazadeh, Z., & Cunningham, C. J.
L. (2011). Dialogical validity of religious measures in Iran:
Relationships with integrative self-knowledge and self-control of the
"Perfect Man" (Ensan-e Kamel). Archive for the Psychology of
Religion, 33, 93-113.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Saeedi, Z, Chen, Z., & Silver, C.
F. (2012). Religious problem-solving and the complexity of religious
rationality within an Iranian Muslim ideological surround. Journal for
the Scientific Study of Religion, 51, 656-675.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Shamohammadi, K., & Cunningham, C.
J. L. (2009). Post-critical beliefs in Iran: Predicting religious and
psychological functioning. Research in the Social Scientific Study of
Religion, 20, 217-237.
Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., & Weathington, B. L. (2009).
Mindfulness in Iran and the United States: Cross-cultural structural
complexity and parallel relationships with psychological adjustment.
Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse
Psychological Issues, 28, 211-224.
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain,
personality inventory measuring the lower-level facetsof several
five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F.
Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychologyin Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7-28).
Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
Gorsuch, R. L., & McPherson, S. E. (1989). Intrinsic/extrinsic
measurement: I/E revised and single-item scales. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 348-354.
Hood, R. W., Jr. (1975). The construction and preliminary
validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 14, 29-41.
Hood, R. W., Jr., Hill, P. C., & Williamson, W. P. (2005). The
psychology of religious fundamentalism. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hood, R. W., Jr., Morris, R. J., & Watson, P. J. (1993).
Further factor analysis of Hood's Mysticism Scale. Psychological
Reports, 73, 1176-1178.
Hood, R. W., Jr., Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Ghramaleki, A. F.,
Bing, M. N., Davison, H. K., Morris, R. J., & Williamson, W. P.
(2001). Dimensions of the Mysticism Scale: Confirming the three-factor
structure in the United States and Iran. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion, 40, 691-705.
Kamble, S. V., Watson, P. J., Marigoudar, S., & Chen, Z.
(2014a). Attitude toward Hinduism, religious orientations, and
psychological adjustment in India. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture,
17, 161-172.
Kamble, S. V., Watson, P. J., Marigoudar, S., & Chen, Z.
(2014b). Varieties of openness and religious commitment in India:
Relationships of attitudes toward Hinduism, Hindu religious reflection,
and religious schema. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 36,
172-198.
Kaplan, R., & Saccuzzo, D. (2012). Psychological testing:
Principles, applications, and issues. Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning.
Stace, W. T. (1960). Mysticism and philosophy. London, England:
MacMillan.
Streib, H., Hood, R. W, Jr., & Klein, C. (2010). The Religious
Schema Scale: Construction and initial validation of a quantitative
measure for religious styles. International Journal for the Psychology
of Religion, 20, 151-172.
van de Vijer, F., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis
for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Watson, P. J. (2006). Friends of the truth, violence, and the
ideological surround: Social science as meetings for clearness. Archive
for the Psychology of Religion, 28, 123-132.
Watson, P. J. (2011). Whose psychology? Which rationality?
Christian psychology within an ideological surround after postmodernism.
Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 30, 307-316.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Ghorbani, N. (2014). Extrinsic
cultural religious orientation: Analysis of an Iranian measure in
university students in the United States. Journal of Beliefs and Values,
35, 6178.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., Ghorbani, N., & Vartanian, M. (2015).
Religious openness hypothesis: I. Religious reflection, schemas, and
orientations within religious fundamentalist and biblical
foundationalist ideological surrounds. Journal of Psychology and
Christianity, 34, 99-113.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (2011). Biblical
foundationalism and religious reflection: Polarization of faith and
intellect oriented epistemologies within a Christian ideological
surround. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 39, 111-121.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Morris, R. J. (2014). Varieties of
quest and the religious openness hypothesis within religious
fundamentalist and biblical foundationalist ideological surrounds.
Religions, 5(1), 1-20.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., Morris, R. J. & Stephenson, E. (2015).
Religious openness hypothesis: III. Defense against Secularism within
fundamentalist and biblical foundationalist ideological surrounds.
Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 34, 125-140.
Watson, P. J., Sawyers, P., Morris, R. J., Carpenter, M., Jimenez,
R. S., Jonas, K. A., & Robinson, D. L. (2003). Reanalysis within a
Christian ideological surround: Relationships of intrinsic religious
orientation with fundamentalism and right-wing authoritarianism. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 31, 315-328.
Authors
P.J. Watson is U. C. Foundation Professor of Psychology at the
University of Tennessee at Chatttanooga. He received a Ph.D. in
Experimental Psychology from the University of Texas at Arlington. His
research focuses on the psychology of religion and on personality
functioning especially as it relates to the self.
Nima Ghorbani, PhD, is a psychologist with the University of Tehran
in Iran. He is a licensed practitioner of Intensive Short-Term Dynamic
Psychotherapy. His reserach interests include cross-cultural
perspectives on the self and emotion and also on the psychology of the
religious self and experience.
Meghedi Vartanian is a graduate student studying psychology at the
University of Tehran in Iran. Her research interests center on the self,
psychotherapy, and the psychology of religion.
Zhuo Chen is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Psychology
at the University of Oregon. His research area is in personality and
social psychology. He received a master's degree in Research
Psychology at the University of Tennessee at Chattanooga and also a
master's degree in Mathematics at the University of Oregon.
Table 1
Relationships Among Christian Religious Reflection,
Religious Orientation, and Varieties of Openness
Measures (N = 250)
Measures 2. 3. 4. 5.
1. Faith Oriented .31 *** .52 *** .60 *** .11
Reflection
2. Intellect Oriented - -.08 .24 *** -.04
Reflection
3. Intrinsic Orientation - - .53 *** .11
4. Extrinsic Personal - - - .24 ***
Orientation
5. Extrinsic Social - - - -
Orientation
6. Extrovertive Mysticism - - - -
7. Introvertive Mysticism - - - -
8. Interpretation - - - -
of Mysticism
9. Need for Cognition - - - -
10. Openness to - - - -
Experience
11. Integrative - - - -
Self-Knowledge
Measures 6. 7. 8.
1. Faith Oriented .10 .05 .24 ***
Reflection
2. Intellect Oriented .14 * -.04 .19 **
Reflection
3. Intrinsic Orientation .11 .06 .24 ***
4. Extrinsic Personal .07 -.06 .26 ***
Orientation
5. Extrinsic Social -.21 ** .00 -.17 **
Orientation
6. Extrovertive Mysticism - .10 .58 ***
7. Introvertive Mysticism - - .24 ***
8. Interpretation - - -
of Mysticism
9. Need for Cognition - - -
10. Openness to - - -
Experience
11. Integrative - - -
Self-Knowledge
Measures 9. 10. 11.
1. Faith Oriented -.01 .24 *** -.02
Reflection
2. Intellect Oriented .14 * .26 *** .13 *
Reflection
3. Intrinsic Orientation .03 .11 -.07
4. Extrinsic Personal .04 .31 *** -.08
Orientation
5. Extrinsic Social -.10 .00 -.15 *
Orientation
6. Extrovertive Mysticism .25 *** .30 *** .33 ***
7. Introvertive Mysticism .09 -.02 -.12
8. Interpretation 32 *** .43 *** .24 ***
of Mysticism
9. Need for Cognition - .42 *** .42 ***
10. Openness to - - .28 ***
Experience
11. Integrative - - -
Self-Knowledge
Table 2
Comparison of Correlations Among Measures Examined in
Both Iranian (N = 250) and American
(N = 350) Christian Samples
Measures 1. 2. 3.
1. Faith Oriented Reflection - .31# *** .52# ***
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection -.18 ** - -.08
3. Intrinsic Orientation .69 *** -.36 *** -
4. Extrinsic Personal Orientation .53 *** .06 .35 ***
5. Extrinsic Social Orientation .11 * .12 * -.09
6. Need for Cognition -.26 *** .25 *** -.20 ***
Measures 4. 5. 6.
1. Faith Oriented Reflection .60 *** .11 -.01#
2. Intellect Oriented Reflection .24 *** -.04 .14 *
3. Intrinsic Orientation .53# *** .11 .03#
4. Extrinsic Personal Orientation - .24 *** .04
5. Extrinsic Social Orientation .20 *** - -.10
6. Need for Cognition -.14 ** -.08 -
Note. Correlations from Iran are above the diagonal,
whereas American relationships appear below. Above the
significantly different diagonal, bold correlations
specify those relationships which proved to
be across the two samples (p < .01).
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
Correlations specify those relationships which proved to
be across the two samples (p < .01) is indicated with #.